Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cresp Exploring the impact of the pandemic on the relationship between individual types and the natural environment: the role of mortality concerns Andrea Marais-Potgieter ∗ , Andrew Thatcher Department of Psychology, University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa, Private Bag 3, 2050 a r t i c l e i n f o Keywords: Environmental threats Human-nature nexus Pandemic Terror management theory Worldviews Natural environment Environmental attitudes Environmental perceptions a b s t r a c t COVID-19 is a global event that has impacted every individual on earth in some way and can be viewed as a mortality salience trigger. Although there were reports of increased nature exposure across the world, research is needed to understand whether the pandemic event impacted the underlying psychology of the human-nature nexus. Given the likelihood of pandemics and environmental challenges increasing in frequency in the future, there is a need for a deeper understanding of how pandemics impact individuals’ relationship with the natural environment in South Africa. To achieve this, the study applied psychological types (grouping individuals based on homogeneity) to explore potential shifts as human nature is neither fixed, nor universal. The study asked: Given the multiple significant impacts of COVID-19 on the African continent, how have perceptions and attitudes towards the natural environment changed within and between types of individuals from 2016 (pre COVID) to 2021 (COVID) in South Africa? In a longitudinal, quantitative study, separate samples 721 in 2016 and 665 in 2021 were obtained. Participants in 2021 were grouped into the same six types using the same criteria, for comparison with the 2016 data. The results showed limited potential for pandemics to act as catalysts for long- term individual change towards increased pro-environmentalism. The study confirmed the main tenets of Terror Management Theory that individuals tend to be driven to uphold worldviews when confronted with mortality. Furthermore, there was a reduced experience of personal control over outcomes that increased reliance on sources of control outside the self as an attempt to buffer against mortality concerns. The study contributes towards Terror Management Theory’s application during pandemics, and how that relates to individual environmental attitudes and perceptions. 1 l t m M a i 2 u m t T o e t C s p s s E c v ( s h i a h R 2 ( . Introduction Individual psychological distance from the environmental crisis has ed humanity to a choice of either addressing our dysfunctional rela- ionship with the natural environment or experiencing runaway cli- ate change and ultimately societal collapse ( Spence et al., 2012 ; cDonald et al., 2015 ). Psychological research is imperative to inform nd address the relationship between individuals and environmental ssues such as climate change ( Saunders et al., 2005 ; Clayton et al., 015 ; Inauen et al., 2021 ). Environmental action is impacted by individ- al characteristics such as attitudes, perceptions ( Lorenzoni and Whit- arsh, 2014 ), worldviews ( Fisher, 2013 ), and mortality salience linked o individual trait responses ( Arndt and Solomon, 2003 ; Wolfe and ubi, 2018 ). Mortality salience refers to the increased awareness of one’s wn mortality that can be triggered by reminders of death, such as those xperienced during the COVID-19 pandemic ( Greenfield et al., 2021 ). ∗ Corresponding author. E-mail address: andrea@plunge.co.za (A. Marais-Potgieter) . ttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100096 eceived 1 November 2022; Received in revised form 27 January 2023; Accepted 9 M 666-6227/© 2023 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. This is an open access http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ ) The pandemic can be seen as a traumatic event that had the potential o rupture the existential path of individuals ( Tomaszek and Muchacka- ymerman, 2020 ) due to it functioning as a strong trigger for mortality alience ( Pyszczynski et al., 2021 ). Singla et al. (2021) found that the andemic was a transformative process for individuals due to increased elf-awareness impacting moral values, empathy, and a sense of per- onal and behavioral responsibility. According to research conducted by vers et al. (2021) values are able to change rapidly during a collective risis, and in the case of their study they observed increased collective alues, rather than individualistic ones during the COVID-19 pandemic seen by the instant collective response to the George Floyd incident, ee Cappelli, 2020 ). There is a need to understand the pandemic’s influence on the uman-nature nexus that relates to the affective relationship between ndividuals and the natural environment. This study explored whether shift in the individual-nature nexus took place between 2016 and 2021 arch 2023 article under the CC BY-NC-ND license https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100096 http://www.ScienceDirect.com http://www.elsevier.com/locate/cresp http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100096&domain=pdf mailto:andrea@plunge.co.za https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100096 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 a m d c o m p t s 1 t t c d e g R u r h t o M o v ( t t v ( fi a a D B L i i t f t b e a d T v m 1 o 1 t i fl h f t S m fl m a o t t ( i s D d i s t 1 1 a c t s g t v p r ( w o t t m f p n a w d w t t t 1 w w ( t b e w h n i t 1 n r p t mongst different individual types of people treating the pandemic as a ortality salience trigger. In this paper we build a research framework iscussing the human-nature nexus, mortality salience, and how these oncepts relate to the COVID-19 pandemic. The concept of type psychol- gy is introduced, and the six types used in this study are explained. Our ethods, procedure and materials discussion sections are followed by a resentation and discussion of significant results at an overall and per ype level. Finally, we discuss study limitations and ideas for future re- earch. .1. Relationship between types of individuals and the natural environment As a result of the complex human-nature nexus, individual charac- eristics and environmental challenges have a mutually reinforcing rela- ionship. Individual attitudes and behaviours, such as having one fewer hild, living car-free, avoiding airplane travel, and eating a plant-based iet, are highly effective to reduce an individual’s impact on the natural nvironment ( Wynes and Nicholas, 2017 ), but far too few people en- age in these behaviors ( Steg and Vlek, 2009 ; Ertz and Sarigöllü, 2019 ; ampedi and Ifegbesan, 2022 ). The development of individual types is a pragmatic approach to nderstand the key differences and similarities between individuals egarding their attitudes that drive perceptions and subsequent be- aviors, while not ignoring the complexity inherent in human na- ure ( Doty and Glick, 1994 ; Fiss, 2011 ). Types can be constructed n a micro (individual) or macro (cultural) level ( McKinney, 1969 ). ary Douglas was one of the seminal researchers in type work, based n her cultural theory, and she developed types based on how indi- iduals perceive risk based on social aspects and cultural adherence Thompson, 1980 ). The types included the Individualistic where na- ure is experienced as benevolent and full of potential, Egalitarian hat perceived nature is fragile, Hierarchical in which nature is per- erse/tolerant, and Fatalistic that experienced nature as unpredictable Douglas and Wildavsky, 1982 ; Oltedal et al., 2004 ). Since then, the eld of type psychology has expanded rapidly with more individu- lised understandings of the human-nature nexus that has included vari- bles such as beliefs, values, worldviews, attitudes, religion, etc. (e.g., efra, 2008 ; Leiserowitz et al., 2009 ; Poortinga and Darnton, 2016 ; ernstein and Szuster, 2018 ; MacDonald et al., 2019 ). The study by eiserowitz et al. (2009) explored the human-nature nexus by look- ng at beliefs about global warming, associated risk perceptions, behav- or, traits, relationship to information and policy. They identified six ypes namely the: Alarmed, Concerned, Cautious, Disengaged, Doubt- ul, and Dismissive ( Leiserowitz et al., 2009 ). Other studies have used ype psychology to explain climate change beliefs ( Hall et al., 2018 ), eliefs about biodiversity and ecosystems ( Buijs et al., 2008 ), pro- nvironmental behavior ( MacDonald et al., 2019 ), relationships with nimals ( Teel and Manfredo, 2010 ), and the relationship between in- ividuals and the natural environment ( Bernstein and Szuster, 2018 ). ype research could provide a deeper understanding of different indi- idual characteristics that influence how they perceive risk and react to ortality threats. .2. Terror management theory and the human-nature nexus Terror Management Theory (TMT) was developed by social psychol- gists Jeff Greenberg, Sheldon Solomon, and Tom Pyszczynski in the 980 ′ s ( Pyszczynski, et al., 2015 ). It draws on the work of cultural an- hropologist Ernest Becker’s ideas about the relationship between mean- ng in life, cultural beliefs, self-esteem, and how these coalesce to in- uence anxiety about one’s mortality ( Becker, 1973 ). TMT posits that umans have a unique and innate need for self-preservation and there- ore engage in behaviors to help manage existential terror related to he, generally unconscious, awareness of death ( Greenberg et al., 1986 ; olomon et al., 1991 ). This terror of death is largely repressed until 2 ade salient ( Solomon et al., 1991 ). Mortality salience is a fear that in- uences thoughts, affect, and behavior using a range of psychological echanisms ( Solomon et al., 2015 ). Therefore, TMT deals with death- nxiety that is the general fear or anxiety related to the thought of one’s wn death ( Rosenblatt et al., 1989 ) which is different to death uncer- ainty that is focused on the lack of certainty about a particular situa- ion or outcome typically observed in patients with advanced diseases Mishel, 1999 ). Death uncertainty has been found to interact with hope n advanced lung cancer patients where they hold onto hope indirectly uch as leaving a legacy or holding onto values ( Borneman et al., 2014 ). eath uncertainty evolves over time ( Wright et al., 2009 ) whereas eath-anxiety exists regardless of whether individuals are chronically ll or not ( Rosenblatt et al., 1989 ). Studies related to death-anxiety have hown that moral principles, as part of psychological structures, assist o reduce anxiety about death ( Rosenblatt et al., 1989 ; Greenberg et al., 997 ). .2.1. Self-esteem and cultural worldviews The mechanisms of self-esteem and cultural worldviews act as nxiety buffers against existential threat ( Greenberg et al., 1997 ). A ultural worldview is a symbolic understanding of reality that at- ributes order, perpetuity, and constancy to the world. A sense of per- onal value and immortality can be achieved by following cultural uidelines ( Pyszczynski et al., 1999 ). Self-esteem is the knowledge hat one is meeting the standards inherent in one’s cultural world- iew ( Pyszczynski et al., 1999 ). The anxiety-buffer hypothesis pro- oses that increased self-esteem or faith in one’s cultural worldview educes anxiety and related behavior in response to mortality threats Greenberg et al., 1997 ). The mortality salience hypothesis posits that hen psychological mechanisms provide protection against the terror f one’s mortality, then mortality reminders would increase the desire o maintain these mechanisms ( Greenberg et al., 1997 ). When people are reminded of their own mortality, their attachment o their cultural worldviews is enhanced triggering the terror manage- ent effect ( Solomon et al., 1991 ). The range of mortality salience ef- ects include in-group bias and prejudice against out-groups, aggression, rosocial behavior, social unanimity, striving for belonging and unique- ess, meeting cultural standards of value, and avoidance of self-focused ttention ( Greenberg et al., 1997 ). The buffering mechanism of cultural orldviews to protect individuals from anxiety about death was first emonstrated in studies by Rosenblatt et al. (1989) that showed that hen mortality salience is activated, people are encouraged to main- ain their cultural-anxiety buffer and therefore are likely to be punitive owards those that oppose their worldviews, and are munificent towards hose that uphold it. .2.2. Individual traits and mortality salience Individual traits moderate mortality salience effects. Individuals ho are high in authoritarianism responded to mortality salience ith increased prejudice against those who have different attitudes Greenberg et al., 1990 ; Motyl et al., 2010 ). Depressed individuals tend o defend their worldview more intensely when mortality is made salient ecause of their fragile relationship with cultural worldviews and self- steem ( Greenberg et al., 1997 ). Arndt and Solomon (2003) found that hen mortality salience is activated, individuals high in neuroticism ave a decreased need for personal control compared to those low in euroticism who report an increased need for personal control indicat- ng the complex relationship between mortality salience and individual raits. .2.3. Proximal and distal defenses The dual-process model in TMT deals with the proximal or distal ature of defenses based on the accessible conscious and unconscious eminders of mortality ( Pyszczynski et al., 1999 ). The mechanisms em- loyed aim to manage the fear of death that is overwhelming and dis- urbing, and to maintain psychological equanimity. Proximal defenses A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 a c i d i d l a d d i i t i i a b m t n e 1 s a ( t 2 2 b T s r i u F s t 2 m a 1 d a t u o ( d t H w p e e f i n i b Table 1 Summary of key differences between types. Type Summary of key differences Disconnected type Poorly connected to nature Lacked an environmental worldview Apathetic Survivalist Uncommitted type Moderate concern about the natural environment Concern about animal exploitation and extinction Reduced sense of personal accountability Alarmed type Moderate-high affective relationship with the natural environment Low-moderate apathy Perceived urgency in both environmental and animal issues Believer type Affective relationship to the natural environment contrasted with their dominant social paradigm Belief in human ingenuity through technology to solve environmental and animal issues Shift responsibility for issues to future generations Concerned type Closest relationship with the natural environment Strongest environmental worldview Pleasure-seeker type Weak affective relationship with the natural environment Low environmental worldview Not concerned about environmental issues Individuals not responsibility for environment issues 1 i s T b a r h e S p Z i t t m p t t w t a t r p t e c m t 2 a t i f re logical, deal with conscious reminders of morality, repress, create ognitive distortions, or deny thoughts, tend to occur directly after the nduction of mortality salience, and do not get activated by subliminal eath stimuli ( Pyszczynski et al., 1999 ). Distal defenses are experiential n nature, activated by the implicit knowledge of death, occur after a istraction from mortality salience, but immediately in response to sub- iminal death stimuli, and embed the person as a valuable member of n eternal immortal reality ( Pyszczynski et al., 1999 ). Global crises such as natural disasters, wars, and pandemics are eath cues that emphasize individual mortality. The COVID-19 pan- emic made mortality highly salient with an onslaught of fear-inducing nformation, combined with potential personal threats such as loss of ncome, and lack of social contact, making it difficult for individuals o manage the terror of death ( Pyszczynski et al., 2021 ). This had an mpact on both proximal and distal defenses. Proximally the pandemic ncreased death anxiety through explicit awareness of personal vulner- bility, while undermining distal defenses that hampered the anxiety- uffering mechanism that people would typically use to defend against ortality threats ( Pyszczynski et al., 2021 ). The contrasting impacts of he pandemic was that the behaviors that would reduce personal vul- erability, impeded the ability of individuals to find meaning and self- steem in their lives ( Pyszczynski et al., 2021 ). .2.4. Mortality salience and human-nature nexus Research has shown that mortality salience impacts the relation- hip between individuals and the natural environment by, for ex- mple, increasing exploitation and consumption of natural resources Kasser and Sheldon, 2000 ). Combined with self-esteem it increased ma- erialism ( Rahimah et al., 2020 ), impacted consumer choices ( Akil et al., 018 ), created a reduced connection to non-humans ( Goldenberg et al., 001 ), increased climate change denial ( Dickinson, 2009 ), was capa- le of both limiting as well as advancing climate action ( Wolfe and ubi, 2018 ), impacted pro-environmental behavior based on norm alience ( Fritsche et al., 2010 ), reduced motivation to protect the natu- al environment for its intrinsic value ( Fritsche and Häfner, 2012 ), and mpacted environmental concern depending on whether or not the nat- ral environment was a source of self-esteem ( Vess and Arndt, 2008 ). or a systematic review of TMT and nature/ environmental research ee Smith et al. (2022) . From a TMT perspective the pandemic can be reated as a mortality reminder (Pyszczynski et al., 2021; Evers et al., 021 ). The pandemic provided an opportunity for the exploration of ortality salience in relation to the relationship between individuals nd the natural environment. .3. The pandemic and the human-nature nexus Although not mortality salience studies per se, research conducted uring the pandemic explored the relationship between individuals nd nature. The pandemic impacted nature activities based on “Nur- ure and Recreation ” values and “Inspiration and Nourishment ” val- es. ( Morse et al., 2020 ). The pandemic also had a positive impact n the relationship between intention and pro-environmental behavior Zebardast and Radaei, 2022 ), and increased environmental awareness ependent on the belief that the pandemic was caused by human in- rusion into nature ( Daryanto et al., 2022 ). An exploratory study by aasova, et al. (2020) showed a relationship between connectedness ith nature, individual representations, behavioral tendencies, and the erceived impact of the pandemic on one’s life and the lives of oth- rs. According to Lucarelli et al. (2020) , there were no differences in nvironmental intentions or pro-environmental behavior prior to and ollowing COVID-19. A study by Stahl (2022) suggested that increased nteractions with nature did not correspond to an increase in human- ature connection during COVID-19. As a result of this inconsistency n findings, it is likely that the impact of the pandemic on the nexus etween humans and nature requires further investigation. 3 .4. The current study This study extends previous work done on the human-nature nexus n South Africa that identified six types of human-nature relation- hips in 2016 through K-means cluster analysis ( Marais-Potgieter and hatcher, 2020 ). Table 1 provides a summary of the main differences etween the Disconnected, Uncommitted, Alarmed, Believer, Concerned nd Pleasure-seeker types. COVID-19 had multiple significant impacts on mortality beliefs with educed hospital beds, lower GDP, diabetes as a co-morbidity, and igher average age being significant risk factors for mortality on differ- nt continents ( Zahid and Perna, 2021 ), 9.1% of the population in sub- aharan Africa being plunged into extreme poverty, and 31.8 million eople severely food deprived after an 8-week lockdown ( Teachout and ipfel, 2020 ), food security impacts due to reduced seed availabil- ty, reduced labor, finance, and farm input access ( Nchanji and Lu- omia, 2021 ), mental health related to anxiety, depression and post- raumatic stress disorders ( Semo and Frissa, 2020 ), gender-based do- estic poly-violence ( Nduna and Tshona, 2021 ), and litter which am- lified three times as lockdowns were alleviated ( Ryan et al., 2020 ) on he African continent. Given these broad social and individual impacts, his study asked how the individual types’ perceptions and attitudes to- ards the natural environment have changed from 2016 (pre COVID) o 2021 (COVID) in South Africa. Although there are various criticisms leveled against TMT by Martin nd van den Bos (2014) and responded to by Pyszczynski et al. (2015) , his study aimed to add to existing TMT literature related to critique egarding applicability across cultures, and that the theory aims to ex- lain the smorgasbord of human behavior. Our study aimed to extend he theory’s applicability to South Africa (rather than mostly in western, ducated, industrialized, rich, democratic countries), and more specifi- ally to individual relationships with the natural environment. Further- ore, by applying individual types to understand shifts, it expands TMT o include individual worldviews. . Method Two surveys were conducted online. One pre-pandemic in 2016 and nother during the pandemic in 2021 in South Africa. The purpose of he 2016 study was to develop types to understand the relationship that ndividuals have with the biosphere in South Africa forming a base for urther exploration in 2021. A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 2 t w m t f t r i w 2 m i g 2 u t o c d i o r T o d e i t r f w a F t p 2 2 t c T c w f a a c t f t c s s m u r 2 E t ( c 0 s p 0 2 b a a f o 2 e t c K p s t a ( 𝛼 2 w u e t ( w 𝛼 2 f m t p v 2 r E r s fi i b P m a a d t d .1. Participants Convenience sampling was used that is a non-probability sampling echnique where individuals participate based on their accessibility and illingness to participate ( Creswell, 2018 ). Due to the data gathering ethod, the sample included mainly urban, literate individuals with In- ernet access, and was likely to include individuals with higher carbon ootprints (rural and those with reduced incomes are likely to lack desk- op internet access), but potentially excluded those living in remote ru- al areas or informal urban settlements. No criteria were set regarding nclusion to obtain the maximum variation possible. 721 respondents ere retained for analysis in 2016 and 655 in 2021. The sample from 021 was post-weighted by age, gender, and ethnicity to match the de- ographic profile of 2016 to compare the two study periods. The partic- pants were not the same individuals in 2021 as in 2016, but they were rouped into the same types based on their responses to the survey. .2. Procedure For both studies social media marketing was used to invite individ- als across South Africa (no limitations were placed on areas) to par- icipate. Invitations were made through an advertising banner placed n Facebook. Using social media as a recruitment tool is becoming in- reasingly popular ( Topolovec-Vranic and Natarajan, 2016 ). Social me- ia have a wide reach, with Facebook being the fourth most visited site n South Africa according to Alexa.com (American web traffic analysis rganization), with 21 million users ( Pienaar, 2018 ). A risk of Facebook ecruitment is cross-participant communication ( Gelinas et al., 2017 ). his possible risk was managed by switching off the comment function n the Facebook invitation. An online survey tool (Alida) was used to eploy the participant information sheet and survey. Individuals could ither scroll past, or click on the banner to be directed to the participant nformation sheet. If participants agreed to participate, they were guided o the online questionnaire battery. Questionnaires and questions were otated. Data quality was checked and duplicate information removed rom the data set, and all those with identical responses across items ere removed. A lucky draw, as a separate survey option, was offered s compensation for their time. The fieldwork took place between 26 ebruary and 31 May in 2016 and 21 May and 10 June in 2021. Due o anonymity, matching participants between 2016 and 2021 was not ossible. .3. Materials .3.1. The environmental issues and attitudes questionnaire (EIAQ) To understand environmental awareness, attitudes, and concern he EIAQ was self-developed to include global concerns (i.e., climate hange) and issues specific to South Africa (i.e., canned lion hunting). he sections included ratings of environmental and animal (awareness, oncerns, and accountability) and general attitudes. The awareness sections explored environmental and animal issues here 1 = I am now less aware (where an individual might have stopped ollowing latest developments on an issue) than before to 3 = I am more ware than before. The concerns sections explored environmental and nimal issues (e.g. biodiversity loss; vivisection), from 1 = Not at all con- erned to 5 = Extremely concerned. The accountability sections asked he extent to which participants felt they had control over the items, rom 1 = I have no control over this to 3 = I have a direct impact. Atti- udes explored, for example, whether participants believed individuals ould contribute to a better environment or it is government’s respon- ibility, from 1 = Strongly disagree to 5 = Strongly agree. The 3-point cales were used based on need for simplicity, less cognitive burden, ore accurate categorical data versus the 5-point scales which were sed in instances were more granularity was required, and a greater ange of measurement was needed (for an analysis see Joshi et al., 015 ). 4 Varimax (orthogonal) rotation factor analysis was conducted on the IAQ in 2016 to determine underlying dimensions in the data for fur- her analysis. The environmental concern subscale yielded four factors shown as 𝛼 = 2016 and 2021): human-centric ( 𝛼 = 0.781 and 0.813), limate-centric ( 𝛼 = 0.816 and 0.855), resource-centric ( 𝛼 = 0.737 and .829), and science-centric ( 𝛼 = 0.643 and 0.704). The animal concern ubscale yielded two factors (shown as 𝛼 = 2016 and 2021): animal ex- loitation ( 𝛼 = 0.890 and 0.874) and animal extinction ( 𝛼 = 0.886 and .875). The attitudes subscale had five factors (shown as 𝛼 = 2016 and 021): shifting responsibility ( 𝛼 = 0.896 and 0.914), individual contri- ution ( 𝛼 = 0.703 and 0.689), government’s responsibility ( 𝛼 = 0.810 nd 0.808), technological solutions ( 𝛼 = 0.815 and 0.854), and apathetic ttitudes ( 𝛼 = 0.712 and 0.811). The EIAQ appeared to have sufficient ace validity (that it measures what it was intended to measure) based n the self-explanatory nature of the items. .3.2. The connectedness to nature scale (CNS) Measuring affect allows for the understanding of the underlying motional processes that impact environmental attitudes and percep- ions. Affect in the form of empathy impacts the relationship between ertain personality traits and connectedness to nature ( Di Fabio and enny, 2021 ). The CNS ( Mayer and Frantz, 2004 ) measured these as- ects as connectedness to nature using 14 items that included feelings uch as a sense of community, egalitarianism, and belongingness to na- ure. Participants responded on a 5-point scale, where 1 = strongly dis- gree and 5 = strongly agree. The test–retest reliability was r = 0.79 Mayer and Frantz, 2004 ). In 2016 the Cronbach 𝛼 = 0.826 and in 2021 = 0.825. .3.3. The new ecological paradigm scale (NEP) Worldviews represent an individual’s inner experience of the outer orld ( Dunlap, 2008 ). The NEP was selected based on its extensive se for measuring environmental worldviews, and use in emerging conomies ( Hawcroft and Milfont, 2010 ). It used 15 items to determine he ecological paradigm (8 items) versus the dominant social paradigm 7 items) ( Dunlap, 2008 ). Participants responded on a 5-point scale, here 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. In 2016 Cronbach = 0.726 and in 2021 𝛼 = 0.737. .4. Ethical considerations Ethical clearance (number H15/11/15 and H21/03/14) was granted rom the University of the Witwatersrand’s Human Research Ethics Com- ittee. The participant information sheet contained detailed informa- ion about the study and contact details of the lead researcher should articipants have any questions before participating. Participation was oluntary. All records were anonymised. .5. Data analysis For the EIAQ, CNS and NEP scales, respondents who had identical esponses across all items in a question batch were excluded. For the IAQ environment and animal subscales, responses of ‘no opinion’ were ecoded as ‘ not at all concerned’. For the EIAQ attitudes subscales, re- ponses of ‘don’t know’ were recoded as ‘neither agree/disagree’. The nal subscales were formed by averaging the scores of the items mak- ng up the subscale. For 2021, respondents were classified into one of six types ased on a discriminant analysis model developed in 2016 ( Marais- otgieter, 2020 ). The seven scales used for the discriminant analysis odel to group individuals into a type were connectedness to nature, nd the EIAQ shifting responsibility, individual contribution, apathetic ttitudes, human-centric, resource-centric, and science-centric. Respon- ents who did not have scores for one or more of the seven scales used o classify respondents into a type were excluded. Appendix 1 shows the emographics between study periods and that there were no significant A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 d e s p F a v t a X s s c s t d t u f l 0 0 b u 3 i r t t c n c c v t 3 t w a n 3 v i A t t c ( s w d t V c Table 2 Overall and type shifts between 2016 and 2021. ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS Overall 2016 2021 n 721 665 % % p More aware 65.1 ∗ 58.2 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.11) Same aware 28.6 ∗ 36.1 ∗ Less aware 2.6 4.1 I don’t know 3.7 1.5 ENVIRONMENTAL CONCERNS Concerned 2016 2021 n 170 153 M M p Climate-centric 4.4 (3.8–4.6) 4.6 (4.0–5.0) < 0.00 ∗ ( r = 0.22) Pleasure-seeker 2016 2021 n 76 93 M M p Resource-centric 3.5 (2.8–4.0) 2.8 (2.5–3.5) < 0.00 ( r = 0.28) ∗ ENVIRONMENTAL CONCERNS ACCOUNTABILITY Overall 2016 2021 n 677 587 % % p Water scarcity Direct impact 37.4 ∗ 27.1 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.14) Small impact 42.5 43.3 No control 19.7 ∗ 27.6 ∗ I don’t know 0.4 2.0 n 562 502 Climate change Direct impact 10.5 ∗ 15.6 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.11) Small impact 35.1 ∗ 40.6 ∗ No control 48.6 ∗ 39.5 ∗ I don’t know 5.9 4.2 n 631 541 Natural resource depletion Direct impact 26.6 ∗ 17.5 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.11) Small impact 41.8 4.,9 No control 28.1 32.9 I don’t know 3.5 3.7 n 483 435 Over consumption Direct impact 45.1 ∗ 29.5 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.18) Small impact 40.2 ∗ 53.9 ∗ No control 13.0 15.6 I don’t know 1.7 1.0 n 353 388 Natural disasters Direct impact 4.3 8.5 < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.18) Small impact 8.2 ∗ 17.8 ∗ No control 82.7 ∗ 71.0 ∗ I don’t know 4.8 2.8 ( continued on next page ) ifferences between surveys within any of the types for age, gender, or thnicity. There was a significant, weak, association, between type and urvey year ( p = 0.013; Cramer’s V = 0.12): 2021 had a greater pro- ortion of Believers, and a lower proportion of Alarmed respondents. urthermore, the 2021 study had a greater proportion of unemployed nd self-employed individuals. The association between survey (2016/2021) and the other study ariables was determined as follows: The X 2 test was used to assess he relationships between survey and categorical variables. Fisher’s ex- ct test was used for 2 × 2 tables or where the requirements for the 2 test could not be met. The strength of the associations was mea- ured by Cramer’s V and the phi coefficient respectively. The following cale of interpretation was used: > 0.50 indicated a high/strong asso- iation, 0.30 to 0.49 a moderate association, 0.10 to 0.29 a weak as- ociation, and below 0.10 little if any association. The relationship be- ween survey and continuous variables was assessed by the indepen- ent samples t -test. Where the data did not meet the assumptions of he t -test, a non-parametric alternative, the Wilcoxon rank sum test was sed. The strength of the associations was measured by the Cohen’s d or parametric tests and the r-value for non-parametric tests. The fol- owing scale of interpretation was used: > 0.80 indicated a large effect, .50 to 0.79 a moderate effect, 0.20 to 0.49 a small effect, and below .20 a near zero effect. Overall, the false discovery rate was controlled y the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. The 5% significance level was sed. . Results Table 2 shows significant results at an overall level, as well as per ndividual type. Significant shifts occurred at an overall level for envi- onmental awareness, environmental concerns and accountability (wa- er scarcity, climate change, natural resource depletion, over consump- ion, natural disasters), animal concerns, as well as attitudes and per- eption environment (shifting responsibility, role of technology). Sig- ificant shifts per type occurred for environmental concerns (climate- entric and resource-centric), natural disasters, animal concerns, animal oncerns accountability (domestic welfare), attitudes and perception en- ironment (shifting responsibility, role of technology), and connection o nature. .1. Environmental awareness Table 2 shows that there was a significant, weak, association, be- ween environmental awareness and year ( p < 0.00; Cramer’s V = 0.11) here 2021 had a greater proportion of “same level of awareness ”, and lower proportion of “more awareness ”, compared to 2016. There were o significant differences between type. .2. Environmental concerns and accountability Overall, there were no significant shifts in the median scores for en- ironmental concerns. For the climate-centric scale there was a signif- cant, but small, increase in the median score for the Concerned type. lthough the resource-centric scale was used to classify respondents into ypes, there was a significant, but small, decrease in median score for he Pleasure-seeker type. For environmental concerns accountability, there was a signifi- ant, weak, association between concern about water scarcity and year p < 0.00; Cramer’s V = 0.14) where 2021 showed a lower level of per- onal accountability compared to 2016. This shift was not significant ithin any of the types. Similar results were found for natural resource epletion, and overconsumption. There was a significant, weak, associa- ion between concern about climate change and year ( p < 0.00; Cramer’s = 0.11) where 2021 showed a higher level of personal accountability ompared to 2016. There was a significant, weak, association between 5 A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 Table 2 ( continued ) Concerned n 353 388 Natural disasters Direct impact 3.2 ∗ 12.8 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.27) Small impact 5.4 ∗ 15.9 ∗ No control 86.0 ∗ 70.4 ∗ I don’t know 5.4 1.0 ANIMAL CONCERNS Overall 2016 2021 n 673 583 M M p Animal exploitation 3.8 (3.0–4.3) 3.9 (3.1–4.6) < 0.00 ∗ ( r = 0.10) Uncommitted 2016 2021 n 113 76 M M p Animal exploitation 3.8 (3.1–4.3) 4.1 (3.6–4.6) < 0.00 ∗ ( r = 0.24) ANIMAL CONCERNS ACCOUNTABILITY Alarmed 2016 2021 n 129 91 % % p Domestic animal welfare Direct impact 48.1 ∗ 27.2 ∗ < 0.00 ∗ ( V = 0.26) Small impact 45.7 ∗ 53.1 ∗ No control 5.4 ∗ 18.4 ∗ I don’t know 0.8 1.3 ATTITUDES AND PERCEPTION ENVIRONMENT Overall 2016 2021 n 721 665 M M p Shifting responsibility 2.3 (0.9) 2.6 (1.0) < 0.00 ∗ ( d = 0.32) Role of technology 2.7 (0.8) 2.9 (0.9) < 0.00 ∗ ( d = 0.24) Believer 2016 2021 n 104 137 M M p Shifting responsibility 3.4 (3.0–3.9) 3.8 (3.2–4.3) < 0.00 ∗ ( r = 0.23) Role of technology 3.2 (0.8) 3.6 (0.9) < 0.00 ∗ ( d = 0.47) CONNECTION TO NATURE Disconnected 2016 2021 n 75 71 M M p Connection to nature 3.8 (0.7) 4.1 (0.6) < 0.00 ∗ ( d = 0.46) EIAQ: Environmental Issues and Attitudes Questionnaire. CNS: Connectedness to Nature Scale. ∗ p < 0.05. c w t w l e G 3 a n ( T w i y i e t w a c 3 t s s n A 3 s t n a 4 ( ( c d m f d a p p b i 2 r m a e o 6 oncern about natural disasters and year ( p < 0.00; Cramer’s V = 0.18) here 2021 showed a higher level of personal accountability compared o 2016. This shift was significant within the Concerned type. There ere no shifts for air pollution, rising sea levels, global warming, ozone ayer depletion, human overpopulation, loss of nature areas, nuclear en- rgy, waste generation, water pollution, urbanization, use of pesticides, MOs, biodiversity loss. .3. Animal concerns and accountability There was no significant association between animal concerns wareness and year overall or within any of the types. There was a sig- ificant increase in the median score of the Animal Exploitation scale p < 0.00; r = 0.10). This was significant also for the Disconnected type. here was no significant shift in the Animal Extinction score, overall or ithin any of the types. For the animal concerns accountability responses there was no signif- cant association between concern about domestic animal welfare and ear, however there was a shift towards lower personal accountabil- ty within the Alarmed type. There were no shifts for animals used in ntertainment, canned lion hunting, legal rights and protection, domes- ic animal breeding, wild animal breeding, deforestation, over-fishing, hale hunting, dolphin capturing, animal cruelty, factory farming, farm nimal welfare, poaching, trophy hunting, animals used for testing or onsumer products. .4. Attitudes and perception environment Environmental attitudes and perception had a significant increase in he mean scores of the Shifting Responsibility and Role of Technology ubscales p < 0.00 ( d = 0.32) and p < 0.00 ( d = 0.24) respectively. These hifts were significant also within the Believer typology. There were o significant shifts in the median Role of Individual, Government or pathetic Attitudes overall or within any of the types. .5. Connectedness to nature Overall, there were no significant shifts in the mean CNS and NEP cores. Although the CNS score is used to classify respondents into types, here was a significant, small, increase in mean score for the Discon- ected type. There was no significant shift in the mean NEP score for ny of the types. . Discussion Disasters have the ability to act as catalysts for social change Cohen, 2020 ) or to change the existential path of individuals Tomaszek and Muchacka-Cymerman, 2020 ). Little research has been onducted, particularly in Africa, that explores the impact of the pan- emic on the human-nature nexus, and what that means for environ- ental challenges. The pandemic did not only function as a distraction rom the climate crisis but had the added potential to improve our un- erstanding of the human-nature nexus under times of extreme threat nd heightened mortality salience. The study asked: Given the multi- le significant impacts of COVID-19 on the African continent, how have erceptions and attitudes towards the environment changed within and etween types of individuals from 2016 (pre COVID) to 2021 (COVID) n South Africa? In this section we discuss the significant shifts that occurred between 016 and 2021 at an overall and individual type level looking at envi- onmental awareness, environmental concerns and accountability, ani- al concerns and accountability, attitudes and perception environment, nd connectedness to nature. The results from this study contribute to xisting research that explored the human-nature nexus, pandemic, and ther variables for example that the pandemic had a positive impact on A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 s w t s o p o w a T 4 a n C i l i a 2 s i D s c w l fi v 2 r f n o n w o 2 4 b c 2 u s ( fi o p t t t w u v d p r e o u c t r t t s a i a r 2 a 2 f d b i i s p t s c n 4 w o c s T i m 4 i t s fi ( 4 c T w t f D w 2 r o T w d i t ome aspects of the human-nature nexus depending on whether there as a belief that the pandemic was caused by human intrusion into na- ure ( Daryanto et al., 2022 ) or whether connectedness to nature impacts upport for COVID-19 travel restrictions ( Pensini and McMullen, 2022 ), r how the pandemic relates to the relationship between intention and ro-environmental behavior ( Zebardast and Radaei, 2022 ). For many f the variables assessed no significant shifts occurred suggesting that hen faced with disaster, individuals prioritize areas of concern that lign with their existing worldviews, supporting the main tenets of MT. .1. Environmental awareness There was a significant decrease in those that were “more aware ” nd a significant increase in those that had the “same level of aware- ess ” of environmental issues. This compared to a study that showed OVID-19 having had the greatest impact on sustainable consumption n Brazil and Portugal, followed by environmental awareness and to a esser extent, social responsibility ( Severo et al., 2021 ). A similar study n Malaysia found a significant and positive impact on environmental wareness, sustainable consumption, and social responsibility ( Ali et al., 021 ). As a result of COVID-19, public awareness of natural resources uch as forests and birdlife increased, while awareness of environmental ssues such as climate change did not change according to Rousseau and eschacht (2020) . The lack of increased awareness of environmental is- ues in our study could potentially be attributed to a decrease in media overage on key environmental issues, like climate change, while there as increased human-centric pandemic coverage (see Mocatta and Haw- ey, 2020 ; Nacu-Schmidt et al., 2020 ; Rauchfleisch et al.,2021 ). This nding could also be attributed to the lack of individuals seeing the irus as an indication of environmental stressors (see Tsantopoulos et al., 021 ) or that variances in connectedness with nature impacted rep- esentations about the pandemic (see Haasova et al., 2020 ) there- ore those more connected already had high environmental aware- ess prior to the pandemic, and those less connected were guided by ther areas of focus (financial, mortality) during the pandemic. Fi- ally, the finding could be a result of the complex social context in hich environmental issues are psychologically contrasted with issues f social justice in South Africa (see Marais-Potgieter and Thatcher, 022 ). .2. Environmental concerns and accountability For environmental concerns only the Concerned type was more trou- led about climate-centric issues and the Pleasure-seeker type was less oncerned about resource issues (they were also the least concerned in 016). These findings support the idea that individuals were driven to phold worldviews, and when in a situation where mortality is made alient, they tended to respond with predictable, and replicable defenses see Wolfe and Tubi, 2018 ). The result for the Concerned type con- rmed that COVID-19 increased environmental concern that expands n studies that indicated shifts in environmental concern during the andemic. Jian et al. (2020) found that fear and uncertainty during he pandemic increased environmental concern and green hotel brand rust, Kenward and Brick (2021) showed higher environmental priori- ization was determined less by social identity and more by individual ell-being. Schiller et al. (2022) used pre-lockdown data and followed- p during the lockdown and found that mental health was reduced, en- ironmental concern increased, and prejudice against asylum-seekers ecreased. This is aligned with the overall worldview profile described er type ( Table 1 ) that indicated the Concerned who had strong envi- onmental worldviews compared to the Pleasure-seeker who had weak nvironmental worldviews. This finding confirms one of the main tenets f TMT in that when faced with mortality salience, individuals tend to phold their worldviews ( Pyszczynski et al., 2015 ). 7 For environmental concerns accountability, there was an overall de- rease in the experience of control where participants either felt no con- rol or that they could have a small impact on water scarcity, natural esource depletion, and over consumption suggested individuals felt that he world was not predictable or controllable. These findings could point o memories and perceived lack of control based on the significant water hortages ( “day zero threats) experienced in South Africa between 2016 nd 2020 ( Robins, 2019 ). Atalay and Meloy (2020) showed that mortal- ty salience after exposure to natural disasters and extreme events had n impact on the appeal of evacuation due to reduced locus of control. For natural disasters the Concerned type showed an increase in “di- ect ” or “small impact ”, but 70.4% still felt like they had no control in 021. There was an increase in individuals that felt like they could have direct impact on climate change, but most of the sample (83.7% in 016, 80.1% in 2021) still felt they could only have a small impact or elt like they had no control. A drop in media focus on the climate crisis uring the pandemic did not correspond to a decline in public concern, ut perhaps only a temporal shift in focus ( Leiserowitz et al., 2020 ). The ncrease in perceptions of control in our study might be because some ndividuals felt that behavioral adjustments (masks, social distancing, anitizing) gave protection during COVID. This finding can also be com- ared to that of Arndt and Solomon (2003) who found that individual raits impacted the sense of, or need for control. In their study the re- ponse to mortality becoming salient, individuals who had high neuroti- ism reported reduced need for personal control, whereas those low in euroticism reported the opposite. .3. Animal concerns and accountability The significant increase in concern regarding animal exploitation as potentially linked to the media coverage of how zoonotic diseases riginate (see Mocatta and Hawley, 2020 ). This aligned with the signifi- ant increase for the Uncommitted type who tended to be influenced by ocial dialogues and media (see Marais-Potgieter and Thatcher, 2020 ). he significant decrease in “direct impact ” and “no control ” and increase n “small impact ” for the Alarmed type pointed to their concern for ani- als and how the pandemic potentially made them feel more hopeless. .4. Attitudes and perception environment The decreased sense of control possibly explained the increased shift n responsibility, and the role of technology at an overall level, and for he Believer type specifically, as they looked for sources of control out- ide the self. Again, the significant movement for the Believer type con- rmed that their existing worldviews were upheld during the pandemic see Table 1 ). .5. Connectedness to nature Interestingly, while connectedness to nature increased for the Dis- onnected type, they were still the least connected out of all the types. he fact that their environmental worldviews did not shift meaningfully as in line with TMT that individuals maintain worldviews to protect hemselves from the terror of mortality and therefore tended to be de- ended during times of mortality salience ( Greenberg et al., 2014 ). The isconnected type most likely experienced increased survival concerns hich could be seen as a subset of mortality salience ( Greenfield et al., 021 ) The study contributed to existing TMT in that it demonstrated a ange of different impacts the pandemic had on individual types based n their worldviews and relationship with the natural environment. he study confirmed that individuals tend to uphold worldviews, and hen there is potential mortality salience, respond with substantial, pre- ictable, and replicable defenses (see Wolfe and Tubi, 2018 ), depend- ng on type. The results related to other work that showed that during imes of crisis, like the pandemic, individual worlds become tighter and A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 s W t i b f o g c u t d 2 v t t c a r a i a n i p t i s g i f p d h r T r i w m 5 n d s b n m E U s A A p p e n d ix 1 S a m p le d e m o g ra p h ic s b y in d iv id u a l ty p e . D is co n n e ct e d U n co m m it te d A la rm e d B e li e v e r C o n ce rn e d P le a su re -s e e k e r Y e a r 2 0 1 6 2 0 2 1 p -v a lu e 2 0 1 6 2 0 2 1 p -v a lu e 2 0 1 6 2 0 2 1 p -v a lu e 2 0 1 6 2 0 2 1 p -v a lu e 2 0 1 6 2 0 2 1 p -v a lu e 2 0 1 6 2 0 2 1 p -v a lu e n 7 5 7 1 1 2 0 9 0 1 7 6 1 2 1 1 0 4 1 3 7 1 7 0 1 5 3 7 6 9 3 % % % % % % % % % % % % A g e g ro u p 1 8 – 2 5 y e a rs 4 8 2 8 0 .1 4 2 4 2 3 0 .4 1 2 2 3 0 0 .1 5 3 1 2 8 0 .9 5 1 1 2 3 0 .0 1 ( V = 0 .2 1 ) 2 9 3 9 0 .1 9 2 6 – 3 5 y e a rs 2 4 3 5 3 1 3 3 3 4 2 5 3 8 4 0 2 2 2 1 3 6 2 6 3 6 – 4 5 y e a rs 1 7 2 5 2 1 2 5 2 1 1 4 2 0 1 9 2 2 2 1 2 2 1 5 4 6 – 5 5 y e a rs 8 1 0 2 2 1 3 1 9 2 6 9 1 1 3 2 3 0 1 2 1 5 5 6 + y e a rs 3 1 3 6 5 5 3 2 1 3 5 1 5 G e n d e r M a le 4 4 5 2 0 .4 1 4 1 3 4 0 .3 2 3 5 4 1 0 .3 3 4 7 4 1 0 .3 6 3 0 3 1 0 .9 0 5 0 4 2 0 .3 6 F e m a le 5 6 4 7 5 9 6 6 6 5 5 9 5 3 5 9 7 0 6 9 5 0 5 8 P o p u la ti o n G ro u p B la ck 6 4 5 8 0 .0 6 4 3 4 2 0 .2 3 3 5 1 9 0 .0 1 ( V = 0 .2 3 ) 5 7 6 0 0 .5 2 1 8 2 7 0 .2 2 3 8 3 5 0 .7 8 M ix e d ra ce / C o lo u re d 1 1 1 2 9 1 2 1 0 9 1 4 1 0 9 1 2 1 5 1 0 A si a n / In d ia n 9 1 1 2 6 1 5 6 8 4 4 6 9 W h it e 1 5 2 5 3 3 4 0 5 1 6 0 1 9 1 9 6 4 5 4 3 8 4 3 O th e r/ R e fu se 1 5 4 0 4 7 4 4 5 4 3 4 ∗ p < 0 .0 . maller, similar to subsistence communities ( Greenfield et al., 2021 ). ithin the context of Greenfield et al. (2021) our results posit that if he environment was already important to a particular type, the clos- ng in of their world would include the natural environment. This can e contrasted with a type where survival was important, and there- ore, COVID-19 impacted those individuals to focus even more closely n their personal survival. Additionally, Greenfield et al. (2021) sug- ested that when there are significant sociodemographic changes, those hanges impact all other areas of living that could mean that the higher nemployment in our sample during 2021 compared to 2016 had fur- her impacted mortality salience and the ability of certain types of in- ividuals to manage the potential threat of death ( Pyszczynski et al., 021 ). The implication for psychology’s role in supporting action against en- ironmental issues is that nuanced approaches are required to deal with he psychological responses from individuals, and that working within he worldviews of individuals is likely to be more effective than trying to hange them. Additionally, this study suggested that mortality salience, nd individual traits and processes could have a significant impact on esponses to climate change. Furthermore, it highlights that climate in- ction, and the perception of general apathy should be challenged as t cannot be explained as a universal and unchanging human nature s aspects such as mortality salience, worldviews, and other human- ature nexus aspects are complex. A generalised approach reduces the mportance of psychological variability between populations, oversim- lifies psychological research and its ability to guide policy, does not ake aspects of culture into account in individual behavior, and justifies naction ( Atkinson and Jacquet, 2021 ). The study had various limitations. The first is that we did not mea- ure behaviors in 2021 that could have provided a broader picture re- arding the impact of the pandemic on claimed behaviors. Another lim- tation was that we were not able to use the same sample of individuals rom 2016 due to anonymity, and that there was no qualitative com- onent in 2021 that explored in-depth responses in relation to the pan- emic. It is also possible that other social concerns from COVID-19 could ave had a greater influence than the health-mortality concerns. We also ecommend caution that we did not have data that conclusively linked MT (or COVID-19) to the trends in the 2016 and 2021 data. Future esearch could focus on establishing pre-existing worldviews, trigger- ng a pandemic reminder, and then conducting a follow-up measure of orldviews after a delay (see Cox et al., 2020 ; Smith et al., 2022 for ethods). . Conclusion The pandemic had an impact on certain aspects of the human-nature exus and for specific types. The results suggested that individuals are riven to uphold worldviews, and when threatened they tend to re- pond with considered, predictable, and replicable defenses as suggested y TMT ( Wolfe and Tubi, 2018 ). The results conclude that psychology eeds to work within the psychological boundaries of types for maxi- um efficacy when addressing environmental challenges. thics & informed consent statement ∗ The authors declare that ethical approval was received from the niversity (H210314 – attached) ∗ The authors confirm that all participants provided informed con- ent for participation ppendix 8 A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 D i t C m s c D A R S t R A A A A A B B B B C C C C C D D D D D D E E F F F F G G G G G G G H H H I J J K K L L L L M eclaration of Competing Interest The authors declare that they have no known competing financial nterests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence he work reported in this paper. RediT authorship contribution statement Andrea Marais-Potgieter: Conceptualization, Data curation, For- al analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Re- ources, Visualization, Writing – original draft. Andrew Thatcher: Con- eptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ata availability Data will be made available on request. cknowledgement This study was made possible by the University of the Witwatersrand esearch Committee Postdoctoral Fellowship. upplementary materials Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in he online version, at doi: 10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100096 . eferences kil, H., Robert-Demontrond, P., Bouillé, J., 2018. Exploitation of mortality salience in communication on climate change. Rech. Appl. Mark. (English Edition) 33 (1), 2–29. doi: 10.1177/2051570717745839 . li, Q., Parveen, S., Yaacob, H., Zaini, Z., Sarbini, N.A., 2021. COVID-19 and dynamics of environmental awareness, sustainable consumption and so- cial responsibility in Malaysia. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 28, 56199–56218. doi: 10.1007/s11356-021-14612-z . rndt, J., Solomon, S., 2003. The control of death and the death of control: the effects of mortality neuroticism and mortality salience on desire for personal control. J. Res. Pers. 37, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00530-5 . talay, A.S., Meloy, M.G., 2020. Improving evacuation compliance through control: impli- cations for emergency management policy and disaster communications. J. Nonprofit Public Sect. Mark. 32 (4), 364–378. doi: 10.1080/10495142.2020.1798855 . tkinson, Q.D., Jacquet, J., 2021. Challenging the idea that humans are not designed to solve climate change. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 17 (3), 619–630. doi: 10.1177/17456916211018454 . ecker, E., 1973. The Denial of Death. Simon & Schuster, New York . ernstein, J., Szuster, B., 2018. Beyond unidimensionality: segmenting contemporary pro-environmental worldviews through surveys and repertory grid analysis. Environ. Commun. 12 (8), 1062–1076. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2018.1504809 . orneman, T., Irish, T., Sidhu, R., Koczywas, M., Cristea, M., 2014. Death awareness, feelings of uncertainty, and hope in advanced lung cancer patients: can they coexist? Int J Palliat Nurs 20 (6), 271–277. doi: 10.12968/ijpn.2014.20.6.271 . uijs, A.E., Fischer, A., Rink, D., Young, J.C., 2008. Looking beyond superficial knowl- edge gaps: understanding public representations of biodiversity. Int. J. Biodivers. Sci. Ecosyst. Serv. Manag. 4, 65–80. doi: 10.3843/Biodiv.4.2:1 . appelli, M.L., 2020. Black Lives Matter: the emotional and racial dynamics of the George Floyd protest graffiti. AASoci 9 (10), 323. doi: 10.4236/aasoci.2020.109020 . layton, S., Devine-Wright, P., Stern, P.C., Whitmarsh, L., Carrico, A., Steg …, L., Bonnes, M., 2015. Psychological research and global climate change. Nat. Clim. Change 5, 640–646. doi: 10.1038/nclimate2622 . ohen, M.J., 2020. Does the COVID-19 outbreak mark the onset of a sustainable consumption transition? Sustainability: science, Practice and Policy, 16(1), 1–3, doi: 10.1080/15487733.2020.1740472 . ox, N., Ganong, P., Noel, P., Vavra, J., Wong, A., Farrell ..., D., Deadman, E., 2020. Initial impacts of the pandemic on consumer behavior: evidence from linked income, spending, and savings data. Brookings Pap Econ Act 2020 (2), 35–82 . reswell, J.W. (2018). Research design: qualitative, quantitative, and Mixed Methods Ap- proaches (5th ed.). Sage publications. aryanto, A., Song, Z., Soopramanien, D., 2022. The COVID-19 pandemic as an impetus for pro-environmental behaviours: the role of causal attribution. Pers. Individ. Differ. 187, 111415. 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111415 . ickinson, J., 2009. The people paradox: self-esteem striving, immortality ideologies, and human response to climate change. Ecol Soc 14 (1), 34 . i Fabio, A., Kenny, M.E., 2021. Connectedness to nature, personality traits and empathy from a sustainability perspective. Curr. Psychol. 40, 1095–1106. doi: 10.1007/s12144-018-0031-4 . oty, D.H., Glick, W.H., 1994. Typologies as a unique form of theory building: toward improved understanding and modeling. AMR 19 (2), 230–251. doi: 10.2307/258704 . 9 ouglas, M., Wildavsky, A., 1982. Risk and Culture. University of California Press, Berke- ley and Los Angeles . unlap, R.E., 2008. The New Environmental Paradigm Scale: from marginality to world- wide use. J. Environ. Educ. 40 (1), 3–17. doi: 10.3200/JOEE.40.1.3-18 . rtz, M., Sarigöllü, E., 2019. The behavior-attitude relationship and satisfac- tion in proenvironmental behavior. Environ. Behav. 51 (9–10), 1106–1132. doi: 10.1177/0013916518783241 . vers, N.F., Greenfield, P.M., Evers, G.W., 2021. COVID-19 shifts mortality salience, ac- tivities, and values in the United States: big data analysis of online adaptation. Hum. Behav. Emerg. 3 (1), 107–126. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.251 . isher, A., 2013. Radical ecopsychology: Psychology in the Service of Life. Suny Press, New York, NY . iss, P.C., 2011. Building better causal theories: a fuzzy set approach to ty- pologies in organization research. Acad. Manage. J. 54 (2), 393–420. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23045087 . ritsche, I., Häfner, K., 2012. The malicious effects of existential threat on motivation to protect the natural environment and the role of environmental identity as a modera- tor. Environ Behav 44 (4), 570–590. doi: 10.1177/001391651039775 . ritsche, I., Jonas, E., Kayser, D.N., Koranyi, N., 2010. Existential threat and compliance with pro-environmental norms. J Environ Psychol 30 (1), 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.08.007 . elinas, L., Pierce, R., Winkler, S., Cohen, I.G., Lynch, H.F., Bierer, B.E., 2017. Using social media as a research recruitment tool: ethical issues and recommendations. AJOB 17 (3), 3–14. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2016.1276644 . oldenberg, J.L., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Kluck, B., Cornwell, R., 2001. I am not an animal: mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human creatureliness. J Exp Psychol Gen, 130(3), 427–435, doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.427 . reenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., 1986. The causes and consequences of a need for self-esteem: a terror management theory. In: Baumeister, R.F. (Ed.), Public Self and Private Self. Springer-Verlag, Germany, pp. 189–207 . reenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., Lyon, D., 1990. Evidence for terror management theory II: the effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. J Pers Soc Psychol 58 (2), 308–318. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.308 . reenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., 1997. Terror Management Theory of Self-Es- teem and Cultural Worldviews: empirical Assessments and Conceptual Refinements. In: Zanna, Mark (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 61-139. Aca- demic Press, Orlando, Fla . reenberg, J., Vail, K., Pyszczynski, T., 2014. Terror management theory and research: how the desire for death transcendence drives our strivings for meaning and signifi- cance. In: Elliot, A. J. (Ed.), Advances in Motivation Science, Vol. 1, Elsevier Academic Press, pp. 85–34. reenfield, P.M., Brown, G., Du, H., 2021. Shifts in ecology, behavior, values, and relationships during the coronavirus pandemic: survival threat, subsistence activ- ities, conservation of resources, and interdependent families. CRESP 2, 100017. 10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100017 . aasova, S., Czellar, S., Rahmani, L., Morgan, N., 2020. Connectedness with nature and individual responses to a pandemic: an exploratory study. Front. Psychol. 11, 2215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02215 . all, M.P., Lewis Jr, N.A., Ellsworth, P.C., 2018. Believing in climate change, but not behaving sustainably: evidence from a one-year longitudinal study. J Environ Psychol 56, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.03.001 . awcroft, L.J., Milfont, T.L., 2010. The use (and abuse) of the new environmental paradigm scale over the last 30 years: a meta-analysis. J Environ Psychol 30, 143–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.10.003 . nauen, J., Contzen, N., Frick, V., Kadel, P., Keller, J., Kollmann ..., J., van Valken- goed, A.M., 2021. Environmental issues are health issues: making a case and set- ting an agenda for environmental health psychology. Eur Psychol 26 (3), 219. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000438 . ian, Y., Yu, I.Y., Yang, M.X., Zeng, K., 2020. The impacts of fear and uncertainty of COVID-19 on environmental concerns, brand trust, and behavioral intentions toward green hotels. Sustainability 12, 8688. doi: 10.3390/su12208688 . oshi, A., Kale, S., Chandel, S., Pal, D., 2015. Likert Scale: explored and Explained. Br. J. Appl. Sci. 7, 396–403. doi: 10.9734/BJAST/2015/14975 . asser, T., Sheldon, K.M., 2000. Of wealth and death: materialism, mortality salience, and consumption behavior. Psychol Sci 11 (4), 348–351. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00269 . enward, B., Brick, C., 2021. Even conservative voters want the environment to be at the heart of post-COVID-19 economic reconstruction in the UK. J. Soc. Political Psychol. 9 (1). doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00961-0 . eiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., 2009. Global warming’s six Americas: an audience segmentation. Yale Program on Climate Change Communication. Retrieved from http://research.yale.edu/environment/climate . eiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Rosenthal, S., Kotcher, J., Bergquist, P., Ballew, M., Gold- berg, M., Gustafson, A., Wang, X., 2020. Climate Change in the American mind. Yale University and George Mason University. Yale Program on Climate Change Commu- nication, New Haven, CT . orenzoni, I., Whitmarsh, L., 2014. Climate change and perceptions, behaviors, and com- munication research after the IPCC 5th assessment report. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim Change 5 (6), 703–708. doi: 10.1002/wcc.319 . ucarelli, C., Mazzoli, C., Severini, S., 2020. Applying the theory of planned behavior to examine pro-environmental behavior: the moderating effect of COVID-19 beliefs. Sustainability 12, 10556. doi: 10.3390/su122410556 . acDonald, E., Harbrow, M., Jack, S., Kidd, J., Wright, A., Tuinder …, P., Poutasi, M., 2019. Segmenting urban populations for greater conservation gains: a new approach targeting cobenefits is required. Conserv. Sci. Pract. 1 (10), 1–12. doi: 10.1111/csp2.101 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2023.100096 https://doi.org/10.1177/2051570717745839 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11356-021-14612-z https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00530-5 https://doi.org/10.1080/10495142.2020.1798855 https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211018454 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0006 https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2018.1504809 https://doi.org/10.12968/ijpn.2014.20.6.271 https://doi.org/10.3843/Biodiv.4.2:1 https://doi.org/10.4236/aasoci.2020.109020 https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate2622 https://doi.org/10.1080/15487733.2020.1740472 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0013 http://10.1016/j.paid.2021.111415 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0017 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0031-4 https://doi.org/10.2307/258704 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0020 https://doi.org/10.3200/JOEE.40.1.3-18 https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916518783241 https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.251 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0024 http://www.jstor.org/stable/23045087 https://doi.org/10.1177/001391651039775 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.08.007 https://doi.org/10.1080/15265161.2016.1276644 https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.427 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0030 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.308 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0032 http://10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100017 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02215 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.03.001 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.10.003 https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000438 https://doi.org/10.3390/su12208688 https://doi.org/10.9734/BJAST/2015/14975 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00269 https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00961-0 http://research.yale.edu/environment/climate http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0044 https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.319 https://doi.org/10.3390/su122410556 https://doi.org/10.1111/csp2.101 A. Marais-Potgieter and A. Thatcher Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology 4 (2023) 100096 M M M M M M M M M M M N N N O P P P P P P R R R R R R R S S S S S S S S S S S T T T T T T V D W W W Z Z arais-Potgieter, A., 2020. Developing Typologies to Use As an Ecopsychological Framework For Understanding the Relationship That People Have With the Bio- sphere in South Africa (Doctoral Thesis). University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg . arais-Potgieter, A., Thatcher, A., 2020. Identification of six emergent types based on cognitive and affective constructs that explain individuals’ relationship with the bio- sphere. Sustainability 12, 7614. doi: 10.3390/su12187614 . arais-Potgieter, A., Thatcher, A., 2022. The relationship between the COVID-19 pan- demic and environmental attitudes and what this means for environmental justice. PINS 64, 90–108. doi: 10.57157/pins2022Vol64iss1a5457 . artin, L.L., Van den Bos, K., 2014. Beyond terror: towards a paradigm shift in the study of threat and culture. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 25 (1), 32–70. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2014.923144 . ayer, F.S., Frantz, C.M., 2004. The connectedness to nature scale: a measure of individuals’ feeling in community with nature. J Environ Psychol 24, 503–515. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001 . cDonald, R.I., Chai, H.Y., Newell, B.R., 2015. Personal experience and the ‘psychological distance’ of climate change: an integrative review. J Environ Psychol 44, 109–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.10.003 . cKinney, J.C., 1969. Typification, typologies, and sociological theory. Soc Forces 48 (1), 1–13. doi: 10.2307/2575463 . ishel, M.H., 1999. Uncertainty in chronic illness. Annu Rev Nurs Res 17 (1), 269–294 . ocatta, G., Hawley, E., 2020. The coronavirus crisis as tipping point: communicat- ing the environment in a time of pandemic. Media Int. Aust. 177 (1), 119–124. doi: 10.1177/1329878X20950030 . orse, J.W., Gladkikh, T.M., Hackenburg, D.M., Gould, R.K., 2020. COVID-19 and human-nature relationships: vermonters’ activities in nature and associated nonma- terial values during the pandemic. PLoS ONE 15 (12), e0243697. doi: 10.1371/jour- nal.pone.0243697 . otyl, M., Hart, J., Pyszczynski, T., 2010. When animals attack: the effects of mortality salience, infrahumanization of violence, and authoritarianism on support for war. J Exp Soc Psychol 46 (1), 200–203. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.012 . acu-Schmidt, A., Pearman, O., … Boykoff, M., 2020. This historic decline in emissions is happening for all the wrong reasons. MeCCO Monthly Summary, Issue 40. April. Available at: https://scholar.colorado.edu/concern/articles/wm117p983 . chanji, E.B., Lutomia, C.K., 2021. Regional impact of COVID-19 on the produc- tion and food security of common bean smallholder farmers in Sub-Saharan Africa: implication for SDG’s. Glob. Food Sec. 29, 100524. 10.1016/j.gfs.2021. 100524 . duna, M., Tshona, S.O., 2021. Domesticated Poly-Violence Against Women Dur- ing the 2020 Covid-19 Lockdown in South Africa. Psychol. Stud. 66, 347–353. doi: 10.1007/s12646-021-00616-9 . ltedal, S., Moen, B., Klempe, H., Rundmo, T., 2004. Explaining Risk perception: An eval- uation of Cultural Theory. Rotunde, Trondheim . ensini, P., McMullen, J., 2022. Anthropomorphising nature in times of crisis: a se- rial mediation model from connectedness to nature via anthropomorphism on support for COVID-19 travel restrictions. Curr. Res. Soc. Psychol. 3, 100024. 10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100024 . ienaar, J., 2018. October. Profiling the social media user in South Africa. Bizcommunity. Retrieved from. https://www.bizcommunity.com/Article/196/19/183501.html . oortinga, W., Darnton, A., 2016. Segmenting for sustainability: the development of a sustainability segmentation model from a Welsh sample. J Environ Psychol 45, 221– 232. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.009 . yszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., 1999. A dual process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: an extension of terror management theory. Psychol Rev 106, 835–845. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.835 . yszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., 2015. Thirty Years of Terror Man- agement Theory: from Genesis to Revelation. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 52, 1–70. doi: 10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.03.001 . yszczynski, T., Lockett, M., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., 2021. Terror Management Theory and the COVID-19 Pandemic. J. Humanist. Psychol. 61 (2), 173–189. doi: 10.1177/0022167820959488 . ahimah, A., Khalil, S., Dang, H.P., Cheng, J.M.S., 2020. The terror of death and consumers’ sustainability attitudes. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 57, 102196. 10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102196 . ampedi, I.T., Ifegbesan, A.P., 2022. Understanding the Determinants of Pro- Environmental Behavior among South Africans: evidence from a Structural Equation Model. Sustainability 14 (6), 3218. doi: 10.3390/su14063218 . auchfleisch, A., Siegen, D., Vogler, D., 2021. How COVID-19 displaced climate change: mediated climate change activism and issue attention in the Swiss media and online sphere. Environ. Commun. 1–9. doi: 10.1080/17524032.2021.1990978 . obins, S., 2019. Day Zero’, hydraulic citizenship and the defence of the commons in Cape Town: a case study of the politics of water and its infrastructures (2017–2018). J. South Afr. Stud. 45 (1), 5–29. doi: 10.1080/03057070.2019.1552424 . osenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Lyon, D., 1989. Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. J Pers Soc Psychol 57 (4), 681–690. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681 . 10 ousseau, S., Deschacht, N., 2020. Public awareness of nature and the envi- ronment during the COVID-19 crisis. Environ. Resour. Econ. 76, 1149–1159. doi: 10.1007/s10640-020-00445-w . yan, P.G., Maclean, K., Weideman, E.A., 2020. The Impact of the COVID-19 Lock- down on Urban Street Litter in South Africa. Environ. Process. 7, 1303–1312. doi: 10.1007/s40710-020-00472-1 . aunders, C.D., Brook, A.T., Myers, O.E., 2005. Using psychology to save biodiversity and human well-being. Conserv Biol 20 (3), 702–705. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00435.x . chiller, B., Tönsing, D., Kleinert, T., Böhm, R., Heinrichs, M., 2022. Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic nationwide lockdown on mental health, environmental con- cern, and prejudice against other social groups. Environ. Behav. 54 (2), 1–22. doi: 10.1177/00139165211036991 . evero, E.A., De Guimarães, J.C.F., Dellarmelin, M.L., 2021. Impact of the COVID-19 pan- demic on environmental awareness, sustainable consumption and social responsibil- ity: evidence from generations in Brazil and Portugal. J. Clean. Prod. 286, 124947. 10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.124947 . emo, W., Frissa, S.M., 2020. The Mental Health Impact of the COVID-19 Pan- demic: implications for Sub-Saharan Africa. Psychol Res. Behav. 13, 713–720. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S264286 . ingla, H., Mehta, M.D., Mehta, P., 2021. Manifesting hope in despair: explor- ing prosocial behavioural outcomes of COVID-19. Int. Soc. Sci. J. 71, 51–67. doi: 10.1111/issj.12290 . mith, L.K., Ross, H.C., Shouldice, S.A., Wolfe, S.E., 2022. Mortality management and climate action: a review and reference for using Terror Management Theory meth- ods in interdisciplinary environmental research. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim e776. doi: 10.1002/wcc.776 . pence, A., Poortinga, W., Pidgeon, N., 2012. The psychological distance of climate change. Risk Anal 32 (6), 957–972. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01695.x . olomon, S., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., 1991. A terror management theory of so- cial behavior: the psychological functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews. In: Zanna, M.P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Academic Press, Or- lando, pp. 91–159 . olomon, S., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T.A., 2015. The worm at the core: on the role of death in life. Random House. tahl, J.K., 2022. Nature in crisis? Exploring the Effects of the COVID-19 Pandemic On Human-Nature Interaction and the Connection in Germany (Unpublished masters Dis- sertation). Humboldt University, Berlin . teg, L., Vlek, C., 2009. Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour: an inte- grative review and research agenda. J Environ Psychol 29 (3), 309–317. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004 . eachout, M., Zipfel, C., 2020. The economic impact of COVID-19 lockdowns in sub- Saharan Africa. IGC, www.theigc.org/covid-19 . eel, T.L., Manfredo, M.J., 2010. Understanding the diversity of public interests in wildlife conservation. Conserv Biol 24 (1), 128–139. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-1739.2009.01374.x . hompson, M., 1980. An Outline of the Cultural Theory of Risk (Working Paper No. WP-80-177). IIASA, Laxenburg . omaszek, K., Muchacka-Cymerman, A., 2020. Thinking about my existence during COVID-19, I feel anxiety and awe – the mediating role of existential anxiety and life satisfaction on the relationship between PTSD symptoms and post-traumatic growth. Int J Environ Res Public Health 17 (7026), 1–3. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17197062 . opolovec-Vranic, J., Natarajan, K., 2016. The use of social media in recruitment for medical research studies: a scoping review. JMIR 18 (11), e286. doi: 10.2196/jmir. 5698 . santopoulos, G., Papageorgiou, A.C., Karasmanaki, E., 2021. COVID-19: an outcome of biodiversity loss or a conspiracy? Investigating the attitudes of environmental stu- dents. Sustainability 13, 5307. doi.org/10.3390/su13095307 . ess, M., Arndt, J., 2008. The nature of death and the death of nature: the impact of mortality salience on environmental concern. J. Res. Pers. 42 (5), 1376–1380. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.007 . epartment for Environment, Food and Rural Affairs (Defra). 2008. A framework for pro-environmental behaviours. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/ uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/69277/pb13574-behaviours-report- 080110.pdf olfe, S.E., Tubi, A., 2018. Terror Management Theory and morality awareness: a missing link in climate response studies? WIRE’s Climate change, 10, 10.1002/wcc.566. right, L.J., Afari, N., Zautra, A., 2009. The illness uncertainty concept: a review. Curr Pain Headache Rep 13 (2), 133–138. doi: 10.1007/s11916-009-0023-z . ynes, S., Nicholas, K.A., 2017. The climate mitigation gap: education and government recommendations miss the most effective individual actions. Environ Res Lett 12 (7), 1–6 . ahid, M.N., Perna, S., 2021. Continent-wide analysis of COVID-19: total cases, deaths, tests, socio-economic, and morbidity factors associated to the mortality rate, and forecasting analysis in 2020-2021. Int J Environ Res Public Health 18, 5350. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18105350 . ebardast, L., Radaei, M., 2022. The influence of global crises on reshaping pro- environmental behavior, case study: the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Total Environ 881, 151436. 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2021.151436 . http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0048 https://doi.org/10.3390/su12187614 https://doi.org/10.57157/pins2022Vol64iss1a5457 https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2014.923144 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.10.001 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.10.003 https://doi.org/10.2307/2575463 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0055 https://doi.org/10.1177/1329878X20950030 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0243697 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.012 https://scholar.colorado.edu/concern/articles/wm117p983 http://10.1016/j.gfs.2021.100524 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-021-00616-9 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0062 http://10.1016/j.cresp.2021.100024 https://www.bizcommunity.com/Article/196/19/183501.html https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.009 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.835 https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.03.001 https://doi.org/10.1177/0022167820959488 http://10.1016/j.jretconser.2020.102196 https://doi.org/10.3390/su14063218 https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2021.1990978 https://doi.org/10.1080/03057070.2019.1552424 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10640-020-00445-w https://doi.org/10.1007/s40710-020-00472-1 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2006.00435.x https://doi.org/10.1177/00139165211036991 http://10.1016/j.jclepro.2020.124947 https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S264286 https://doi.org/10.1111/issj.12290 https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.776 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01695.x http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0083 http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0085 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.004 http://www.theigc.org/covid-19 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1523-1739.2009.01374.x http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0089 https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17197062 https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.\penalty -\@M 5698 http://doi.org/10.3390/su13095307 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.007 https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/69277/pb13574-behaviours-report-080110.pdf https://doi.org/10.1007/s11916-009-0023-z http://refhub.elsevier.com/S2666-6227(23)00009-6/sbref0096 https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18105350 http://10.1016/j.scitotenv.2021.151436 Exploring the impact of the pandemic on the relationship between individual types and the natural environment: the role of mortality concerns 1 Introduction 1.1 Relationship between types of individuals and the natural environment 1.2 Terror management theory and the human-nature nexus 1.2.1 Self-esteem and cultural worldviews 1.2.2 Individual traits and mortality salience 1.2.3 Proximal and distal defenses 1.2.4 Mortality salience and human-nature nexus 1.3 The pandemic and the human-nature nexus 1.4 The current study 2 Method 2.1 Participants 2.2 Procedure 2.3 Materials 2.3.1 The environmental issues and attitudes questionnaire (EIAQ) 2.3.2 The connectedness to nature scale (CNS) 2.3.3 The new ecological paradigm scale (NEP) 2.4 Ethical considerations 2.5 Data analysis 3 Results 3.1 Environmental awareness 3.2 Environmental concerns and accountability 3.3 Animal concerns and accountability 3.4 Attitudes and perception environment 3.5 Connectedness to nature 4 Discussion 4.1 Environmental awareness 4.2 Environmental concerns and accountability 4.3 Animal concerns and accountability 4.4 Attitudes and perception environment 4.5 Connectedness to nature 5 Conclusion Ethics & informed consent statement Appendix Declaration of Competing Interest CRediT authorship contribution statement Acknowledgement Supplementary materials References