Learning about Astronomy: a case study 
exploring how grade 7 and 8 students 
experience sites of informal learning in 
South Africa  
Anthony Douglas Lelliott 
A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Humanities, University of the 
Witwatersrand, Johannesburg in fulfilment of the requirements of the 
degree of Doctor of Philosophy. 
Johannesburg 2007 
 
 
 v
 Table of Contents 
A b s t r a c t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i 
Keywords ....................................................................................................................... i i 
Declaration .................................................................................................................... i i i 
Acknowledgements ....................................................................................................... iv 
Table of Contents . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v 
List of Appendic e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i x 
List of Figures ................................................................................................................ x 
List of Tables ............................................................................................................... xi i 
Abbreviations .............................................................................................................. xiv 
1  Introduction to the study............................................................................................. 1  
1.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 
1.2 Backgrou n d and Rational e for the Study ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 
1.3 Researc h Problem and Researc h Questio n s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 
1.4 Conceptual Framework ........................................................................................ 6 
1.5 The Research e r and Position a l i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 
1.6 Structure of the Thesis .......................................................................................... 8 
2 Literature Review ...................................................................................................... 10  
2.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 0 
2.2 Research in Science Educatio n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 0 
2.3 Astronomy Educatio n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 
2.3. 1  Resea r c h in Plane t a r i a . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 3  
2.4 Informal Le arning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 
2.5 Edutainme n t or Entercat i o n ? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 8 
2.6 Science commun i c a t i o n and science litera c y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 1 
2.7 Learnin g in museums and science centres . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 3 
2.8 Theoreti c a l Framewor k . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 7 
2.8. 1  The Cont e x t u a l Mode l of Learn i n g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 7  
2.8. 2  Hu man Cons t r u c t i v i s m . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 9  
2.8. 3  Conc e p t u a l Chan g e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 5  
2.9 Summary ............................................................................................................. 47 
3  Research Design and Methodology .......................................................................... 49  
3.1 Introduction and Overview ................................................................................. 49 
3.2 Methodology ....................................................................................................... 50 
3.2. 1  Parad i g ms of Resea r c h . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 0  
3.2. 2  A Case Stud y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 1  
3.3 Research Instruments .......................................................................................... 54 
3.3. 1  Perso n a l Mean i n g Mapp i n g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 5  
3.3. 2  Inter v i e w Sche d u l e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 9  
3.3. 3  The pre-v i s i t inte r v i e w sch e d u l e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 9  
 vi
 3.3 . 4  The post - v i s i t inte r v i e w sche d u l e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 1  
3.3. 5  Inter v i e w s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 1  
3.4 Data Collection ................................................................................................... 62 
3.4. 1  Selec t i o n of the Stud y Site s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 2  
3.4. 2  Selec t i o n of the Part i c i p a n t s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 4  
3.4. 3  Tria n g u l a t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 7  
3.5 Ethical issues ...................................................................................................... 68 
3.6 Issues of credibil i t y and trustwor t h i n e s s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 1 
3.6. 1  Vali d i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 1  
3.6. 2  Relia b i l i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 2  
3.6. 3  Refl e x i v i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 2  
3.7 Reflection ........................................................................................................... 73 
4  Setting the Scene ? a narrative of the visits ............................................................. 74  
4.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 4 
4.2 A visit to Hartebe e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Obse rva t o r y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 7 
4.2. 1  Befor e the visi t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 7  
4.2. 2  ?Plan e t s and stars and like, well, space ? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 9  
4.2. 3  The Sun, Whisp e r s and Rock e t s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 2  
4.2. 4  Break with Grav i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 7  
4.2. 5  Takin g the Sola r Sy st e m for a Walk. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 9  
4.2. 6  Land i n g on the Moon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 1  
4.2. 7  The Teles c o p e and Contr o l Room . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 3  
4.2. 8  After the visi t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 5  
4.3 Discussion ........................................................................................................... 96 
4.4 A visit to the Johannesburg Planetarium ............................................................ 99 
4.5 Discussion ......................................................................................................... 106 
5  An Analysis of Collective Learning ........................................................................ 108  
5.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 0 8 
5.2 The Research Sample ....................................................................................... 108 
5.3 Learning in Science Centres . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 1 
5.4 Big Ideas ........................................................................................................... 113 
5.4. 1  Grav i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 3  
5.4. 2  Stars and the Sun . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 6  
5.4. 3  Sola r Sy st e m . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 8  
5.4. 4  Size and Scal e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 9  
5.5 Signific a n t Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 0 
5.5. 1  Day and Nigh t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 0  
5.5. 2  Phase s of the Moon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 2  
5.5. 3  Do min a n t Artef a c t : Para b o l i c / S a t e l l i t e Di sh . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 2  
5.6 Coding for Big Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 3 
5.6. 1  Hele n and grav i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 6  
5.6. 2  Hele n and star s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 8  
5.6. 3  Helen ? s conc e p t i o n of the Sun . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 9  
5.6. 4  Helen ? s conce p t i o n of the solar sy ste m . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 9  
5.6. 5  Helen ? s conce p t i o n of size and scale . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 0  
5.6. 6  Helen ? s under s t a n d i n g of day and night . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 0  
5.6. 7  Helen and the phas e s of the Moon . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 1  
 vii
 5. 6 . 8  Siph o and sate l l i t e dish e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 2  
5.7 The Extent of Learning ..................................................................................... 133 
5.7. 1  Prepa r a t i o n for, antic i p a t i o n of a nd know l e d g e rega r d i n g the visi t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 4  
5.8 Knowled g e about Big Ideas in as trono my prior to the visit .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 8 
5.8. 1  Grav i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 8  
5.8. 2  Stars and the Sun . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4 3  
5.8. 3  The Sola r sy ste m . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4 6  
5.8. 4  Size and scal e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4 7  
5.8. 5  Day and Nigh t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 0  
5.8. 6  The Moon Phas e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 5  
5.8. 7  Parab o l i c / S a t e l l i t e Dish e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 6  
5.9 Summary of collecti v e learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 7 
6  A Human Constructivist An alysis of Learning ..................................................... 160  
6.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 0 
6.2 Human Construc t i v i s m and Conceptu a l Change .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 1 
6.3 Cognitiv e Do ma in .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 3 
6.3. 1  Addi t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 3  
6.3. 2  Emer g e n c e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 5  
6.3. 3  Diffe r e n t i a t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 6  
6.3. 4  Disc r i m i n a t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 7  
6.3. 5  Recon t e x t u a l i s a t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 8  
6.3. 6  Super o r d i n a te Lear n i n g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 9  
6.4 Affectiv e Do ma in .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 1 
6.4. 1  Enjo y ab l e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 1  
6.4. 2  Germ a n e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 1  
6.4. 3  Sali e n t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 2  
6.4. 4  Wond e r . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 2  
6.5 Conative Domain .............................................................................................. 173 
6.6 Coding for Human Construc t i v i s t categori e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 5 
6.7 Problems encount e r e d with the coding process . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 5 
7 How individual students learnt about astronomy (1) ........................................... 178  
7.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 8 
7.2 The Schools ...................................................................................................... 178 
7.2. 1  Balfo u r Fore s t Scho o l (BFS ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 8  
7.2. 2  Lourd e s Girl s Scho o l (LGS) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 9  
7.2. 3  Achie v e m e n t Scho o l (AS) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 9  
7.2. 4  Boka mo s o Scho o l (BS) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 7 9  
7.3 Big Ideas and Individu a l Learning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 0 
7.4 Use of Astronomy- R e l a t e d Vocabula r y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 5 
7.5 The Students ..................................................................................................... 187 
7.5. 1  Portr a i t of Nonku l u l e k o (swo 2 6 ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 8  
7.5. 2  Portr a i t of Botho (tsw 0 4 ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 9 5  
7.5. 3  Port r a i t of Neo (swo 4 2 ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 0 2  
7.5. 4  Portr a i t of John (vho 16) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 1 1  
7.6 Discussion ......................................................................................................... 221 
 viii
 8 How individual students learnt about astronomy (2) ........................................... 223  
8.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 3 
8.1. 1  Portr a i t of Fatim a (scf 1 5 ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 3  
8.1. 2  Portr a i t of Brend a (swo 7 0 ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 3 1  
8.1. 3  Portr a i t of Helen (scf 1 1 ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 4 0  
8.2 Discussion ......................................................................................................... 248 
9  Discussion and Implications.................................................................................... 251  
9.1 Introduc t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 1 
9.2 Big Ideas in astronomy ..................................................................................... 252 
9.2. 1  Grav i t y . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 2  
9.2. 2  Stars and the Sun . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 3  
9.2. 3  The Sola r Syst e m . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 4  
9.2. 4  Size and Scal e . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 5  
9.2. 5  Day and Nigh t . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 6  
9.2. 6  Moon Phas e s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 6  
9.2. 7  Satel l i t e / P a r a b o l i c Dish . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 7  
9.3 How Learning Occurs ....................................................................................... 258 
9.3. 1  Cogn i t i v e Lear n i n g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 5 8  
9.3. 2  Affec t i v e and Conat i v e Lear n i n g . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 6 2  
9.4 Methodological Findings .................................................................................. 265 
9.4. 1  Issue s in Inter v i e w s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 6 5  
9.4. 2  Person a l Meanin g Mappin g as a tec hn i q u e for data coll e c t i o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 6 9  
9.5 Misconceptions ................................................................................................. 271 
9.6 Science Literacy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 7 5 
9.7 Role of Vocabulary ........................................................................................... 275 
9.8 Critical Reflection of the Research Process ..................................................... 276 
9.9 Conclusi o n s and recomme n d a t i o n s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 7 7 
9.10 Future Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 8 0 
9.11 Endpiece ........................................................................................................... 281 
References .................................................................................................................. 283  
10  Appendices................................................................................................................ 297  
 
 ii
 Abstract 
A l l studen t s are able to learn some th i n g about  astron o my when they partic i p a t e in a school 
visit to a site of informal learni ng such as a scienc e centr e . I examin e d how childr e n from 
four schools experienced presentations and part ici p a t e d in activi t i e s about astronomy during a 
two to four hour visit to either the Hart eb e e s t h o e k Radio Astron o my Observ a t o r y or the 
Johannesburg Planetarium in South Africa. Th e case study involv e d obser vi n g thirty- f o u r 12- 
to 14-year - o l d studen t s at the science centre and interv i e w i n g them about astron o my concep t s 
includ i n g those based on person a l meanin g ma ps th ey drew prior to and after their visit . The 
data were analyse d using a human constru c t i v i s t framewor k to determin e both what and how 
studen t s learnt during their visit.  
Despite a lack of teacher involv e me n t  I show how student s collectively and 
individ u a l l y learnt about con cep t s in astrono my , which I categor ised into a set of seven Big 
Ideas: gravity, stars and the S un, size and scale, the Solar System, day and night, Moon 
phases and parabolic dishes. Co llect i v e l y , there was an improve me n t in their knowled g e of 
Big Ideas dealt with at the study sites , inclu d i n g gravit y , stars, the Sun, size and scale, and 
parabol i c dishes . The student s s howed little change in their know led g e of day and night or the 
phases of the Moon. Indivi d u a l l y , all student s l earn t princ i p a l l y by increme n t a l addit i o n of 
knowle d g e , while some studen t s also de mons t r a t e d greater knowled g e restruc t u r i n g . Student s 
with the least prior knowledge added additional basi c facts to their repe rt oi r e , while those 
with greater prior knowle d g e were able to re organise their knowledge and achieve greater 
understanding. All students also  showed that the affective domain (for exampl e enjoyme n t 
and wonder) contribu t e d to their learning by encouraging interest  in astronomy . Some 
students demonstr a t e d examples of conative learning in whic h their experi e n c e s prompt e d 
them to furth e r actio n after their visit . While the visi t chang e d the misco n c e p t i o n s of some 
students, it made little difference to others , and promote d miscon c e p t i o n s in a few. 
Methodo l o g i c a l findin g s includ e d the value of using perso n a l meani n g maps, the impor t a n ce 
of using mo dels during the in terview process and observations of how students used language 
in their descrip t i o n of astrono mi c a l process e s . 
The study suggest s that student s learn best from a range of  activities clustered around 
a central theme, and that enj oyable activities appear to enha nce learnin g . I recomme n d that 
the astrono m y present e d at the centres focus on a limit e d numbe r of conce p t s in astro n o my , 
and that presentations and activities be stru ctured around those Big Ideas. Science centres 
 iii
 shou l d provid e teache r s with guidel i n e s for thei r visit. I also propos e that activi t i e s aim to 
recall student s ? prior knowled g e and provide situatio n a l interest  to encou r a g e motiv a t i o n in 
the topic of astrono my and the subject of scie nce . Finall y I suggest that science centre s 
should combin e cognit i v e learni n g with affective fun, as r ecomme n d e d by students 
parti c i p at i n g in the study. 
Keywords 
A s t r o n o my , Astron o m y Educat i o n , Constr u c t i v i s m, Huma n Cons t r u c t i v i s m, Infor ma l 
Learnin g , Museum, Planeta r i u m, Science Centre. 
 
 ix
 List of Appendices 
Appendix A ..................................................................................................................... .. 298 
Appendix B ..................................................................................................................... ... 299 
Appendix C ..................................................................................................................... ... 302 
Appendix D ..................................................................................................................... .. 305 
Appendix E ..................................................................................................................... ... 308 
Appendix F ..................................................................................................................... ... 309 
Appendix G ..................................................................................................................... .. 311 
Appendix H ..................................................................................................................... .. 312 
Appendix I ..................................................................................................................... .... 313 
Appendix J ..................................................................................................................... .... 314 
Appendix K ..................................................................................................................... .. 323 
 x
 List of Figures 
F i g u r e 1.1 Current foci of research into visitor studies (Rennie, 2001) ............................. 5 
Figure 2.1 The Contextual Model of Learning (Falk and Dierking 2000) ....................... 38 
Figure 2.2 Concept Map of Human Construc t i v i s m (aft e r Mintz e s and Wander s e e 
1998) ............................................................................................................... 42 
Figure 3.1 A Personal Meaning Ma p constructed by Bhekiwe........................................ 56 
Figure 3.2 Example of initial PMM drawn on chalkboa r d . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 8 
Figure 3.3 The sequence of even ts in data collecti o n . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 7 
Figure 4.1 The Turntable .................................................................................................. 80 
Figure 4.2 Moon Phases ................................................................................................... 82 
Figure 4.3 Projection of the Sun ...................................................................................... 83 
Figure 4.4 Sundial ............................................................................................................ 8 4 
Figure 4.5 Whisper Dish and Telescope .......................................................................... 85 
Figure 4.6 Launchin g Rockets . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 7 
Figure 4.7 Coke cans and gravity ..................................................................................... 88 
Figure 4.8 Cos mic Pinball . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 9 
Figure 4.9 Taking the Solar System for a walk: the Sun .... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 0 
Figure 4.10 The Second South African in Space ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 2 
Figure 4.11 The radio tele scope at HartRAO ..................................................................... 94 
Figure 4.12 Zeiss star project or in the planetarium .......................................................... 100 
Figure 5.1 Student preparat i on for the visit (n=34) ........................................................ 136 
Figure 5.2 Student anticip a t i o n of visit.  Some student s gave more than one purpose , 
so the totals do not add to 100% ................................................................... 137 
Figure 5.3 Students? conceptions of gravit y (Pre-visit: n=28; post-visit: n=27) ............ 140 
Figure 5.4 Students? ideas of gravity on Jupiter (n=24) .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4 2 
Figure 5.5 Students? ideas of gravity on the Moon (n=25) ............................................ 143 
Figure 5.6 Students ? concep ti o n s of stars (n=34) .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4 4 
Figure 5.7 Student s ? concep t i o ns of the Sun (n=34) ...................................................... 145 
Figure 5.8 Students? conceptions of the Solar System (n=8) ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 4 7 
Figure 5.9 Student s ? concep t i o ns of size and scale ........................................................ 148 
Figure 5.10 Student s ? knowled g e of the si ze of the Sun and Moon (n=34 pre-visit, 
n=29 post-visit) ............................................................................................. 150 
Figure 5.11 Students ? concepti o n s of th e cause of day and night (n=34) .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 1 
Figure 5.12 Student s ? explana t ion of Moon phases (Pre-visit  n=34; post-visit n=19) .... 156 
Figure 5.13 Students ? concepti o n s of parabol ic/satellite dishes (n=26; only HartRAO 
students) ........................................................................................................ 157 
 xi
 Figure 5.14 Summary of pe rcenta g e changes in levels for 5 Big Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 8 
Figure 5.15 Summar y of percen t a g e change s in levels for signif i c a n t ideas and 
domi nant artefact . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 5 8 
Figure 6.1 Part of Brenda?s post- visit Personal Meaning Map ...................................... 164 
Figure 7.1 Scatter g r a m of student s ? pre- and post-vi s i t mean scores . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 1 
Figure 7.2 Graph showin g studen t s acro ss the range of learnin g about whom 
portrait s are written. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 3 
Figure 7.3 Nonkulul e k o ? s pre-vi si t Personal Meaning Map ..... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 9 0 
Figure 7.4 Nonkululeko?s post-visit PMM .................................................................... 192 
Figure 7.5 Botho?s pre-visit PMM ................................................................................. 197 
Figure 7.6 Botho?s post-vis i t PMM ..... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 9 9 
Figure 7.7 Neo?s PMM (pre and post) ........................................................................... 204 
Figure 7.8 John?s pre-visit PMM ................................................................................... 214 
Figure 7.9 John?s post-visit PMM .................................................................................. 216 
Figure 8.1 Fatima?s PMM (pre- and post-vis i t ) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 6 
Figure 8.2 Brenda?s PMM (pre and post) ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 3 4 
Figure 8.3 Helen?s PMM (pre and post) ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 4 3 
 
 xii
 List of Tables 
T a b l e 1.1 Researc h avenues as propose d  by Rennie and colleag u e s (2003) that I 
used in my study. .............................................................................................. 3 
Table 1.2 School groups as a frac tion of total child visitors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4 
Table 2.1 List of peer-reviewed studies of  astrono my educat i o n resear c h since 1975.. . 1 6 
Table 2.2 Number s of articl e s relate d to planetaria in databases .................................... 23 
Table 2.3 Wellington?s (1990) cl assification of learning ............................................... 26 
Table 3.1 Key concept s in astronom y identi f i e d at each study site .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 0 
Table 3.2 Participant schools in the study ...................................................................... 66 
Table 3.3 Informed consent returns . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 8 
Table 4.1 Relation s h i p of data sources to  criteria for constructing narrative ................. 76 
Table 5.1 De mogra p h i c informa t i o n on sample of student s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 0 
Table 5.2 Big Ideas used in my study ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 1 3 
Table 5.3 Criter i a for assign i n g studen t s to knowle d g e catego r i e s for Big and 
Significant Ideas and the Domi nant Artefact . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 2 4 
Table 5.4 Response s to Question B1 and B2: Where are you going/di d you go on 
the forthcoming visit? What subject a nd topi c area is/wa s the visit relat ed 
to? ................................................................................................................. 134 
Table 5.5 Purpose of visit: Response s to Questio n B3: What do you think is/was 
the purpose of the visit? .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 3 5 
Table 6.1 Knowledge Construction categor i e s defined in my study .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 6 2 
Table 7.1 Categor i s a t i o n scheme for stude nts (n=34) based on their pre- and post-
 vis i t knowledg e of Big Ideas concepts . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 4 
Table 7.2 Catego r i s a t i o n scheme showin g studen t number s from each school . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 5 
Table 7.3 Summar y of increa s e in astr onomy - r e l a t e d vocabula r y shown by students 
(n=34) ........................................................................................................... 186 
Table 7.4 Mean number of astron o m y - r e l a t e d words used by student s in the pre- 
visit and addit i o na l words used in post- v i s i t PMMs categ or i s e d by Big 
Ideas scores .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 8 7 
Table 7.5 Student s select e d for whom portra i t s were writte n, using the 
catego r i s a t i o n scheme of studen t s developed in Chapter 6. ......................... 187 
Table 7.6 Position of Nonkululeko on Big Ideas classification table ........................... 188 
Table 7.7 Nonkulul e k o ? s know ledge of Big Ideas........................................................ 188 
Table 7.8 Frequency of Human Constr u c t i v i s m c odes for Nonkulu l e k o . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 9 4 
Table 7.9 Position of Botho on Bi g Ideas classification table ...................................... 195 
Table 7.10 Botho?s knowledg e of Big Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 9 5 
Table 7.11 Frequency of Human Co nstruc t i v i s m codes for Botho ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 0 1 
 xiii
 Tabl e 7.12 Positio n of Neo on Big Ideas classi f i c a t i o n tabl e .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 0 2 
Table 7.13 Neo?s knowledg e of Big Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 0 2 
Table 7.14 Frequency of Human Co nstructivism codes for Neo ................................... 210 
Table 7.15 Positio n of John on Big Ideas classif i c a t i o n table .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 1 1 
Table 7.16 John?s knowledg e of Big Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 1 1 
Table 7.17 Frequency of Human Co nstructivism codes for John ................................... 220 
Table 8.1 Positi o n of Fatima on Bi g Ideas classification table ..................................... 223 
Table 8.2 Fatima? s knowled g e of Big Ideas .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 3 
Table 8.3 Ways in which Fatima ch anged her ideas about concept s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 7 
Table 8.4 Frequency of Human Co nstru c t i v i s m codes for Fatima . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 2 9 
Table 8.5 Position of Brenda on Bi g Ideas classification table .................................... 231 
Table 8.6 Brenda?s knowledge of Big Ideas ................................................................. 232 
Table 8.7 Ways in which Brenda ch anged her ideas about concept s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 3 5 
Table 8.8 Frequency of Human Co nstruc t i v i s m codes for Brenda .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 3 9 
Table 8.9 Position of Helen on Bi g Ideas classification table ...................................... 240 
Table 8.10 Helen?s knowledg e of Big Ideas ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 4 0 
Table 8.11 Frequency of Human Co nstruc t i v i s m codes for Helen ... . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 4 7 
Table 9.1 Summary of cogniti v e learning by case study students ................................ 258 
Table 9.2 Summary of affectiv e and c onative learning by case study students . . . . . . . . . . . 2 6 3 
Table 9.3 Activit i e s at HartRAO which resulted in affective learning ......................... 263 
Table 9.4 Presen t a t i o n s at th e planet a r i u m whi c h result e d  in affective learning.......... 264 
 
 xiv
 Abbreviations 
A S Achie v e me n t Schoo l 
BFS Balfou r Forest School 
BS Boka mos o School 
CCM Conceptu a l Change Model 
CML Context u a l Model of Learnin g 
GET Genera l Educat i o n and Traini n g 
HartRA O Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Observ a t o r y  
HC Human Const r u ct i vi s m 
HSS Human and Social Scienc e 
HU Herme n e u t i c Unit 
IK Indigeno u s Knowledg e 
LGS Lourde s Girls School 
MLC Museum Learning Collaborative 
NARS T National Association for Resear c h in Scienc e Teach i n g 
NASA Nation a l Aeron a u t i c s and Space Admi ni s t r a t i o n 
NRF Nation a l Resear c h Founda t i o n 
NS Natur a l Scien c e 
PCK Pedagogi c a l Content Knowledg e 
PMM Persona l Meanin g Map 
RNCS Revised Nation a l Curric u l u m State me n t 
SALT South African Large Telesco p e 
SD Standar d Deviati o n 
 
 
1 
Chapter 1 
1  Introduction to the study 
This chapter provides an overview of the study, why it was conducted, how the 
research questions are framed conceptually, the researcher?s positionality and 
the sequence of the chapters. 
1.1  Introduction 
A s t r o n o my can be regard e d as a relati v e l y familiar disc ipli n e for students and their 
teache r s , as it crossc u t s the subjec t s of sc ienc e , techno l o g y , geogra p h y and histor y and can 
be used as a unifyin g theme for project work in schools. For some phenome n a , such as day 
and night, star constel l a t i o n s , the seasons and the phases of the moon, direct observ a t i o n is 
simple and practic a l . Howeve r , what makes the study of astronomy more challenging are 
the non-i nt ui t i ve , relat i ve l y compl e x and abstr a c t explan a t i o n s needed to elucida t e the 
observat i o n s . A further attracti o n of astron o my as a discip l i n e is the fact that it asks some 
of the ?big quest i o n s ? in sc ienc e and philos o p h y , such as ?Are we alone? ? and ?Where do 
we come from? ? While the aski n g of such que sti o n s is someti me s encou r a g e d by more 
innovat i v e teacher s , astrono my is a topic which lends itself to practical and out-of-school 
approa c h e s where childr e n can experie n c e the st udy of the univers e or the force of gravity 
in a more authe n t i c conte x t than that of the classr o o m. This thesis expl or e s what process e s 
of learni n g take place when a school stude n t goes outsi d e the classr o om and experi e n ce s 
the prese n t at i o n of astrono my conce p t s in an infor ma l setti ng . What does she learn? How 
does he learn about it? How does their pr ior knowled g e influen c e their learnin g ? 
1.2  Background and Rationale for the Study 
M a n y childr e n ? s natur a l inter e s t in space and the stars has been identi f i e d as a way of 
encoura g i n g them into the science s (e.g. Jarma n & McAlee s e , 1996) . Howev e r , 
parti c i p at i o n and achie ve me n t in scien c e at secon d a r y school level in South e r n Africa are 
not as high as educati o n author i t i e s , employ e r s  and insti t u t i ons would like (Depa rt me n t of 
Educat i o n , 2000). As the histor y of scien ce educatio n research shows, understa n d i n g 
learning and developing better peda gogy to assist in the learning of the sciences has been a 
major ai m of scien c e educa t i o ni s t s over the past  twent y - fi v e years . As I demon s t r a t e in the 
2 
liter a t u r e revie w in Chapt e r 2, most resea r c h in astronom y educat i o n has examine d 
studen t s ? concep t i o n s and misconc e p t i o n s . The a im of the empiric a l study describ e d in this 
thesi s is to under s t a n d how child r e n learn , not in  the class r o om , but in the infor ma l setti ngs 
of a planet a r i u m and the visito r s ? centre of an obser v a t or y . The resear ch falls withi n the 
general frame wo r k s of visito r studie s and learnin g at muse u ms and scien c e centr e s, which 
have developed over the past thirty years. Resear c h into this type of learn i n g is parti c ul a rl y 
important in South Africa for a number of reas ons . First, very little  resear c h has been 
carried out in South Afr i ca on visitor studies. Secondly, if scien c e and te chnolo g y are to 
develo p in South Africa as envisa g e d by th e govern me n t (Depa r t me n t of Arts Cultur e 
Science and Technology, 1996), then both the Public Awarene s s of Science and the 
attrac t i o n of larger number s of school child ren into science-related subjects are high 
prioriti e s for research . Thirdl y, scholars such as Gardner (1993) suggest that present day 
for mal schoo l i n g has become less signif i c a n t for the majority of young people. Although 
this may not be as true in South Afric a as it is in the USA, the kind of infor ma l learnin g 
that takes place in a planet a r i u m, museu m or scien c e centr e is impor t a nt to study preci s e l y 
becau s e such insti t u t i o ns can provi de motiv a t i o n for stude n t s and can engag e them in the 
kind of ?big ideas? in sc ie n c e refe r r ed to in Chapt e r 5. Inte re st in lear n i n g in scie n c e cent r e s 
is gainin g a higher profi l e in South Afric a , as the numbe r of scien c e centr e s incre a s e s , and 
both the govern me n t and donors questi o n the imp act that such scienc e centre s can make. 
In Southern Africa, astronomy  is import a n t for other reas ons. Due to the fact that 
we are situa t e d in the south e r n hemi s p h e r e , have wide sky coverag e , lie longitu d i n a l l y 
close to the Green w i c h merid i a n , have rela tively sophisticated communication facilities, 
and have relativ e l y low levels of light polluti on, we are positioned fa vourably to contribute 
to the science of astrono my on a global scale (Square Kilometre Array, nd). The 
establ i s h me n t of the South Afric a n Large Tele scope (SALT) at Suther l an d in the North e rn 
Cape Province and the current bid for the Squar e Kilome t r e Array (a massi v e radio 
telescope) are both examples of  recent initia t i v e s by governme n t and the private sector 
which highli g h t the country ? s commit me n t to astron o my . Politi c a l commit me n t has been 
forth c o mi n g too; in his speec h openi n g parliame n t in 2004, Presiden t Thabo Mbeki 
referred to the developme n t of Southern Africa as a global hub for astronomy, space 
science and technology, while  the Minister of Science and Technology has mentioned 
impor t a n t devel o p me n t s in space scien c e and astron o my in his budget speec h in 2006 
(Depar t me n t of Scienc e and Techno l o g y , 2006). Together, these activities and 
3 
underta k i n g s reflect a growing profile of as trono my within South Africa which provide s 
furth e r motiv a t i o n for condu c t i n g re sear c h into astrono my educat i o n . 
Over the past decade , museu m pract i t i o ne r s and schola r s have attempt e d to define 
an agenda for resear c h at si tes of infor ma l learnin g . Leona Schauble and colleagues at the 
Museum Learni n g Collab o r a t i v e (MLC) have pr opose d a sociocu l t u r a l agenda (Schau b l e , 
Leinhardt & Martin, 1997) for ?g ener a l ? , non science - s p e c i f i c sites, while others have 
suggest e d a science - s p e c i f i c frame wo r k (Marti n , 2001, 2004). Howeve r , a numb er of 
informa l learni n g resear c h e r s have propos e d a wider agenda based on the Nation a l 
Assoc i a t i o n for Resear c h in Scienc e Teach i n g (NARST ) Policy Stateme n t on Informa l 
Scien c e Educa t i o n (Renn i e , Feher , Dierk i n g & Falk, 2003). This wider agenda embrace s 
other theor e t i c a l frame w o r k s for resear c h in additio n to the so ci oc u l t ur a l appr o a c h 
espoused by the MLC. Rennie and colleagues id entify six avenues for future research, and 
when plannin g and conduct i n g my researc h I br oadl y aligne d my object i v e s with three of 
the avenue s propo s e d in the Rennie agend a to ensure that my study was both relevan t and 
current . Table 1.1 shows the three avenues to gether with the respective NARST policy 
state me n t s and how I addres s e d the issue in my own study. 
Table 1.1 Research avenues as proposed by Rennie and colleagues (2003) that I used 
in my study. 
Research avenue NARST policy statement My study 
E x a mi n i n g the prec u r s o r s to 
the actua l engag e men t in 
learn i n g . 
Out-o f - s c h o o l learn i n g is 
self- mo t i v a t e d , volunt a r y , 
and guide d by learn e r s ? need s 
and intere s t s , so certai n 
aspect s of lea rni n g are 
critic a l to invest i g a t e . 
I exami n e d how stud e n t s ? 
inter e s t s , prior knowl e d g e 
and belief s affect e d the 
learni n g exper i e n c e at the 
science centre ? see Chapt ers 
7 and 8. 
Inves t i g a t i n g the proce s s of 
learn i n g . 
Learni n g is both a proces s 
and a produ c t so we need to 
investi g a t e both the process e s 
of learnin g as well as the 
produ c t s . 
I exa mi n e d what studen t s 
learn t about astro n o my (see 
Chapter 5) as well as how 
they learn e d (see Chapt e r s 6 
to 8) 
Expan d i ng the varie t y of 
metho d s used to carry out our 
resea rch. 
Informa l lear ni n g requir e s 
multi p l e creat i v e metho d s for 
asses s i n g it. ?Inno v a t i v e 
resea r c h desi g n s , metho d s , 
and analy se s are critica l . 
I used Perso n a l Meani n g 
Mapping as one resear c h 
tool, analy se d my data 
quali t a t i v e ly using the 
softw a r e prog r a m ATLA S . t i 
and used narra t i v e s and 
portra i t s as way s of sharin g 
my find i n g s . 
4 
Most of the curre n t resea r c h into museu m and science centre learning takes place in 
develop e d countr i e s , where intere s t has focuse d on genera l visito r s , family - g r o u p learni n g , 
and to a lesser extent school visits (Fal k & Dierking , 2000). Table 1.2 shows approxima t e 
percentages of child visitors in the for m  of school groups from muse ums and scienc e 
centres in Europe, the USA and New Zealand co mpa r e d with three sites in South Afric a , 
two of which were the locati ons for my study. The table show s that the proportions of 
childr e n visiti n g in school groups in South Africa are mu ch higher than overseas. I 
theref o r e consid e r e d that sc hool groups, which would includ e a broad spect r u m of childr e n 
represe n t a t i v e of the demogra p h i c s of the populat i o n of Sout h Africa , would be the most 
appropri a t e sector in wh ich to target my study. 
Table 1.2 School groups as a fraction of total child visitors 
Location Percentage of 
total children 
in organised 
school groups 
Source 
N a t u r a l Histo r y Museum, 
Lond on , UK. 
10 (Hawk e y , 2001 ) 
Jodre l l Bank Visit o r s ? Centr e , 
UK. 
23 (Cha p l i n , 1997 ) 
Observa t o r y Science Centre, 
UK 
33 (J. Harris , pers. co mm. ) 
Expl o r a t o r i um, San Franc i s c o , 
USA 
<25 (Exp l o r a t o r i um, 2006 ) 
Stard o m e Obser v a t o r y , NZ 50 (A. Bucki n g ha m, pers. co mm. ) 
SciBo n o Disco v e ry Centr e , 
Johan n e s b u r g, SA 
75 (SciB o n o stati s t i c s 2005) 
Johan n e s b u r g Plane t a r i u m, SA 75 (Joha n n e s b u r g Plane t a r i u m stati s t i c s 2000-
 2 0 0 4 ) 
Harte b e e s t h o e k Radio 
Astro n o my Obser v a t o r y , SA  
89 (Hart R A O stati s t i c s 2002- 20 0 4 ) 
 
1.3  Research Problem and Research Questions 
A t school level, basic astron o my has traditi o n a l l y been taught as part of geograph y in the 
old curric u l u m (prio r to 1990), and later as pa rt of Human and Social Scienc e . With the 
advent of the new curricu l u m introd u c e d in  1997 and its subsequent  revision as the 
Revised Nation a l Curric u l u m Stateme n t (Depar t me n t of Educati o n , 2002) astrono my was 
transf e r r e d to the learni ng area of Natura l Sc ienc e , and given a more promin e n t place in the 
5 
curri c u l u m as part of the ?O ur Place in Space? in the ?P lanet Earth and Beyond? knowledg e 
strand . With the change in curric u l um being such  a recent devel o p me n t , there is little or no 
publish e d resear c h concer n i n g how astrono my is being curren t l y taught and learnt in South 
Afric a n schoo l s . When teachi n g astro n o my , some teache r s have  opted to run a school field 
trip to a scien c e cent r e whic h cove r s the topi c , either as a support or a repl acement for their 
own teachi ng . When develo p i n g my propos a l for my doctor a l resear c h I was intere s t e d in 
how both studen t s and their te ach e r s gaine d from the exper i e nc e of visit i n g a scienc e 
centre relat ed to the topic of astronomy. I developed research questions to investigate the 
exten t to which learn i n g takes place at such centre s , and how what is taught relate s to the 
school curric u l u m. As the resear c h progre s s e d , I realise d that most teache r s who arrang e d a 
visit for their stude nt s did not th emse l v e s partic i p a t e to any gr eat extent in the teachi n g and 
learni n g proces s either before , during or after th e visit, and I decided therefore to focus on 
learnin g by the student s onl y (see Section 3.4.2).  
Scien c e centr e s are a speci a l i s e d form of museu m in which inter a c t i v e displ a y s are 
arrang e d themat i c a l l y (McMan u s , 1992), a nd fall under the broad area of muse um or 
visitor studie s . Within this field, my area of  study is visitor learni n g , as shown in Figure 
1.1 
 
Figure 1.1 Current foci of research into visitor studies (Rennie, 2001) 
Visitor Studies
 Behaviour 
during the visit 
Visitor-related  
(what they do) 
Exhibit-related 
(how they 
interact) 
Social 
Interaction 
Physical 
Environment
 Visitor 
Learning 
Exhibit 
Appraisal 
Profile of  
Visitors 
Impact on the 
environment 
6 
The learni n g sites that I chose were the Johann e s b u r g Plane t a r i u m on the campus of the 
University of the Witwatersrand, and the Hartebeesthoek Radio As tro n o my Obser v a t o r y 
some 50km West of Pret ori a , South Africa .  
My final researc h questio n s were defined as follows : 
1.  To what extent do students learn in the pr oce s s of a visit to a plane t a r i u m or the 
visit or s ? centr e of an astro n o mi c a l obser v a t or y?  
2.  H o w is the content of astronom y communi c a t e d to student s ?  
3.  W h a t are stude n t s ? individ u a l experiences of the visit? 
4.  H o w do students construc t knowledg e during and after the visit?  
5.  H o w do student s ? interes t s and prior knowle dge affect the learning experience of a 
school visit?  
1.4  Conceptual Framework 
T h e study is informed by the notion of inform al learning as involving an out-of-school, 
unstru c t u r e d , volunt a r y activi t y (Crane , 1994). It  is further informed by John Falk and 
Lynn Dierkin g ? s Context u a l Model of Learni n g (Falk & Dierkin g , 2000) that identif i e s the 
personal, sociocultural and physic al contexts as significant features of the environment 
which intera c t to infl ue n c e lear n i n g in museu ms . I also use the conce p t of human 
constru c t i v i s m (Mintze s , Wanders e e & Novak, 1997) as a theoret i c a l constr u c t in which to 
embed the resear c h and to expl a in how childr e n lear n at sites of infor ma l lear ni ng. 
Althou g h based on constr u c t i v i s m, I adopt a ?m odes t realis t ? stance (Osbor n e 1996), which 
I elabor a t e on in sectio n s 1.5 and 2.2. Modest  realis m combin e s the advant a g e s of a 
constr u c t i v i s t theore t i c a l fra mew o r k in the area of studen t learn i n g , while at the same time 
recogn i s i n g that there is an ont ological reality, which has been  establis h e d by scientists by 
repeti t i v e and routin e confi r ma t i o n . It is this  realit y that studen t s are learning about in 
scien c e , assis t e d by their visit to the scien c e centr e . 
In Chapt e r 2 I show that despi t e the cons id e r a b l e resear c h into astron o my educat i o n 
and informa l learning over the past thirty years, no studies combine the two fields, and my 
use of a human const r u c t i vi s t theore t i ca l framew o r k focusi n g on student learni n g makes 
my study both unique and an importa n t  contrib u t i o n to the literat u r e . 
1.5  The Researcher and Positionality 
T h e research I conducte d falls w ithin a qualitative, interpretivi st paradigm (see Chapter 3). 
Accordi n g to qualita t i v e resear c h e r s such as Tobin ( 2000) and Henning (2004), it is 
7 
impor t a n t in inter p r e t i v e resea r c h for the rese a r c h e r to provi d e a clear  accou n t of who he or 
she is, and how he or she has influe n c e d the results by engaging in the research process. 
Positi o n a l i t y is defined as the ?knower ? s spec if i c positi o n in any context as defined by 
race, gender , class, and other social l y signi f i c a n t dime n s i o n s ? (Mahe r & Tetre a ul t , 1994 p. 
22). The followi n g paragr a p h s portra y me as th e researc h e r , and attempt to show how my 
posit i o na l i t y may have inter a c t e d  with the research conducte d . 
I am a white male living in Johannes burg, South Africa. Born in the United 
Kingdo m, I have always been intere s t e d in  amateur astronom y , and, along with many of 
my gener a t i o n , the Apoll o missi ons fired my imagi n a t i on regar di n g space trave l and 
researc h . Howeve r , my main interes t as a youth was a passion for ornithology. Together 
with two friends , I would spend hours visitin g si tes of ornith o l o g i c a l in terest, recording the 
number and types of birds seen, and we w ould compare notes with other enthusiasts. 
Although an obsession during my teenage years, by the time I went to the University of 
Durham, I had decided to pursue Geology as a degree. One of our courses was in lunar 
geology, and it was very exciting to see the recen t l y  acquired moon rocks with which some 
of the professo r s were working. After l eaving Durham I worked as a ?conser v a t i o n 
geolog i s t ? for six mont hs before leavin g for Nepal, where I studie d Himal a y a n Pheas a n t s 
as part of a conser v a t i o n proje c t there . I wrote up the results of this study as a Masters 
thesi s when I return e d to the UK and subse q u e nt l y comple t e d a teach er training course. I 
taught in a Sixth Form Colleg e in Essex for th ree years before applyi ng for a job teaching 
in Botswana.  
I lived in Botswa n a for 10 years, marrie d there and worked in the area of scienc e 
teach e r educa t i o n in a rural colle ge of edu ca t i o n . I moved to Johan n e s b u r g in 1995, a year 
after the end of apart h e i d , and took a job at the Unive r si t y of the Witwate r s r a n d runnin g 
teacher upgradi n g program m e s . With an edu catio n grounded in the scienc es and teaching 
in the disciplines of geology, biology, envi ronmental science and science teaching 
method o l o g y , my outlook has been strongl y in fluenced by positivis t scien c e views . 
Howeve r , since teachi ng in a univer s i t y I have  been able to interr o ga t e thes e to some 
exten t , as my teach i ng style has alway s b een broad l y const r u c t i vi s t , attempt i n g to 
determi n e what prior knowle d g e studen t s po ssess and scaffolding their learning towards 
the goals of the course . My basic premis e is th at in scienc e there is a real world out there 
which can be investigated, so I would regard myself as a ?modest r ealist? (Osborne 1996). 
Whil e I accep t the relat i v i st posi t i o n of const r u c t i v i s m can assist studen t s in learnin g , 
8 
whereb y the teacher needs to work with learner s ? views, I would still ma in t a i n that some of 
those views might be misconc e p t i o n s , and part of the teach e r? s role is to move those 
concep t i o n s toward s more scienti f i c ones. How this is done is crucia l for the learne r , and 
some of the discu s si o n in this thesi s exami n e s ways in which miscon c e p t i o n s can be used 
posit i ve l y as a basis for a progr es s i o n towa rds a more scient i fi c unde r s t a n di ng of a 
concep t .  
My modes t reali s t stanc e means that I have some misgi vi n g s about embrac i n g 
views of indigenous knowledge (IK) and wo rld v i e w theory curren t l y popula r in the 
liter a t u r e . While I consi d e r it impor t a n t to value and acknowl e d g e indige n o u s knowle d g e 
as part of what has histori c a l l y been devel oped and used in South Africa, IK should not be 
confu s e d with scienc e , which has compl e t e l y different standards for veri fi cati on and 
theori s i n g . The values of openne s s and peer revi ew in science are very differen t from the 
often secre t tradi t i o na l l y- c o m mu n i c a t e d means by which IK is trans mi t t e d . For this reason 
I did not choose to investi g a t e studen t s ? cult ur a l or religio u s belief s about astron o my in 
depth, even though they may affect learni n g . I accept that my values are theref o r e very 
much consistent with a White Anglo Saxon Pr otest a n t upbring i n g , an d are not currently 
polit i c al l y corre ct . My value s colou r the interpr et a t i o n I bring to the data in the thesi s , as I 
presu ma b l y am tryin g to searc h for a ?trut h ? in the learni n g by studen t s visiti n g scienc e 
centre s . Howev e r , I feel it is impor t a n t to search for that truth using quali t a t i v e rathe r than 
quantit a t i v e approac h e s , as only the for mer are able to obtai n rich detai l e d data on 
stude n t s? ideas . I need to accept that when I intervi e w e d studen t s during data collec t i o n 
they would have seen me as an  author i t y figur e , not dissimi l a r to some of their teach e r s , 
and what they told me would have been in the light of their percepti o n of what I wanted to 
hear. Some of these issues might be threat s to quality and trustwor thiness of the findings, 
and it is impor t a nt for the reader to know these upfr o n t , so as to be able to better deter mi n e 
the qualit y of the thesis . 
Despi t e these const r ai n t s , I consi d e r that my resea r c h has been able to inves t i g a t e 
childr e n ? s thinki n g about astron o my and at least to some exten t  provide insights into what 
and how they learnt at a scienc e centr e . 
1.6  Structure of the Thesis 
M y thesis consis t s of nine chapte r s , starti n g with this introdu c t i o n to the research. In 
Chapt e r 2 I select i v el y revie w the relev a n t li tera t u r e of astron o my educat i o n and informa l 
9 
learni n g which form the backgr o u n d to the study. Acknow l e d g i n g that much of the 
previo u s resear c h has lacked  a theore t i c a l basis for the claims made, I introduce the 
theore t i c a l frame w o r k of huma n const r u ct i vi s m as a perti ne n t concep t u a l basis for my 
study. Chapter 3 describe s my research me thodology, case study design, my research 
instru me n t s and how the data was collec t e d , as  well as addre s s i n g issue s of ethic s and 
rigour . Chapte r 4 is the fi rst of the ?finding ? s chapters , and co nsists of narratives describing 
students? visits to Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astron o my Obse rv a t o r y and the Johann e s b u r g 
Planetarium. This is followed by Chapter 5 wh ich summarises learni ng about Big Ideas in 
astronomy by the 34 students in my study. Chap ter 6 provides greater detail about how a 
Human Const r u ct i vi s t theor e t i c al frame w o r k is used to analyse learni n g by individ u a l 
students, while Chapters 7 and 8 provide seve n portraits of students who showed differing 
degree s of learnin g at the science centre s . Chapt e r 9 compl e t e s the thesi s by summa r i si n g 
the main finding s of the study, relati n g them to the literature and discussing their 
implic a t i o n s in the field of infor ma l learni n g . 
10 
Chapter 2 
In this chapter I review a selection of th e literature related to science education 
in general and astronomy education in par ticular, as well as the literature on 
informal learning pertinent to the study. In the light of this review I then 
present theoretical frameworks in th e form of human constructivism and 
conceptual change, which underpin the study. 
2 Literature Review 
2.1  Introduction 
A studen t who visits a scienc e centre su ch as the Johann e s b u r g Planet a r i u m or 
Hartebe e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Observatory (HartRAO) visito rs? centre par ticipates in 
an experience which has been influenced by a varied body of work. The major influence is 
proba b l y the popul a r artic l e s of the newsl e t t e r s  and journ a l s read by the pract i t i o ner s of 
museums and science centres. However, the acti viti e s that take place at astronomy science 
centres have their roots in the field of scie nce , science educati o n , astronomy education and 
the literatu r e of infor mal learning . 
This researc h study draws upon three fields of  researc h : the prolifi c litera t u r e on 
scienc e educa t i o n in genera l , the exten s i v e lite r at u r e over the past thirt y year s in the area of 
astron o my educat i o n and, most import a n t l y , the museum and infor ma l learnin g literat u r e . 
As the focus of my study is learning  in the scien c e centr e, the l iter a t ur e revi e w e d in this 
chapter concent r a t e s on article s , books and other resour c e s whic h highligh t learning . 
2.2 Research in Science Education 
S c h o l a r s of scienc e educat i o n resear c h cl aim that during the first half of the 20 th  centu r y, 
scien c e teach i n g as well as learni ng theor i e s  associ a t e d with scienc e educat i o n were 
predomi n a n t l y positi v i s t (Poole , 1995) and be havi o u r i s t (Duit & Treag us t , 1998) in nature. 
Over the past twenty years , there have been va rious attempt s to revi ew researc h in science 
educa t i o n and its develo p me n t , and to place the variou s theor e t i c a l orient a t i o n s into a 
fra mewor k . Eylon and Linn (1988) id ent i f i e d four resear c h posit i o ns : 
11 
? t h e ?deve l o pme n t a l persp e c t i ve ? which wa s heavi l y infl u e nc ed by the theor e t i c a l 
work of Jean Piage t , and encompa s s e s re sea r c h into how me ntal reasoning in 
scienc e develo p s over the sc hool- g o i n g years of a child; 
? t h e ?diffe r e n t i al perspe c t i v e ? which exam i n e d the relat i o n s hi p betwe e n inst r u ct i o n 
and learne r s ? indivi d u a l differ e n c e s in ability and aptitude, and how they can 
explai n conce p t u a l change ; 
? t h e ?conce p t learni n g ? perspe c t i v e , consis t i n g of the concep t i on/ mi s c o n c e p t i o n 1  and 
concept u a l change researc h of  the const r u ct i vi s t schoo l ; and 
? t h e ?probl e m- s o l v i n g ? perspe c t i v e which c overs researc h into the science problem-
 s o l v i n g proces s e s employ e d by learner s 
While the first two positi o ns have long hi stories, overlapping with much of the 
behavi o u ri s t work earlie r in the twenti e t h century , the latter two are based on studies 
conduc t e d since the 1960s. In a length y analys i s  and using repres e n t a t i v e studie s , Eylon 
and Linn compar e the perspe c t i v e s with each ot her , and propo s e an integ r a t e d perspe c t i ve 
on scienc e educa t i o n . They note that the larg e number of studies has provide d somewha t 
fragme n t e d knowle d g e of the scienc e learne r , an d that collab o r a t i o n acr oss the perspec t i v e s 
would provid e the more cohere n t pictur e that the field requir e s . 
Ten years after this review, Duit and Treagus t (1998) report that  there has been ?an 
encouraging tendency towards an ?inclusive ? view of science learning which brings 
togethe r approa c h e s of differe n t theore t i c a l orientations? (p.8). They conclude that 
conceptual change strategies based on social  const r u ct i vi s t views of learni n g may help in 
providing an inclusive frame wo r k for future resea r c h .  
While Eylon and Linn use a persp e c t i va l appr o a c h in their analy se s, Erick s o n 
(2000) takes a programma t i c approa c h which uses  Lakatos? analysis of  scientific progress 
as a lens to examine science education. Eric kson identifies three ?research programme s ? 
withi n scienc e resea r c h over the past 25 year s : the Piage t i a n , the const r u ct i v i s t and the 
phenomen o l o g i c a l programm e s . Erickson consid e r s that while the Piage t i a n progra m m e 
has had a major influen c e on science educati o n ov er the past 30 years , it peake d in the late 
1970s and is now waning, with a consider a b l e decline in the number of studies since the 
                                                 
 
1  In this thesis I will use the term ?misco n cep tio n? to cov er the variou s terms used by differen t auth ors, 
inclu d in g ?altern ativ e concep tio n s?, ?precon cep tio ns?, ?intuitive concep tions? and ?alternative frameworks?. 
12 
1980s. In contras t the constru c tivist research progr am me has become domi nan t in the 
stude n t scien c e learni n g liter a t ur e since the mid-19 8 0 s . Ericks o n ? s own resear c h fits within 
the phenom e n o l o g i c a l progr a m me , and he provid e s a useful summa r y of recent resea r c h in 
the area. Although Erickson?s approach is valuab le in that it provides an altern a t i v e view 
of science educati o n researc h , he provide s little evidence that the phenomenol ogical 
progra m me is likely to b ecome as domina n t as either of the other two.   
An exa min a t i o n of any of the major scienc e educa t i o n journ a l s demon s t r a t e s that 
the curren t predo mi n a n t theor y of pedago g y with in science education is constructivism. As 
a basis for pedagogy it has few rivals, despite possible contenders such as the history and 
philosophy of science school (Matthews, 2000). C onst r u c t i v i s m can be regarde d as having 
two major strand s , radica l constr u c t i v i s m and so cia l const r uct i v i s m. Radic a l const r u c t i v i s t s 
maint a i n a basic assumpt i o n ?that reali t y exis ts but cannot be known as a set of truths ? 
(Tobin , Tippin s & Gallar d , 1994 p. 47). Within the overall strand of radi cal construc t i v i s m 
are views of const r uc t i vi s m in which an indivi dual builds up his or her own view of the 
world from person a l experi e n c e . This ?perso n a l? constructivist school developed from the 
work of Piaget? s develop me n t a l theories and has given rise to a smal l numbe r of rela t e d 
views of learnin g .  
One of thes e is the human cons t r uc t i vi s t view of learnin g develo p e d by Joseph 
Novak and others (e.g. Mintze s et al., 1997; Novak & Gowin, 1984) , which incorporates 
both the theory of meanin g f u l learni n g advan c e d by Ausub e l and colla b or a t o rs in the late 
1970s, and views of constru c t i v ism that involve a radical restructuring of knowledge 
(Mintz e s & Wander s e e , 1998). In human constr u c t i v i s m, learning is regarded as being 
some t i me s slow and incre me n t a l , resul t i n g in a ?weak? form of restructuring of knowledge, 
while at other time s, particu l a r l y during ?aha ? experi e n c e s or ?insi g h t f u l mo ment s ? , it is 
rapid and involv e s a comple t e reorga n i s a t i o n of concepts alrea dy acquired (?strong? 
knowledge restructuring). This theory of know l e d g e acqui s i t i o n has impor t a n t impli c a t i o n s 
for my own study, and I use it as a basis for my theoret i c a l frame wo r k (see section 2.8.2). 
Learni n g in museums and scienc e centre s ha s the potentia l for both weak and strong 
knowledge reorganization, due to the way in which the knowled g e is present e d , both using 
innova t i v e presen t a t i o n method s (such as in a planeta r i u m dome) and as hands-o n , 
inter a c t i v e exhib i t s . It is impor t a n t to note th at the human const r u ct i vi st s are reali st s , at 
least to the exten t that Osbor n e (1996 ) defin e s the ?modes t reali s t ? posit i o n . In his critiq ue 
Osborne shows how the principal strength of co nst r u ct i vi s m is in its use as a pedagogical 
13 
method , taking into account learne r s ? prior knowledge and treating learn e r s as activ e 
particip a n t s in the cons truction of knowl e dge. Osborne maintains however, that 
const r u ct i vi sm fails to provi d e any mecha n i s m or proce sse s to assi s t learne r s to judge 
whether one conception or theory is bett er than another, and move beyond a common-
 sen s e view (whic h in the case of  much of scien c e is not the scientifically-accepted  
viewpoi n t ) . Osborn e sugges t s th ere is a clear role for ?t elling, showing and seeing as 
methods which enable the constru c t i o n of new knowledge? (Osborne, 1996 p. 66). He 
advocate s that science educator s and resear c h e r s adopt a ?modes t reali s t ? epist e mol o g y , 
which accept s that there is an ontolog i c a l reality which scienti s ts investigate. A version of 
that reali t y can be const r u ct e d by stude n t s bo th from their own exper i enc e s of the world 
and from for ma l (and infor ma l ) instr u c t i o n . 
In contr as t , ?soc i al ? cons t r u ct i vi s t s ha ve used the work of psychologists Lev 
Vygots k y and Jerome Bruner to show that knowle d g e is built up by socia l inter a c t i o n 
between individu a l s . Social construc t i v i s t s ar gue that knowled g e is created and shaped by 
human discour s e , and that science educati o n is a process of en cultu r a t i o n whereb y 
?novic e s are introdu c e d to a communi t y of know ledg e through discours e in the context of 
releva n t tasks ? (Driv e r , Asoko , Leach , Morti m e r & Scott, 1994 p. 9). Recen t devel o p me n t s 
relate d to the social constr u c t i v i s t paradigm are the notions of situated cognition, border 
crossin g and world views.  
Situat e d learn i n g or cognit i o n (Brow n , Colli n s & Duguid , 1989; Lave & Wenger , 
1991) refers to knowledge being shared or distributed across physical, social and 
psychological contexts. Lave and Wenger have  examine d how learner s can act like 
apprentices, participating and increasingly taking on critical tasks of their master s in 
authenti c situatio n s . Aspects of situated lear ning are relevant for my study, in that when 
visiting Hartebeesthoek Radio Astrono my Obs e rva t o r y , student s are seeing an authent i c 
site of science investigation. However, for vis itor s to become apprent i c e s in the sense used 
by Lave and Wenger, they would need a mo re prolonge d engageme n t with the science 
centr e and assoc i a t e d obser v a t or y than a single visit . 
Aikenh e a d and his collab o r a t o r s (Aik enhe a d , 1996, 2000; Aikenhea d & Jegede, 
1999) take a differ e n t appro a c h to constr u c t i v i s m: they postulat e that learning science 
involves students crossing cultur a l borders from their own subc ulture to that  of (what is 
curre nt l y predo mi n a n t l y Weste r n ) scien ce . They postu l at e that the cross i n g of these 
borde r s can be smoot h or hazar d o u s . Stude n t s in  the latter categ o r y a void assimi l a t i o n into 
14 
the ?scho ol scien ce ? subcu l t ur e , and either fail or pass scien ce cours e s without true 
compre h e n s i o n of the scienc e involv e d . Agai n , the concep t of border cross i n g has some 
relevan c e to my study: when student s visit either  of the study sites , th ey could be regarde d 
as crossin g a border between their own subcult u r e  and that of ?Western Modern Science?.  
The world view hypothes i s (Cobern, 1991, 1996 ) and its Africa n varian t (Jeged e , 
1995; Jegede & Okebuko l a , 1991; Ogunnyi , 1996) suggest that concepts covered in the 
scienc e class r o o m will be assimi l a t e d into a learne r ? s belief system only if it corresp o n d s 
with his or her world view. Althoug h this idea mi ght be appea l i n g in a study such as mine, 
which incl ude s the influ e n c e of stude n t s ? be liefs during the learni n g exper i e nc e , a small 
number of research e r s (e.g. Dzama & Osbor ne, 1999; Rollnick, 2000) have questioned the 
world view hypothe s i s . In a quantit a t i v e st udy, Dza ma and Osborne de monst r a t e that 
tradi t i o na l belie f s and attit u d e s accou n t for only a small propor t i o n of the varian c e in 
achiev e me n t in a test on electr i c ity. They suggest that other f actors such as the ?absence of 
suppor t i v e enviro n me n t for seriou s scienc e learning? (Dzama & Osborne, 1999 p. 401) are 
more likely to explai n succe s s in scienc e , or  lack of it. Marissa Rollnick (2000) suggests 
that situat e d cognit i o n (which at the time ha d gaine d grea t e r acce p t a n c e in the mathe ma t i c s 
educa t i o n resea r c h commu n i t y ) might provi d e a ?more coherent view of culture? (p. 99) 
whereb y the scienc e learne r become s a partic i pa n t in the socia l pract i c e of scien c e . There 
are impli c a t i o n s here for my study : stude n t s visit i n g a scienc e centr e , espec i a l l y an 
authen t i c conte x t such as a radio teles c o p e , may gain entry to the commu n i t y of scien c e 
throug h such a visit. Howeve r , as in the case  of Lave?s apprenti c e s h i p model, for such 
entry to be gaine d it is likel y that ther e needs to be a sust ai ne d serie s  of visits over a period 
of time. My study did not examine such a situa ti o n , as in South Afri ca school visits to 
scienc e centre s norma l l y take place only once in a year, due to financ i a l and curric u l ar 
constra i n t s . 
Cliff Malcolm and Busi Alant provide a deta iled, if somewha t one-sided, review of 
scien c e and mathe ma t i c s educa t i on a l resea r c h  in South Africa since the early- ni n e t i e s 
(Mal c ol m & Alant , 2004 ) . Writ i n g from a cr itical paradigm and despite providin g the 
reader with many examples, they disapprove of much of the Southern African science 
educat i o n resear c h into learni n g and teachi n g ov er the past 15 years. They regard mu ch of 
it as invol v i n g what they term ?mec h a n i s t i c ? and ?technicist? cognitive approaches, and are 
more comp l i me n t a r y of the mathe ma t i c s educ a t i o n rese ar c h . The latter , they claim, 
involves mo re appropriate expl or a t o r y method o l o g i e s to inves ti g a t e the complex i t y of the 
15 
social and politi c a l enviro n me n t . Malcol m and Alant ma int a i n , using examp l e s from 
langu a g e , Afric a n worldvi e w theor y , teach e r ed ucat i o n and policy studie s , that there is a 
need for emphasi s on critica l partic i p a t o r y stud ie s which addres s more closely the context 
of South Afr i can society.  
Broadly, my study is grounded in the theory  of constructivism, and would likely be 
rega r de d by Malc ol m and Alan t as a ?mech a n i s t i c  cogni t i ve ? appro a c h , as it invol v e s trying 
to determi n e what and how childre n learn over a relatively brie f time period. I would 
howeve r , defend my approac h as being approp r i a t e for the context of my study into how 
indivi d u a l s learn during a scien ce centr e visit . Exten s i ve re se a r c h has demons t r a t e d that, as 
a theory of learni n g constr u c t i v i s m can explain how knowled g e is acquire d . In my study I 
use human constructivism as descri b e d by Mintze s and Wa nder s e e (1998) and Mintze s et 
al . (1997), and as used by Anderso n (1999) a nd Anderso n , Lucas, & Ginns (2003) in their 
study of learnin g in a science centre in Austra lia. In addition, in order to encompa ss the 
affect i v e aspect s of learni n g scienc e , whic h are not as clear l y iden t i f i e d in human 
const r u ct i vi sm , I use a variat i o n of the Conce p t u a l Change Model (CCM) , origi na l l y 
develo p e d by Strike and Posner (1985) . Alsop & Watts (1997) have taken David 
Treagu s t ? s framew o r k of the CCM (Tyson, Venville, Harrison & Treagust, 1997), and 
argue that infor mal learning co ntexts should take into accoun t issues of affect, conation 
and self-es t e e m, and I have adopted their s uggesti o n s . Both the hum an constructivist and 
conce p t u al chang e model posit i o ns are addr es s e d in more detai l in the theor et i c al 
fra mework in sections 2.8.2 and 2.8.3. 
This first part of my review has examin e d resea r c h into scien c e educa t i o n in 
genera l and has identi f i e d how theory can in form the analys i s for my own study. As my 
study investi g a t e s learnin g about astrono my , I believ e it import a n t to consid e r how 
research into the learning of astronom y has been conduc t e d during the 20th Centur y and in 
the early years of the new millenn i u m. Sec tion 2.3 examine s pertine n t researc h into 
astro n o my educa t i o n . 
2.3  Astronomy Education 
A s a prefac e to this sectio n , I provid e a summar y of the peer-r e vi e w e d produ c t s of 
empir i c a l resea r c h into lear ning about astronom y over the past 30 years (Tabl e 2.1). I 
chose to focus on peer-reviewed articles for the summa r y as they are likel y to demon s t r at e 
a greate r degre e of rigou r than other produ c t s such as conferen c e papers. However, Dunlop 
16 
(2000 ) provi d e s a useful revie w of additi o na l articl e s in confere n c e procee d i n g s and other 
journa l s . The majorit y of studies have been in  topic s relat e d to th e Earth-Sun-Moon syste m 
such as the cause of day and night and the s easons (Table 2.1). Othe r popular topics have 
been the Earth and the Moon phases while gravit y and the remain i n g topics have been the 
subject of fe wer studies. Although I have include d nine gravity st udi e s in the table, mo st of 
them were in the subjec t of classr o o m/ u n i v e r s i t y physi c s exper i me n t s ,  and not related to 
astronomy. As Bailey and Slater (2003) noted in  their review of as tronomy education, the 
topics of current astron o m y , such as stars and the universe, have hardly featured as subjects 
of study for educat i o n a l resear c h e r s , even t houg h teachi n g these topi c s has been espou s e d 
by astron o me r s the ms e l v e s (e.g. Pasach o f f , 2001, 2002), althou g h not all astrono me r s 
agree (Sadle r , 2001; 2002). 
Table 2.1 List of peer-reviewed studies of astronomy education research since 1975 
Astronomy Topic  Total number of studies 
D a y and night 16 
Seaso n s 15 
The Earth 13 
Earth - S u n - Mo o n sy st e m 13 
Phase s of the moon 13 
Gravi t y 9 
Stars and Sun 4 
Solar sy stem 3 
Attit u d e s 3 
World v i e w s 3 
Other 3 
Size and scal e 1 
Tota l 96 
Jean Piage t made the first schol a r l y studi e s of how people, particu l a r l y childr e n , conce i v e 
of astron o m i c a l phenome n a in the 1920s. In his two books The Child?s Conception of the 
World a n d  The Child?s Conception of Physical Causality  he describ e s childre n ? s ideas 
about a flat Earth and the cause  of day and night, and refers  to previous psycholo g i s t s? 
work on simila r concep t i o n s (Piage t , 1929, 1930). Piaget identi f i e s three stages of 
17 
development of conceptions regarding the or igin of the sun and moon. The first stage 
regards them as being made artificially by Go d or humans, the second stage ascribes them 
as being partl y natur a l , while in the third st age they are regarde d as having nothin g to do 
with human activ i t y . Piage t ? s work was both pioneering and very va lua b l e in prese n t i n g 
childr e n ? s mental constr u c t s . Howeve r , subse quent research has shown that children?s 
conceptions are dependent not only on age (i.e. cognitive devel opment), but also on 
context and content (Bliss, 1995), and Piaget himself made no cl aims in using his research 
to assist in student learning. 
Wall (1973) summari s e s researc h related to  astron o my educat i o n in the USA over 
the period 1922 to 1972. He revi ewed 58 studies, of which 54 were doctoral and master?s 
studi e s , and only 13 were publi s h e d in the rese arc h liter a t ur e. Writ t e n from the viewp o i n t 
of astronomy as an earth scien ce, Wall produces a rather pede stri a n list of reports grouped 
accor di n g to eleme n t a r y , high schoo l and colle ge level. The studies consist of experim e n t a l 
proje c t s which compa r e diffe r e n t metho d s of in stru c t i o n or teachi n g resour c e s , as well as 
surveys and develop me n t of curricu l a for as trono my teachi n g . The final two decades of 
Wall?s revi e w was a great age of space explor a t i o n , and space scienc e featu r e s heavi l y in 
the studies describ e d . Histori c a l l y  the article is a useful reco rd of research conducted prior 
to and during the era of the space race, a nd Wall concludes with a list of eight 
recom me n d a t i o n s for furth e r resear ch which refle c t s the prior i t i e s of the time, such as ?the 
effect i v e n e s s of audio- v i s u a l materi a l s on stud ent achieveme nt? and ?the effects of student 
variabl e s (such as sex, IQ, ?etc.) on studen t achiev e me n t ? (Wall, 1973 p. 665). 
Since Piaget ? s studie s , much of the res earc h in astrono my educati o n has mirrore d 
the changes that have taken place in scienc e educati o n general l y over the past four 
decad e s , summa r i s e d in secti o n 2.2. In the 1970s  resea r c h e r s such as Nussba u m and Novak 
(1976) , Nussba u m (1979) and Mali and Howe (1979) worked from what Eylon and Linn 
(1988) would ident i f y as the ?devel o p me n t a l ?  perspe c t i v e . Using clinic a l interv i e w 
techni q ue s , they identi f i e d Piaget i a n constr u c ts held by children re garding the Earth in 
space.  
By the late 1980s the constr u c t i v i s t pr ogramme in science education was well 
establi s h e d , and astrono m y e ducat i o n studie s focuse d on misconc e p t i o n s and concept u a l 
change . Nume ro u s studie s of this nature we re conduct e d through o u t the last two decades of 
the twenti eth centur y , both in develo p e d a nd develop i n g country co nt e xt s . These inclu d e d 
those of Gunston e and White (1981), Klei n (1982), Baxter (1989; 1991), Vosniadou and 
18 
associates (Vosniadou, 1991; Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992, 1994), Sharp (1996), Fleer 
(1997), Kikas (1998a; 1998b) and Sadler (1998). Examini n g differe n t concept i o n s held by 
children over varying ages and across differen t cu lture s , these studies form part of the vast 
number of miscon c e p t i o n studie s carrie d out within scien c e educa t i o n durin g this period . 
Like those summari s e d in Table 2.1, most st udies concentrated on the relatively accessible 
concep t s relate d to the Earth-S u n - M o o n system , such as the Earth in space, phase s of the 
moon, ideas about day and night , and ideas about the seasons . Simila r resear c h projec t s 
have been conduc t e d with pre-se r vi c e and in -se r v i c e teache r s , princ i p a l l y in the UK and 
the USA (e.g. Atwood & Atwood, 1996; Barb a & Rubba, 1992; Summers & Mant, 1995). 
Such studies show that the majority of teach ers, particul a r l y at the primary level, hold 
misco n c e p t i o n s simil a r to those of the stude n t s they teach . All these studi e s could be 
classifi e d as belongin g to the ?concep t - l e a r n i n g ? pers pec t i v e (Eylon & Linn, 1988). 
Two papers provide very detaile d account s of pre-ser v i c e primar y teache r traine e s ? 
concept i o n s . Parker and Heywood (1998) examin e pre- and in-ser v i c e primar y teache r 
trainees? conceptions of night and day, the seasons and the moon. Wh at makes this paper 
differe n t from others I have cite d are the impl i c at i o n s the auth or s ident i fy for the key 
featu r e s of the learn i n g proce s s and the tr ainees ? Pedagogi c a l Content Knowledg e (PCK). 
PCK (as discuss e d by Shulma n , 1986) refers to the abili t y of teach e r s to repre s e n t ideas 
being taught in a way that makes them unders ta n d a b l e by students . It is particu l a r l y 
impor t a n t for teache r s to be able to expla i n diffi c u l t three - d i me n s i o n a l conce p t s such as 
those involving the Earth-Sun-Moon sy ste m. Anothe r key paper is by Trundle et al. 
(2002) , who exami n e Ameri c a n pre-s e r vi c e pr imar y school teache r s ? concep t i o ns of the 
phases of the moon before and af ter instru c t i o n . They cite pr evio u s studi e s carri e d out in 
the USA, most of which involv e multip l e - c h o i c e items in the metho d o l o g y , but thems e l v e s 
use inter vi ew s (toge t her with the manip u l a t i o n of model s ) to deter mi n e stude n t s ? 
concepti o n s . They de mo nstr a t e that instruct i o n resul t s in stude n t s being much more likel y 
to hold a scientifi c concepti o n on the cause of the moon phas e s.  
The plethor a of studies on misconc e p t i o n s has contin u e d into the 21 st  century, even 
though there is now a substa n t i a l body of research which shows that both students and 
teache r s hold belief s and miscon c e p t i o n s which ar e resi st a n t to change . Intere st i n g studie s 
include those of Dunlop (2000), Trumpe r (2001a; 2001b), Comi ns (2001), Roald and 
Mikalse n (2000; 2001) Dove (2002) and Agan  (2004) . Other s , such as Diakid o y and 
19 
Kende o u (2001 ) , use instr u me n t s with su ch ambigu o u s mul tip l e choice answer s 2  that any 
claims regard i n g post-i n s t r u c t i o n le arni n g are diffic u l t to evalua t e . 
Althou g h my study did not set out to examin e miscon c e p t i o n s , the data I collec t e d 
did demon s t r at e that sever a l of the misco n c e p t i o n s ident i fi e d in the liter at u r e hold for 
South African students . This in itself is a fi nding , as very little researc h has been carried 
out on this topic in Southern Africa. A typi c al misco n c e pt i o n held by the 12 to 15-year-old 
studen t s in my study relate s to  their unders t a n d i n g of gravit y. While 79% of students were 
able to give a parti a l scien t i f i c al l y accep t a bl e defini t i o n of gravity , a third of the student s 
speci fi c al l y state d prior to their visi t to the st udy site, that there is ?no gravity in space? 
(Lellio t t , Rollnic k & Pendleb u r y , 2006). This mi sconc e p t i o n has been identif i e d by Borun 
et al. (1993 ) in the USA, Bar et al. (1997) in Israe l and Sharm a et al. (2004) in Austr a l i a . It 
is likel y to be widel y preva l e n t , give n the fr equency of i ma ges of astronauts ?floating? in 
freefa l l on the Intern a t i o n a l Space Statio n or space shutt l e . Simil a r l y , 5 studen t s out of 34 
(15%) though t that the phases of the Moon ar e caused by either th e Earth?s shadow or 
someth i n g such as anothe r planet comi ng betwee n the Earth and the Moon. Again, these 
are common misconc e p t i o n s found in the li terat u r e (e.g. Engestr ? m, 1991; Trundle et al ., 
2002), showing that my own researc h mirror s some of the findin g s from elsewh e r e . 
Comins has established a website (www.p h y s i c s . u ma i n e . e d u / n c o mi n s / mi s c o n . h t m) listin g 
the astrono m y miscon c e p t i o n s identi f i e d in his book (Comi ns , 2001), a nd regularly updates 
it. Baile y and Slate r (2003 ) note that sever al of the misconc e p t i o n s re ferred to by Comi ns 
are not true miscon c e p t i o n s , but merel y factu al error s which ?m ight simp ly be correc t a b l e 
with traditi o n a l lecture - b a s e d method s ? (Baile y & Slater, 2003 section 2.6). This may be 
true for some of the fact ua l error s I identi f y in my own researc h , which is why I am more 
inter e s t e d in how students learn about astronomy rather than what they learn . For examp l e 
in Chapter 7 I show that two students learnt abou t sunspot s ; Nonkulu l e k o just reme mb e r e d 
that there are black spots on the Sun which sh e did not discus s furthe r . Neo develo p e d an 
understanding, albeit flawed, about the spot s, their relati v e size and their relati v e 
tempe r a t u r e . These issue s are disc uss e d further in Chapter s 7 and 9. 
In the late 1990s Albanese , Neves and Vicen tini (1997) carried out  a critical review 
of several interna t i o n a l l y publish e d researc h  papers over the peri od 1976 to 1994 on the 
                                                 
 
2  e.g. Questio n 1 fro m their questio nn air e: ?Wh at do you  think the earth look s lik e? (a) a squ are tray; (b) a 
roun d tray; (c) a fish bowl; (d) a bask etb all; (e) a roun d lo af of bread ?. Optio n s (c), (d) or (e) cou ld all be 
regar d ed ?corr ec t ? to some degr e e . 
20 
Earth and its place in the univer s e . Approa c h i n g the field from a ?histo r y of scienc e? 
viewpo i n t , they conclu d e that while the resear c h is valid regardi n g childre n ? s concept i o n s 
on the shape of the Earth , the resea r c h- q u e s t i o n i n g used did not relate the children ? s 
empir i c a l obser v a t i o n s to abst ract models of the Earth-Sun system. They are partic u l a r l y 
critical of Baxter (1989), Klein (1982) and Vosniad o u and Brewer (1994), stating  
Apart from the obvious conclu s i o n that  childre n may learn an abstrac t 
model withou t questi o n i n g the re asons of its validity, no other 
infor ma t i o n may be derive d from the resear c h , or implic a t i o n s drawn for 
didact i c practi c e (Albane s e et al ., 1997 p. 586). 
Althou g h Albane s e and colleag u e s are harsh in their criticism, a simila r argume n t could be 
put forwar d for much of the altern a t i v e con cepti o n research in astronom y educatio n to the 
presen t date. Out of the numero u s paper s on ch ildr e n ? s altern a t i v e conc ep t i o n s cited here, 
less than a quarte r theori s e how the unders t a n d i n g of the altern a t i v e concep t i o n s can be 
used to promot e impro v e d learn i n g . This is  importa n t for my study, as it indicat e s the 
relativ e l y atheor e t i c a l approa c h  that has been taken to astr onomy learni n g , and the need for 
theory to infor m learni n g , as I attempt to show in Chapters  6 to 9. Other authors (e.g. 
Malcol m & Alant, 2004) have said much the sa me for the alter n a t i v e concept i o n researc h 
which has been so preva l e n t in the scien c e educat i o n litera t u re ove r the past 25 years. 
A study in a simila r contex t to mine was by Falc?o and collea g u e s , in a Brazil i a n 
astro n o my museu m (Fa l c ? o  et al., 2004). Focusing on four exhib it s depict i n g astron o mi c a l 
cycles and using a modelling approach, the researchers found that different types of 
teachi n g mo del s were of value in enabli n g visit or s to unders t a n d such cycles as day/ni g h t 
and seasons. An interesting and relevant study which includ e d some fieldwo r k in South 
Africa was a teachi n g interv e n t i o n conduct e d by Schur (1999) . Using an experimental 
resear c h design , this study combin e d a cons tr u c t i v i s t and mediat e d learni n g experi e n c e 
appro a c h . A teachi n g packa g e , the Exper i me n t a l Astrono my Curri c u l u m, consi s t e d of 
engag i n g stude n t s in a ?thin k i n g journ e y to the moon? in order to enab le 14- to 15-yea r - o l d 
Israel i studen t s to change their concep t i o n of the Earth. Althou g h this study examin e d the 
effect of a teachi n g interv e n t i o n over an ex tende d perio d of time, its releva n c e for my 
study is that it identifi e s st udents? understandings of grav ity, one of the foci of my 
exami n a t i o n of stude n t s ? learn i n g . Recen t paper s  on astro n om y educa t i on have appea r e d as 
conferen c e presenta t i o n s in Southern Af rica (e.g. Clerk, 2006; Mosolo a n e & Stanto n , 
2005), and it is likely that this is a gr owth area for resear ch in the region. 
21 
A small number of studie s have examin e d  indige n o u s knowle d g e in relati o n to 
astrono mi c a l and everyda y phenome na (e.g. Ca meron, Rollnick & Doidge, 2005; Jegede & 
Okebuko l a , 1991; Lemmer, Le mmer & Smit, 2003;  Mohapatra, 1991). Such studies are 
conduc t e d partly from the point of view of scient i f i c miscon c e p t i o n s , but relate studen t 
understa n d i n g to culture, religi on and the worldview hypothesis. I decided not to take this 
route in my own study , as I found in my pilot que stio n n a i r e that 12- to 15-year - o l d student s 
in urban schoo l s did not tend to have stron g cultur a l tradi t i o ns assoc i a t e d with astro n om y . 
Some of the most intere s t i n g report s of  resear c h on learni n g about astro n o my do 
not appear in the ?traditional? science educ ation journals. A newl y established on-line 
journa l ?Astro n o my Educat i o n Re view? has begun to provide a va riety of papers within the 
field (e.g. Agan, 2004) as well as a very usef ul review of astron o my educat i o n resear c h 
over recent decade s (Baile y & Slater , 2003). Howe ver , from the point of view of relatin g 
astrono my learnin g to theory, th ere are two key studies which appear to have gone largely 
unnotic e d by researc h e r s . The first, by Enges t r ? m (1991 ) exami n e s a commo n 
misconce p t i o n prevalen t among bot h children and adults (and reporte d extens i v e l y in the 
literature) that the phases of the moon are cau sed by the Earth?s shadow. Engestr?m uses a 
notion of ?synth e t i c stupi d i t y ? devel o p e d by Wagenschein to account for this 
misconc e p t i o n . Wagens c h e i n sugges t s that as a result of misa pplied learning from either 
the classro o m or textboo k s , people do not relate  the model of the Earth- S u n - M o o n to the 
real moon they see in the sky. Engest r ? m s uggest s that the textboo k diagrams bear no 
relati o n s h i p to real scale, and are part of the cause of synth e t i c stupi d i t y . Such diagr a ms , as 
well as the comple t e lack of proble ma t i s a t i o n of moon phas e s versus lu nar eclipse s , are the 
principal culprits in the ?empty sentences? that people use when asked about moon phases 
(Enges t r ? m, 1991). Severa l other au thor s (e.g. Ojala, 1992; Trundl e et al ., 2002; 
Vosniado u , 1991) identify textbook s and their two-dime n s i o n a l diagrams as promotin g 
miscon c e p t i o n s in astron o my . Engest r ? m howe ve r , then uses activi t y theory to propos e an 
ambiti o u s new model for learni n g in school s . Build i n g on the Legi t i ma t e Periphe r al 
Partici p a t i o n theory of Lave and We nger (1991)  Enges t r ? m shows that in order to counter 
misconc e p t i o n s such as the moon phases, a co mplet e l y revise d approa c h to learnin g is 
necess a r y in school s . Engest r ? m? s work is re leva n t to my study in that the notion of 
?synt h et i c stupi d i t y ? was ident i fi ed durin g the analysis of some of  my data, and is 
discus s e d in Chapte r 9. 
22 
A second study, based in Sweden (S chou l t z , S?lj? & Wyndha mn, 2001) is 
impor t a n t as it quest i o n s the basis of one  of the princ i p a l metho d s used by many 
resear cher s in the fiel d: the inter view. Wh il e a large numb e r of studi e s in astro n o my 
education have been based on multiple-choice  tests and questionnair es (e.g. Dove, 2002; 
Dunlop, 2000; Trumper, 2001a), seve ra l studie s have used interv iews to elicit views from 
student s (e.g. Sharp, 1996; Vosniad o u & Br ewer, 1992, 1994). Schoultz and colleagues 
introd u c e d a globe into  the interview process, and f ound the respons e s of quite young 
children were very different from those obt ained by other research ers. Based on these 
responses, they argue that the presence of the globe allowed the subjec ts to conceptualise 
their understa n d i n g very differen t l y than if the prompt had not been present. Schoultz et al. 
go on to theor i s e that the inter v i ew is a s ituate d event, and the respons e s obtaine d by 
researchers such as Vosniadou and colleague s do not represent ch ildren?s underlying 
?menta l mo dels ? , but are merely reflec t i o n s of the natur e of the inter vi ew . Simil a r findi n g s 
(witho u t theori s i n g about the event) have been noted by Trundl e et al. (2002), and it 
appea r s that the avai l a b i l i t y  of 3-dime n s i o n a l model s has a significant effect on 
respon d e n t s ? thinki n g . While the socioc u l t u r a l approac h adopted by Schoult z and 
collea g u e s appea r s to cast doubt on many of  the findings of misconce p t i o n studies, 
Vosniad o u has sought to defend her cogniti v e positi o n . In a recent paper (Vosni a d o u, 
Skopel i t i & Ikospen k a t i , 2005) she replicates her previous work from the 1990s as well as 
that of Schoul t z and collea g u e s . Her findin g s sugges t that only the older childr e n (7- to 10-
 year-olds) can use the globe effectively, and th at somet i me s childr e n base thei r answe r s on 
the globe and sometime s on their prior knowledg e . Further, the children do not know when 
they are makin g incon s i s t e n t re sp o ns e s with respe c t to the gl obe and their prior knowle d g e . 
Vosnia d o u mainta i n s that a combin a t i o n of cognit i ve and socio- c u l t u r a l appr oa c h e s is 
import a n t in this type of resear c h , and that pur ely discursi v e analysis is not appropr i a t e . As 
I will attem p t to show in Chapt e r s 7 to 9,  my own resea rc h demon s t r a t e d that the 
introduc t i o n of a model had a strong effect on a studen t ? s abili t y to expla i n the cause of 
day and night. Like Vosnia d o u , I would argue that  a combi n a t i o n of appr oa c h e s is the most 
approp r i a t e way to explai n the intera c t i o n betwe e n inter v i ew e r and interv i e w e e when a 
model is introdu c e d into the discuss i o n . These recent studie s by Engestr? m , Schoultz and 
Vosniad o u would be classif i e d  as being part of the c onstru c t i v i s t pr ogramme of 
educational research (Erickson, 2000). I cons ider them key papers, as they provide 
impor t a n t insig h t s into the use of model s in  astron o my learni n g which many resear c h e r s 
23 
have decline d to use in their studies , basi ng their findin g s on ques t i o nna i r e s and writt e n 
tests inste a d . 
2.3.1  Research in Planetaria 
I n contrast to the substan t i a l volume of astrono m y studi e s , there is limit e d recen t 
liter a t u r e on resea r c h into learni n g at plane t a r i a . The modern planet arium was invented in 
1920, and the earlier studie s can be found in Wa ll? s revie w , but are di fficu l t to obtain. An 
accepte d techni q u e for determi n i n g the prevale n c e of resear c h in a partic ul a r field is to 
make search e s throug h databa s e s of abstra c t s (Lucas , 1991; White, 1997). A search for the 
terms ?plan e t a r i u m? ?astr o n o my? and ?educ a t i o n ? using key words from summari e s in the 
ERIC and the NASA As trophysics Data System (ADS) 3  databases is summarised in Table 
2.2. Unlike White?s method on science trends whic h present s the proport i o n s for the target 
word per 10,000 article s , Table 2.2 shows simple counts for the terms of interes t , by 
publica t i o n date, and grouped at 5-year interval s for more recent years.  
Table 2.2 Numbers of articles rela ted to planetaria in databases 
 Term: 
?planetarium  
and learning? 
ERIC 
Term: 
?planetarium and 
education? ERIC 
Term: 
?planetarium and 
education? ADS 
Term: 
?astronomy? 
and 
?education? 
ERIC 
Interval Number 
of 
abstracts 
% No. of 
abstracts 
% No. of 
abstracts 
% No. of 
abstracts 
P r e - 1 9 7 0 1 3 5 5 2 2 47 
1970 -7 9 17 47 47 51 5 5 >540 
198 0 -8 4 7 19 25 27 2 2 330 
198 5 -8 9 4 11 6 6.5 5 5 212 
199 0 -9 4 5 14 6  6.5 17 18 176 
199 5 -9 9 2 6 4 4 32 34.5 279 
200 0 -0 4 0 0 0 0 30 32.5 97 
Total 
abstracts 
36 100 93 100 93 100 1681+ 
 
                                                 
 
3  This cons ists of 3 biblio gr a ph ic datab a s e s cover ing astr ono my, astr o ph ysic s and phys ic s based at Harva r d. 
See http ://ad swww.h arvard . edu 
24 
Table 2.2 shows the relati v e l y limite d extent of the abstra c t s involv i n g planet a r i a in 
educational journals over the pa st 30 years. The ERIC databa se shows a peak in the 1970s 
and early 1980s, with only 10 to 20% of th e articles publishe d since 1990. The ADS 
databases show a different trend, with over 80% of planetariu m- r e l a t e d abstr a c t s 
?published? since 1990. However, it is importa nt to note that most of the planetarium-
 r e l at e d abstr a c t s cited in the ADS datab a s e s are in fact abstr a c t s from the Ameri c a n 
Astron o mi c a l Associ a t i o n meetin g s , and do not  get published. In contrast, the ERIC 
abstra c t s relate mainly to articl e s pub li s h e d in educa t i o n a l journal s , such as Journal of 
Research in Science Teaching , and Science Education , as well as books, conference papers 
and official documents. Also shown in Tabl e 2.2 is a summary of astronomy education-
 related abstracts for the same time period. Although it shows a peak in the 1970s, probably 
relat e d to the Apollo missi o n s , it does not show  the same drop- o ff in resea rc h that is 
indica t e d in the case of plan et a r i u m educa t i o n . This begs th e question : why has there been 
so littl e educa t i o na l resea r c h carried out related to  plane t a r i a since th e 1980s? I posed this 
questio n to an Interne t listse r v e of about 1,200 planetarium directors and educators in 2003 
and again in 2006. I had three repli e s in 2003 and seven in 2006. A gene ral concern raised 
by most respo n d e n t s was that in order to survi v e finan c i a l l y , plane t a r i a have to stress the 
entertainment aspects of their shows rather than educatio n . Nume r ous planetaria have 
closed over the past two decade s , and there is de bat e as to wheth e r this is due to the move 
towar d s enter t ai n me n t and the attem p t to co mpe t e with I-MAX and Disne y - s t yl e theme 
parks , or a genera l loss of publi c inter e s t . A very recen t examp l e is the Londo n 
Planetarium which closed its doors as a planet arium in May 2006: the owners blame a lack 
of inter e st by visit or s , while astro no me r s ident i fy the move towar d s enter t ai n me n t as the 
reason for its demi se , citing other successful planetaria run by astrono me r s elsewhe r e 
(Bale, 2006). 
Riorda n (1991) provid e s a useful review , which includ e s a brief histor y of the 
planet a r i u m as an educat i o n a l prese n t a t i o n and a summar y of  research over a period of 
about 30 years. In common with  research in science educat ion described in section 2.2, 
researc h into the effecti v e n e s s of the pl anetar i u m as a teaching tool was mainly 
quanti t a t i v e , using experi me n t a l and quasi - e x p e r i me n t a l method s , during the 1960s and 
1970s. By the 1980s, more participatory plan et a r i u m pr esen t a t i o n s  were gaining in 
popula r i t y , and althou g h some resear c h showed  no signifi c a n t differe n c e s in learnin g 
achie v e me n t , other studi e s demon s t r a t e d impr o v e d learni ng gains using a plane t a r i u m. 
25 
Riordan?s overall conclusion can be summe d up early in the re vie w , that ?the effec t i ve n e s s 
of the planeta r i u m as a teachin g / l e a r n i n g  device is yet undete rmined? (Riordan, 1991 
p.19). Much of the researc h pres ented in the review shows c ontradictory results, and the 
grea t majo r i t y focu s e s on the attai nme n t of behavi o u r a l object i v e s . Si nce Riordan?s review, 
occas i o na l studi e s repor t i n g th e use of planetaria have been publishe d (e.g. Rusk, 2003; 
Urke, 1993), but they do not re late directly to my study. Th ere have also been opinion 
pieces relati n g planet a r i a to museum resear c h (Parke r , 1995) as well as the entert a i n me n t -
 e d u c a t i o n debate (Brunell o , 1992). 
Although the field of astronomy education rese arch has advanced since the days of 
Piage t and Novak , I consid e r that it became some wh a t bogged down in miscon c e p t i o n 
resear c h by the 1990s, with many studie s publis h e d into the 21 st  centur y which have not 
added to our understa n d i n g of the nature of learnin g . There is little  doubt that if the classic 
documen t a r y ?A Private Univers e ? (Pyrami d  Film and Video 1988) was repeate d now, 
very simila r result s would be  obtai n e d . This short film  showed graduating Harvard 
stude n t s being quizz e d about astrono mi c a l ph enome n a such as the cause of the Moon 
phases and the season s . These highl y train e d gradu a t e s demon s t r at e very little knowl e d g e 
about common phenome na. Clearly, a different approach to learning about astronomy is 
require d ; it may be that learnin g outside the classro o m has an importa n t part to play, and 
sectio n 2.4 examin e s the role of informal learning in education. 
2.4  Informal Learning 
A s a mode of learn i n g , infor ma l educa t i o n is  usual l y contr a s t e d with the terms ?forma l ? 
and ?non formal?. According to Falk (Falk & Dierking, 2001) these terms, develo p e d by 
resear c h er s in inter n at i on a l devel o pme n t , go back nearly 50 years and were later used by 
the museum commun i t y to distin g u i s h betw een school-based learni ng and out-of-school 
activi t i e s . Coombs and Ahmed (1974) provid e a distinc t i o n between the three terms, as 
follo w s : 
Formal education : the highly institut i o n a l i sed, chronologically graded 
and hierar c hi c a l l y struct u r e d ?educa t i o n syste m? , spann i n g lower prima r y 
school and the upper reaches of the univers i t y ? . 
Non-formal education : any organiz e d , systema t i c , educati o n a l activit y 
carri e d on outsi d e the forma l syste m to  provide selected types of learning 
to particu l a r subgrou p s in the popul ation, adults as well as children. 
26 
Informal education : the lifelo n g proces s by wh ich every person acquires 
and accumul a t e s knowle d g e , skills , a ttitu d e s and insight s from daily 
experi e n c e s and exposur e to the envir o n me n t at home, at work, at play; 
from the exampl e and attitu d e s of the fa mily and friend s ; from travel , 
readin g newspa p e r s and books or by listeni n g to the radio or viewing 
films or televi s i o n .  
(Coombs & Ahmed , 1974 p. 8) 
The category of non-forma l edu cation, although relevant to the concept of learning in 
museu ms , seems to have falle n into disus e in  the resear c h litera t u r e , where the princi p a l 
distin c t i o n is now betwee n the terms for mal and informal . Wellingt o n (1990) distingu i s h e d 
betwe e n the informa l learn i n g of field trips and the for mal learning of school (Table 2.3). 
Table 2.3 Wellington?s (1990) cl assification of learning 
Informal learning ? field tr ips Formal learning - school 
V o l u n t a ry Co mpu l s o r y 
Unstr u c t u r e d Struc t u r e d 
Open- e n d e d Close - e n d e d 
Many unin t e nd e d outc o mes Fewer unint e n d e d outco m e s 
Non-asse ssed Asse ss ed 
Learner-led T e a c h e r - l e d 
Hofste i n and Rosenf e l d (1996) reject Wellin g t on ? s distin c t i o n as being too simpli s t i c and 
creatin g a binary approa c h which does not refl e c t the true situa t i o n of reali t y . Inste ad 
Hofste i n and Rosenf e l d prefer the ?hybri d ? ap proach of Crane (1994) which allows that 
certain ?forma l ? aspects of learning can take place under infor ma l condi t i o n s , but that 
informa l learni n g is essen t i a l l y an out-of - s c h o o l , unstr uctured, voluntary activity. 
Falk (2001) identi f i e s di ffic u l t i e s with the term informal when it is appli e d to the 
term learning . He mai nta i n s that ?there is no c onvin c i n g evide n c e that the fundame n t a l 
processes of learning differ solely as a func tion of the physical se tting? (2001 p. 7), and 
postu l a t e s that child r e n exper i e nc i n g a lectur e in a museu m audit o r i u m is no diffe r e n t from 
experie n c i n g the same lecture in a school audito r i u m. He makes a similar point about 
open- e n d e d disco v e r y learn i n g in  the same two settin g s , and identi f i e s the social contex t 
and a learner?s motivation as being more impo rtant. Falk and Dier king (2000) and Falk 
(2001) have therefo r e argued that the term free-choice learning  is more appropriate for 
museu m setti n g s . The chara ct e ri s t i c s of this t ype of learnin g have b een ident i f i e d as being 
non-seq u e n t i a l , self-pa c i n g , non-ass e s s e d , and of ten involvin g groups (Dierkin g & Martin, 
27 
1997). Althou g h there has been some consens us for the use of this term, partic i p a n t s at a 
confer e n c e on free-c h o i c e learni n g in 1998 were di vided as to whether it should replac e the 
more famil i a r and establ i s h e d term ?info r ma l learn i n g ? (Luke , Camp, Dierk i n g & Pearc e , 
2001). It is also intere s t i n g to note that recent  issues of science edu cation journals (e.g. the 
Journal of Research in Science Teaching  2003 and Science Education  2004) have devoted 
issue s to scien c e learni ng in informal environments. For the purposes of my study, I will 
use the two terms free-choice  and informal l ea r ni n g inte r c han g e a b l y , as the term info r ma l 
learni n g has a wide usage while the term free- choice is a useful expression which is likely 
to gain increa s i n g curr e nc y in the futur e . 
Most of the recen t resea r c h on infor ma l scien c e learni n g has been carri e d out in 
museum enviro n me n t s , with a lesser (but increa sing) amount at science centres. Relatively 
few studies have been execute d at either planet aria or observatory vi sit or s ? centr e s . At the 
latter sites, Dierking and Mar tin?s (1997) characteristics of informal learning are less in 
evidenc e . While non-sc h o o l vis its to a planeta r i u m are us ually social, volunta r y and non-
 asse s s e d , school visits are usually compulso r y  and may well be assessed in some ma nner. 
Furth e r , the visit s to such insti t u t i o ns are us ually structured, sequentia l and at least partly 
didact i c . Despit e these differ e n c e s , I would cont e n d that learni n g at planet a r i a and visito r s? 
centres can indeed be classif i e d  as infor ma l , on the basis that it is out-of- s c h o o l , and, that 
parts of the programme have some aspects of free-c h o i c e , of  being unstru c t u r e d , self-
 pa c e d , explora t o r y and non-seq u e n t i a l . 
Unlik e the astro n o my educa t i o n resea r c h de scribed in section 2.3, research into 
informa l learn i n g has not mirror e d that of sc ience education so closely. Although research 
until the 1970s was behav i o u r i s t in natur e (Hein, 1998; Rennie & McClaff e r t y , 1996), and 
has more recen t l y embra c e d const r u c t i v i s m, there has been much less empha s i s on 
misco n c e p t i o n resea r c h than in the broad e r science education community. Instead, we can 
ident i fy the follo w i n g key theme s withi n infor ma l learni n g rese a r c h:  
? E n t e r t a i n me n t & educat i o n 
? S c i e n c e commu n i c a t i o n and scienc e liter a c y 
? L e a r n i n g in museums and scienc e centr e s 
A fourth theme, which does not strictl y invol ve researc h , include s issues relevant to 
pract i t i o n e r s, such as evalu a t i o n and impac t studi e s . I now discu s s each of these theme s as 
28 
refl e c t e d in the litera t u re , with particul a r referenc e to learning ,  as it forms the key resear c h 
questio n in my study.  
2.5  Edutainment or Entercation? 
T h e lame n t of numer o u s museu m schol a r s ove r the decad e s has been wheth e r the purpo s e 
of museu ms and simil a r insti t u t i o n s shoul d be to amuse or educa t e the publi c (Falk & 
Dierking, 2000; Lucas, 1991; Shortland, 1987). In her excellent analys is of the changing 
museum, Li sa Roberts devote s a whole chapte r to  the subjec t , and argues that is not a new 
issue (Rober t s , 1997). In the early days of  Ameri c a n mu seu ms throu g h to the early 
twentie t h - c e n t u r y , the debate was around schol a r s h i p versus popula r i s a t i o n as mus eums 
sought to become both profess i o n a l l y and comm e r c i a l l y viabl e . As the twentieth-century 
progr e s se d , the issue was fairly clear - c u t , with  ?scho l a r l y ? curat o r s on one side arguing that 
muse u ms were prima r i l y impo r t a n t for thei r co lle ct i o n s and the rese arch that could be 
carrie d out on them, while ?prog r e s s i v e ? educa t o r s on the other side mainta i n e d that such 
collec t i o ns needed to be availa b l e for the upliftme nt and educati o n of the visitor s .  
However by the 1970s, with the introdu c t i o n of audio- v i s u a l displ a y s , compu t e r 
termi n a l s , theat r e , music and other featu r e s more associated with show business, the 
argume n t had shifte d to ?enter t a i n me n t versu s  educa t i o n? . Museu ms felt they were losin g 
audien c e s to the leisur e indust r y (such as th eme parks) and respon d e d by trying to provid e 
entert a i ni n g activ i t i es withi n the mu seu m to retai n their visit o r s . Museum ?puri s t s ? found 
this unacce pt a b l e ; the depth of feeling is shown by Willia m Fagel y in 1973: ?the curat o r 
shoul d not prosti t u t e his instit u t i o n by transf o r mi n g it into an amuse m e n t center full of ?fun 
house? gimmic k s in an attempt to win the inte rest of all potential visitors? (in Roberts, 
1997 p. 39). Interestingly, many educators in cre a s i n g l y found themse l v e s on the same 
?side ? as the curat or s , tryin g to promo t e serio u s learni ng in the face of ?friv o l o u s ? 
enter t a i n me n t .  
As the field of ?visitor studies? develope d over the 1970s, resear ch began to show 
that leisur e and play were motiv a t i ng facto r s for peopl e ? s visit s to muse u ms . Once at the 
museu m, learn i n g could still take place , even if learn i n g was not ?cogn i t i v e gain? , as it had 
tradi t i o na l l y been viewe d . Resea r c h into motiv at i o n and leisur e in museu m s has 
demons t r a t e d that they are often precu r s o r s or partne r s in learni n g (Csiks z e n t mi h a l y i & 
Hermans o n , 1995; Hood, 1980 in Roberts , 1997) . While some museu m educa t o r s have 
embra c e d the devel o p me n t of enter t a i n me n t activi t i e s in museu ms , other s have resis t e d the 
29 
change s . Robert s sugges t s th at ?the vote is st ill out, however , on whether these 
devel o p me n t s are for bette r or for worse ? (R obert s , 1997 p. 44). She not es that change s in 
?auth o ri t y? withi n museu ms , with a shift towa rd s educat o r s and the pub lic (and away fro m 
curat or s ) , as well as great e r accep t a n c e that exper i e nc e in museu ms is a wider notion than 
mere cognit i v e gain, reflec t broade r change s in societ y , at least in  developed countries. 
The critici s ms by some , regardi n g ?frivol i t y ?  and ?hands-on, minds-off?, have been 
espec i a l l y sever e in scien c e centr e s as they  have emerg e d over the last four decades as 
separa t e instit u t i o n s from scienc e museums . Science centres have grown exponentially, at 
the rate of thirty percen t per year, since th e opening of the Explorat o r i um in San Franci s c o 
in 1969 (Beetle s t o n e , Johnso n , Quin & White, 1998). Instead of disp la y i n g artef a c t s , 
science centres focus on hands-on exhibits, ofte n inter a ct i ve, which attem p t to engag e and 
educat e their visito r s (Renni e & McClaf f e r t y , 1996). Critici s ms involvi n g ?sloppy science ? 
(Champa g n e , 1975) , ?scie n c e as simply play  and innocen t enterta i n me n t ? (Shortl a n d , 1987 
p. 213) and ?an endles s series of unconn e c t e d , entertai n i n g magical events? (Parkyn, 1993 
p. 31) are just some of the dispara g i n g rema rks directed towards science centres. Although 
written as opinion pieces and not researc h - b a s ed, such critiques have had considerable 
influ e n c e over the years. As the body of resear c h in these insti t ut i o ns incre a se s , there is 
begin n i n g to be accept a n c e that both enter t a i nme n t and educa t i o n have a role to play in 
scienc e centre s and mus eums . Studie s such as  those of Boisve r t & Slez, (1994), Silverma n 
(1995), Falk and associat e s (Falk & Dier kin g , 2000; Falk, Mousso u r i & Coulson , 1998) 
and Gilbe r t (2001 ) demon s t r a t e th at for many visitors , their visi t to a scienc e centr e is to 
learn and also to have fun. In my own study I incl uded a strand of questioning regarding 
the views of the student s on learnin g and fun. As  will be demons t r a t e d in Chapte r 5, most 
student s regarde d learnin g and fun as natural be dfel l o w s when partic i pa t i n g in a visi t away 
from school . In an analys i s relati n g Dewey? s work to the museu m envir o n me n t , Ted 
Ansbach e r identif i e d no conflic t between an enj oyab l e and an educati v e experie n c e ; he did 
howeve r cautio n that ?an enjoya b l e experi e n c e can ?.. have negativ e outcome s and be a 
mis-ed u c a t i v e experi e nc e ? (Ansba c h e r , 1998a ; Dewey, 1938). In my study I found no 
evide n c e to suppo r t this claim, but the fact th at I worked with teenager s may mean that a 
school visit experie n c e preclud e s such conflic t s : every studen t interv i e wed enjoyed at least 
part of the visit, and each one of them learnt  at least someth i n g from their experi e n c e .  
30 
In the public arena, the debate betwee n en tert a i n me n t and educa t i o n contin u e s . In 
2003, I followe d a fascina t i n g discus s i o n thread on the ASTC listser v e 4 . Kurt Koll e r of the 
Pacifi c Scien c e Cente r in Seatt l e , quoti n g Michae l Crigh t o n?s views on socie t y? s ?nee d ? to 
be enterta i n e d , posed a questio n regardi n g th e relevanc e of scien c e centr e s in the new 
centur y : ?What are the core va lues of science / t e c h n o l o g y / ma t h e ma t i c s that we would want 
to deliver and how (well) do they interse c t with contem p o r a r y in terest s and cultural 
needs?? The resulting discussion over the next  few days demonst r a t e d  that scienc e centre 
practit i o n e r s have str ong feelings about how their instit u t i o ns compa r e with theme parks 
such as those of the Disney Corporation. Th e central argumen t of the discuss i o n was that 
while theme parks do entert a i n their visit or s very effecti v e l y , managin g to get them to 
?suspe n d disbel i e f ? as part of the experi e n c e , scienc e centre s have a differ e n t mi ssio n , 
much more concer ne d with scienc e aware n es s and science processe s . The followin g 
quotat i o n s demons t r a t e some of the tensi on s that science centre s grappl e with: 
I?ve notic e d that Disne y has become a so rt of icon for science center folk 
? for better or worse. ?..scie n c e cente r s and museu ms shoul d simpl y 
avoid comp ari s o n with Disney and its ilk. ?.We?re not about fairy tales, 
but we are about the amazing histor y of discover y , the uses of those 
discov e r i e s , and the possib i lities inherent in thos e discoveries. (Lisa Jo 
Rudy, www.lisarudy.com) 
As far as I am concerned , if people l eave our exhi b i t s with a thirst (or 
even an itch) to learn more about a subject , then we have provide d a 
valuab l e servic e (read: releva n c e ) . (Wesle y Creel, Pink Palace Family 
of Museu ms , Memph i s ) 
Say what you will about Disney they make money .    We at scien c e 
center s struggl e and strai n . ?.Bec a u s e the publi c has been condi t i o ne d to 
be enterta i n e d , it has now become th e expecta t i o n that everyth i n g be 
entertai n i n g . ?.If you don?t hop on the enterta i n me n t bandwag o n , you 
won?t stay open. But how wonderfu l that science centers have truly 
played a role in changi ng the way we teach and embrac e scien c e . (Marc i a 
Hale, Discovery Center of Idaho) 
The bad about Disne y : I think it  boils down to one ?word? ? 
eduta i n me n t . It assume s that anyt hing educational can?t possibly be 
enjoyab l e , and tries to steer us in to some horrib l e moras s . ? I would 
never say that science center s should n? t be entertai n i n g , only that we 
should never assume that the science we prese n t is someh o w defic i e n t in 
this area. (Jonah Cohen, Science Center of Connect i c u t ) 
                                                 
 
4  The Asso c ia tio n of Scien c e -T e c hn o lo g y Cente r s is a worldw id e networ k of scien c e museu ms and related 
institutions. Their emai l listse r ve can be acce ssed at www.isen-astc- l@home.ease.lsoft.com 
31 
It appear s that the ente rt a i n me n t vs. educat i o n debate is as  strong as ever. While the issues 
that face scienc e centr e s in South Afric a may not be quite the same as those in the USA, 
they are broad l y simil a r : fundi n g and compe t i t i o n from ?ente r t a i n me n t ? centr e s such as 
theme parks and shoppi n g malls. My own study does  not addres s this deba te in great detail, 
but as Falk and Dierking suggest, the rela tions h i p between educ ation and enjoyment 
appea r s to be becomi n g more compl e x as leisu r e and lifel o ng lear n i n g activi t i e s incre a s e . 
Although neither of these activities are as prevalent in S outh Africa as they are in the 
develop e d world, this is likely to be  a fruitful area for future research.  
2.6  Science communication and science literacy 
T h e r e is an increa s i ng l y exten s i v e litera t u re in a fiel d which combi n e s scien ce 
communic a t i o n and science liter a c y in relat i o n to museu ms and science centres, though it 
consists more of opinion pieces and conceptu al research than empi rical studies. Sc ience 
commun i c a t i o n is a relati v e l y new field whic h dissemi n a t e s the process e s and product s of 
scienc e to the genera l public . It include s popular science j ournals and books as well as 
websites (Gregory & Miller, 1998). Scientific (o r science ) litera c y , a lthough it has a longer 
histor y than scienc e co mmun i c a t i o n , is still a relativ e l y recent discipline. Shamos ( 1995) 
suggest s that the concept of sc ient i f i c liter a c y devel o p e d in  the 1950s as a rather vague 
term to promo t e a goal of ?scie n c e for effec t i v e citize n s h i p ? (p. 82). After the launch of 
Sputnik  by the Sovie t Union in 1957, a cleare r goal for scient i f i c liter a c y became the actua l 
understanding of science and technology, so  that the USA could imple me n t schoo l 
curric u l a in an attempt to catch  up and overtake the Soviet Union. 
Early attempts to define the field incl uded those of Conant (in Shamos, 1995) and 
Shen (1975). Shen make s a distinct i o n between pr acti c a l scient i f i c lite racy, civic scientific 
liter a c y and cultu r a l scie nti f i c literac y . Althoug h Shen has advocates from within the field 
of infor mal learning (e.g. Lucas, 1983; Re nnie, 2001), both Durant (1993) and Shamos 
(1995) are very clear that any definiti o n of scient i f i c litera c y n eeds to include the sense that 
some degree of the understanding  of scienc e shoul d be implic i t .  
Duran t ? s class i fi c at i o n of scien t i fi c liter ac y ident i fi e s three areas : 
? U n d e r s t a n d i n g as knowing a lot of science , 
? U n d e r s t a n d i n g as knowing how science works, and  
? U n d e r s t a n d i n g as knowing how science really works. 
32 
While the first two are regarded  as degree s of scie ntific literacy, Durant  suggests that the 
public needs ?a feel for the way th at the socia l syste m of scie n c e actua l l y works to deliv er 
what is usuall y reliab l e knowle d g e about the na tura l world? (Duran t in Gregor y & Miller , 
1998 p. 91). Shamos (1995) provid e s a simila r categori s a t i o n as Durant, identify i n g 
?dime n s i o n s ? of scien t i f i c liter a c y as level s  of understanding. Di mension one consists of 
cultural s c i e n t i f i c litera c y , simila r to the ?knowin g a lot of scie nce? of Durant, but merely 
at the level of vocabu l a r y and jargon . Di me ns i o n two is functional scient i fi c litera cy , in 
which the words and defi n i t i o ns of the first dime n s i o n can be used in a ?mean i n g fu l 
discourse? regarding, for example, science arti cles in the popular pr ess. Sha mos? highest 
level is ?true?  scient i fi c liter ac y , in which scienc e pr oce s s e s and theor y are combi n e d with 
the function a l dime nsio n to unde rstand the ?overall scientific enterprise?. Shamos suggests 
that this dimension is only achieved by  up to 5% of the population in the USA. 
Helpful as Duran t ? s and Shamo s ? clas s i fic a t i o ns of scien t i fi c liter a c y are, neith er of 
them is parti c u l ar l y appr o p r i at e for relat i n g to the outco me s of a visit to a scien ce centr e . 
The natur e of learn i n g in scien c e centr e s is  discuss e d in section 2.7, and althoug h there is 
conside r a b l e eviden c e that learnin g does o ccur (e.g. Anders o n , Lucas, Ginns & Dierkin g , 
2000) , Renni e (2001 ) sugge s t s that ?a deep cogni t i ve under st a n d i n g of scien c e conc e p t s is 
unlikel y to result from every scienc e centre visit? (p. 114). Both Lucas (1983) and, by 
impli c a t i o n , Renni e ident i fy Shen? s analy s i s of scientif i c literacy as being appropri a t e for 
museu ms . Shen (1975 ) ident i f i e s thr ee types of scient i f i c litera c y :  
? P r a c t i c a l scient i fi c literac y , in which a person uses their scie nt i f i c and techni c a l 
knowled g e to solve problems , 
? C i v i c scienti fi c liter a c y , in which a pers on?s knowledge of sc ienc e - r e l a t e d issues 
can be brought to bear on society - r e l a t e d issues, and  
? C u l t u r al scien t i fi c liter a cy , where b y the ach ie ve m e n t s of scienc e can be appre c i a t e d 
(and possibl y critici s e d ) . This is clearly not the same as Sha mos? version of cultura l 
scienti f i c literac y (know i n g a lot of science ) . 
Lucas consid e r s that while museums could po ssibly contribute to a ll three areas, typica l 
displa y s are most likely to develo p cultur a l sc ienti f i c liter ac y . It should be noted however , 
that Lucas conduc t e d no empiri cal work of his own, and hi s opinion piece has not brought 
about researc h in this area.  
33 
In my own study, studen t s were asked que sti o n s relate d to their knowle d g e of 
astron o my (see Chapte r 3 and Append i c e s B and C), as identif y i n g learnin g at the study 
site was a key resea rc h issue . While the major i ty of these questions could be regarded as 
being at Durant?s and Shamos? lowest level of scientif i c liter acy (knowing a lot of 
scien ce ) , some quest i o n s dealt with scien ce in ter e s t and scienc e in the news. In additi o n, 
the use of Perso n a l Meani n g Maps (PMMs , see 3.3.1 ) allowe d the stude n t s to expre s s ideas 
that they regar d e d as being impor t a n t withi n astronomy . In this manner, aspects of Shen?s 
cultur a l scien t i fi c liter a cy can be ident i fi e d in the findi n g s . 
Rennie (2001) stresse s that most researc h  suppor t s the idea that  ?people [during a 
scienc e centre visit] have a scienc e - r e l a t e d encou n t e r which enabl e s them to make more 
sense , in a scient i fi c way, of thei r exper i ences?. (p. 114-115). This  echoes the views of 
Rudy and Creel from the ASTC listserv e . Re nnie identifies the significance of science 
centr e s for scienc e commu n i c a t i on as follow s : as they prolif e r a t e they reach a larger 
audien c e , and they assist to engage with adults whose sch ooling system has not created 
scientif i c a l l y literate school-l e a v e r s . Howeve r , her contex t appear s to be develo p e d 
count r i e s , where famil y and adult visit o r s to sc ien c e centr e s are likel y to  be in the ma jor i t y . 
In the conte x t of South Afric a , the bulk of visito r s are school partie s (Table 1.2), and 
there f o r e the aims of scien c e centr e s for these vi sito r s are likely to be differe n t to those of 
developed nations. Currently in  South Africa there is wide spread concern regarding the 
low number s of school leaver s who have part ic i p a t e d in a meanin g f u l experi e n c e of 
scien ce and mathe ma t i c s at school , as well as achie v eme n t at this level (Tayl o r & 
Vinjevo l d , 1999). The focus of school visits is therefo r e not only about ?the nature of the 
scienti f i c enterpr i s e and the role society plays in it? (Rennie, 2001 p. 117), but also on 
science content knowledg e , which appears to be downplayed in developed countries. For 
this reason , my own study focus e d less on studen t attit u de s towar d s science and 
astron o my , and more on the effect of the sc hool visit on cognit i ve learni n g that may or 
may not have taken place . This is an area of c onsi d e r a bl e deba t e in the liter a t ur e , and it is 
to learn i n g in museu ms and scien c e centre s to which we now turn. 
2.7 Learning in museums and science centres 
A s Braund & Reiss (2004) have stated, ?trying to define learni n g is an almos t impo s s i b l e 
task? (p 4). At a na?ve level , a laype r s o n usin g a standard dictionary  to define the term 
would find an entry such as ? noun: acqui r e d knowl e d g e or skill , esp knowledge acquired 
34 
by study or educati o n ? (Allen, 2000). The word is laden with ideas of facts and memory, 
probab l y due to its associa t i o n with school and exams. Alt hou g h na?ve, it is probabl y on 
this premi s e that much of the resea r c h in  the 1970s and 1980s in museums and scienc e 
centre s appear s to show that  little learni n g take s place (McCla f f e r t y , 1995; Uzzel, 1993). 
Howeve r , by the follow i n g decade it was realis e d that the resea r c h me tho d o l o g i e s used, 
parti c u l a r l y exper i me n t a l studi es measuring particular outcom e s were inappr o p r i a t e for the 
museum enviro n me n t . Resear c h e r s such as Falk and Dierking (1992) suggested that 
schola r s were misgui d e d in their search for na rrow l y - d e f i n e d learni n g object i v e s , and that 
the learn i n g that was in fact takin g place was not being ident i fi e d . 
If we take a broade r view of learni n g , as  more than just an increa s e in knowle d g e , a 
definit i o n such as the followi n g might be more appro p r i a t e :  
Learnin g is an interna l change in a person ? the format i o n of new mental 
associ a t i o n s or the potenti a l for new respons e s ? that comes about as a 
result of experie n c e (Renni e ,  2001 p. 112 after Woolfol k 1987) 
In this case, ther e is refer e n c e to the ps ychological view of learnin g (?new mental 
associ a t i o ns? ) as well as the f act that such assoc i a t i o n s are due to experience. However, for 
the purpose s of my study, a richer and more el aborate view of lear ni n g is import a n t to 
reflec t the relati v e l y comple x world outside the school enviro n me n t . Such a defini t i on 
might be: 
Learning is a process of active engage me n t with experie n c e . It is what 
people do when they want to make se nse of the world. It may involve the 
develop me n t or deepeni n g of skills, knowle d g e , unders t a n di n g , 
aware n e s s , value s , ideas and feelin g s  or an increa se in the capaci t y to 
reflec t . Effec t i v e learn i ng leads to ch ange , devel o p me n t and the desire to 
learn more (Braund & Reiss, 2004 p. 5 after Campaig n for Learnin g ) 
Such an explana t i o n is very broad, capturi n g expe r i e nc e , meani n g makin g and feeli n gs , as 
well as a range of more ?traditionally recogn ised? aspects of learning, such as knowledge 
and skills . Simila r attemp t s at provid i n g a st ipula t i v e definit i o n of learnin g are made by 
museu m resea r c h e r s . Allen (2002 ) , in her study  of visit o r talk in an exhib i t i o n on Frogs , 
descri b e d a fra mew o r k for defini n g learni n g . This includ e d refere n c e to Bloom? s 
taxono my , the use of a socioc u l t u r a l perspe c t i v e , a de-li nk i n g from for mal learni n g 
asses s me n t , yet a relati ve l y narro w defi n i t i o n of learn i n g as ?disc u s si o n of the exhi b i t s and 
the exhibi t i on? .  
35 
Intere s t i n g l y , in their book on muse um l earning Falk and Dierking (2000) do not 
provide a definit i o n of learnin g . Instead , they st ress repea t e d l y that le arning is a series of 
process e s depende n t on three context s , the pe rson a l , the socioc u l t u ral and the physical. I 
elaborate on Falk and Dierki ng ? s contextu a l mode l of learning in se ction 2.8.1. Such a 
broad, all-e n c o mpa s s i n g view of learni n g is howeve r not withou t its critic s within the 
museum commu n i t y . Allen chose her relati v e l y  narrow definition of learning so that she 
should make stronger claims about learning as a response to the view ?Yes, visitors have 
fun in mus eums , but what do they really  learn? ? (Allen, 2002 p. 262). As a museum 
pract i t i o n e r , Ted Ansbac h e r argue s that be cau se learni n g has multi p l e meani n g s , and is 
interp r e t e d differ e n t l y by differ e n t people ,  we should avoid the term altogeth e r 
(Ansbacher, 1998b). Instead, he pr opo s e s that we use the term outcomes of a visitor?s 
exper i e n c e s in a museum , and identi f i e s a num ber of possib l e outco me  categ o r i e s such as 
?achie v e unders t a n d i n g ? , ?devel o p physic a l knowle d g e ? (a Piaget i a n term) and ?chang e 
feelin g s or attitu d e s? (Ansba c h e r , 2002a) . Ansbache r ? s critique has been countere d by 
Dierkin g and others who maintai n that while outco me s are a useful measu r e for expre s s i ng 
what an indiv i d u a l has exper i e n c e d duri n g a museu m visi t , the term lear n i n g is still 
appropri a t e . Dierking et al. (2002), citing eviden c e for learnin g in museums over the past 
20 years and accept i n g that there are differ e n t views of the word learning , argu e that it is 
an appro p ri a t e umbre l l a term as long as it is ?broadl y and clearly defined and emerge[ s ] 
from the meani n g the visit o r makes of the exper i e n c e ? (p 2).  
My study examin e s learning  at two out-of-school sites. I have cited this exchange 
of views in some detai l to demon s t r at e th ere is not consens u s within the mu seum 
commu n i t y on what consti t ut e s learning . Howeve r , I conside r that to abandon the term 
learning  and instead use outcomes is not a progre s s i v e ste p. The fact that there are 
differing understandings of ?learn i ng ? is in itsel f not a conv in c i n g argume n t that use of 
anoth e r term will resul t in clari t y . For the purpo s e s of my study, I will theref o r e conti n u e to 
refer to ?learn i n g ? , and use Braund & Reiss? s descr i pt i o n (2004 ) as a stipu l a t i v e defin i t i o n 
of learni n g in my contex t .  
Learning is a process of active engagement with experience.  
In a visitors? centre (such as at HartRAO) there is ample oppor t unity for students to 
engag e with exper i e n c e s made avail a b l e , such  as water rockets , whisper dishes and other 
small e r exhib i t s . In the planet a r i u m there is  less opportunity to do so, although the unusual 
setting and quality of the presenta t i o n can enabl e active engage me n t .  
36 
It may involve the development or deepening of skills, knowledge, 
understanding, awareness, values, ideas and feelings 
T h e focus of my study is on the deepen i n g of  knowle d g e and unders t a n d i n g , and to a lesser 
extent awaren e s s and ideas (of the solar system  and univer s e ) . At this point I should clarif y 
what I see as the differ e n c e betwee n knowle d g e and underst a n d i n g , at least within the 
contex t of my study. In common usage, knowle d g e and unders t a n d i n g can be used 
synonymous ly in the sense of a person?s awareness and comprehe nsion of an artefact such 
as a house. There are also numer o u s situa t i o n s where they are used in slightly different 
senses , for example whe r e a person knows what a television is but has no understanding of 
how it works. Since Tennyson?s distinction between declarative knowledge (?knowledge 
that?), procedural knowledge (?knowledge how ?) and contextu a l knowledg e (?knowle d g e 
why, when and where?) researchers have used  his frame w o r k as a basis for classifying 
types of knowle d g e involv e d in learni ng (Tennyso n & Rasch, 1988; Wellingt o n , 1990). 
Most scholar s would conside r that the most valuab l e form of knowle d g e is contex t u a l 
where a learne r is able to explai n why so mething occurs. For example, contextual 
knowled g e of the phases of the Moon would involv e  not just that they occur (declarative) 
or that they are due to sunlig h t falli ng on differing amounts of the Moon?s surface 
(procedu r a l ) , but that the variab le sunlight is due to the orbit of the Moon around the Earth. 
In this example , I am using contex t u a l knowledge in the same sense as understanding, 
where a true underst a n d i n g of the Moon?s phases is synonymo u s with context u a l 
knowledge. In this thesis , I will use the term knowledge  to refer to Tennyso n ? s declara t i v e 
and procedural knowledge and understanding  to refer to an abili t y to expla i n fully a 
phenome n o n at the contex t u a l level. 
Effective learning leads to change, de velopment and the desire to learn 
more 
I n Chapt e r s 5 to 8, usi ng the post-v i s i t in terviews and Personal Meaning Maps, I will 
demonst r a t e that student s have changed so me of their knowle d g e , unders t a n d i n g ideas and 
feelings , and that some of them ha ve been motiv a t e d to learn more.  
So far, this chapte r has review e d re searc h in science educati o n , astrono my 
education and informal learning, and has identi fie d some key papers releva n t to my own 
study. Althoug h previou s re sear c h has examine d what students learn there has been 
relativ e l y little work done on how they learn in scien c e centr e s . The next secti o n shows 
how I locate my research in a theore t i c a l framew o r k releva n t to informa l learni n g . 
37 
2.8 Theoretical Framework 
2.8.1  The Contextual Model of Learning 
P r i o r to the 1990s, the majorit y of museum resear c h was either based on behaviourist 
theorie s (Hein, 1998) or was atheore t i c a l (Anders o n et al . , 2003). Scott Paris sugges t s that 
the idios y n cr a t i c natur e of visit o rs ? museu m exper i e nc e s has been ?used as evidenc e that 
no genera l i z a t i o n s can be made about what or how people learn? (Paris, 1999 p. 2). Over 
the past 15 years relati v e l y few studie s have  examin e d scienc e museum learni n g using a 
theore t i c a l lens. Promi n e n t among those that have are John Falk and Lynn Dierking , who 
have conduc t e d numero u s resear c h and evalua t i v e studie s in scienc e centre s and museums 
since the 1980s . Their initi a l m odel of learn i n g in museu m s , the Inter ac t i v e Expe r i e n c e 
Model (Falk & Dierking, 1992), proposed a fram e wor k for organisi n g how visitor learning 
and behavio u r could be approac h e d . In the ne w century they recast the model into the 
Contex t u a l Model of Learni n g (Falk & Di erkin g , 2000), a more detaile d and refined 
approa c h which descri b e s how learni n g in  museums involv e s the three overla p p i n g 
context s of the persona l , the phys ical and the sociocul t u r a l .  
38 
 
Figure 2.1 The Contextual Model of Learning (Falk and Dierking 2000) 
T h e Context u a l Model of Learnin g (CML) (Fig ure 2.1) helps researchers to design their 
resear c h eithe r to fall withi n one of the thre e ma in contexts of lear ning, or integrate across 
all three . In the origi n a l propo s a l for my study I had inten d e d to  integrate all three contexts, 
but John Falk, in comment on the researc h qu est i o n s in my propos a l  advised that any 
differen c e s I might find between  indivi d u a l s in the study mi ght be obscur e d by too many 
?varia b l es ? . As a result of th is advice, I chose the personal c ontext as being an appropriate 
fra mewor k within which to base the resear c h , and concent r a t e d on student expecta t i o n , 
prior knowled g e , interes t s and beliefs as fact ors which may influence student learning. 
Altho u g h the CML provi d e s a model into whic h differ e n t types of museum and science 
centre learning can be fitted, and also provide s a holist i c way of exami n i n g such resea r c h, 
it does not provid e a theore t i c a l frame wo r k for th e learni ng itself . This is an approp r i a t e 
Interaction  
T he Personal Context The Sociocultural Context 
The Ph y sical Context
 Motivation 
Expectations 
Prior Knowledge, 
Interests and 
Beliefs 
Choice and 
Control 
Conversations: 
Within groups of 
peers, 
Within families. 
Facilitated 
mediation by 
others 
Advance 
organizers and 
orientation 
Design of 
exhibits 
Reinforcing 
events & 
experiences 
after visit
 Al l three 
contexts 
interact with 
each other 
and the 
dimension of 
time 
39 
point to revisit my researc h questio n s , as it is they which shape d the final choice of a 
theoreti c a l framewor k . My fi rst quest i o n is as follo w s : 
To what extent do students learn in th e proce s s of a visit to a plane t a r i u m 
or the visitors ? centre of an  astronomical observatory? 
The emphasi s on learnin g by individ u a l s who part i c i p at e in the visi t to a scien c e cent r e 
sugges t e d that some for m of concep t u a l ch ange theory might be appropri a t e . During my 
literat u r e review for the researc h propos a l I found that David Anders o n had used human 
const r u c t i vi sm as a theory on which to base his resea r c h in a scienc e centr e . Howev e r , his 
study empha s i s e d the cogni t i ve side of learni n g and he placed heavy emphas i s on post-v i s i t 
activ i t i e s carri e d out in the cl assr o o m after the studen t s return e d to school . My pilot studie s 
had shown that many teacher s visiting the planetarium and HartRAO tended neither to 
prepare their studen t s to visit the centre, nor follow up the vi sit afterwa r d s . This has also 
been found in other studies through o u t the world (e.g. Gri ffin & Symi ngton, 1997; 
Koosimi l e , 2004). Storks d i e c k  stated ?acknow l e d g i n g the fact that teache r s tend to not 
prepar e or follow - u p on field trips, out-of - s c h o o l learni n g enviro n me n t s could also provid e 
experiences that are va luable without preparat i o n or follo w-up? (Storksdieck, 2004 p. iv). I 
there f o r e conce n t r a t e d on the stude nt s ? exper i e nc e s of the visit rathe r than incor p ora t i ng 
the teache r into the study . In the light of th is, the theor e t i c a l frame w o r k s that most 
appeale d to me were those of huma n constr uc t i v i s m and the conceptu a l change model as 
put forward by Alsop and Watts  (1997) when studying info rma l learni n g . These are 
discuss e d in the followi n g section s . 
2.8.2 Human Constructivism 
H u ma n const r u ct i vi s m (HC) is a variat i o n of individual constr uctivism developed by 
Joseph Novak and his collaborators (Mintze s & Wanders e e , 1998; Mintzes et al ., 1997; 
Novak, 1985, 1988). Combinin g Ausubel? s theo ry of meaningful  learning with 
episte mol o g i c a l princi p l e s of constr u c t i v i s m tha t were emer gi n g in the 1970s and 1980s, 
Novak develope d a research programme and prac ti c a l tools (such as concep t mappin g ) that 
assist e d stude n t s in a meta- a n a l y s i s of thei r own learni n g . In his analys i s of scienc e 
learnin g Novak identif i e d eight principles of learning together  with examp l e s relev a n t to 
teachin g . While the first four principl e s were portraye d as being ?general l y agreed upon by 
most researc h e r s in the field? (Novak, 1988 p. 82) , from the viewp o i n t of this thesi s it is 
worth briefl y examin i n g these princi p l e s to see how they have been upheld by more recent 
40 
resear c h , and how they can be regarde d as be ing relevant to infor mal learning . The first 
four princip l e s are as follows : 
? C o n c e p t s are acquir e d early in life 
? M i s c o n c e p t i o n s are acquire d early and are resist a n t to modifi c a t i o n 
? P r i o r learni ng influ e n c e s new learni ng 
? T h e brain?s infor ma t i o n process i n g capacit y is limited . 
Since the late 1980s there has been consid e r a b l e  further research evidence to confirm these 
four princip l e s , notably the work of  Driver and associates (e.g. Driver et al . , 1994), 
resear c h into concept u a l change (e.g. Hewson & Hewson, 1992; Strike & Posner, 1992) 
and neurolo g i c a l resear ch (e.g. Calvin, 1996).  
Novak regard e d the remaini n g four princip l es as being more c ontroversial and still 
open to confir ma t i o n or reject i o n by furthe r resear c h . Princi p l e 5 is ?Most knowledge is 
store d hiera r c h i c a l l y ? , and is one of the prin c i p a l ratio n a l e s for the use of conce p t mappi n g : 
that the ma ps thems e l v e s repre s e n t ?ment a l mo de l s ? whic h exis t in a simi l a r form in the 
brain. While some propon e n t s of concep t mapping support this not ion (e.g. Mintzes et al ., 
1997; Pearsall, Skipper & Mintzes, 1997), othe rs are mor e circums p e c t (e.g. McClur e , 
Sonak & Suen, 1999), and still others are cr itica l (e.g. Kagan, 1990). From the point of 
view of informa l learni n g , a sma ll number of studie s (e.g. Anders o n et al ., 2003; Anderson 
et al ., 2000) have successfully used concept mappin g to determi n e the develop me n t of 
students ? understa n d i n g of con c e p t s as a resul t of a visit to  a scienc e cent re. Subsequent 
resea r c h there f o r e , while is has not entir e l y di sca r d e d princ i p l e 5, has also not confi r me d it, 
except as a heuristic method whic h appears to be quite valuable  in research about learning. 
Princ i p l e 6 is ?Learn e r s are seldom consc i o u s of their cogniti v e process e s ? and relates to 
Flavel l ? s work on metaco g n i t i o n (Flav e l l , 1976) . Research studies over the last 15 years 
have generall y confirme d this  notion (e.g. Adey & Shayer , 1994) and many interventions 
now aim to make learners consciou s of their own learning in order to  impro v e th e learning 
that takes place. In informa l learni n g this is usually difficul t to do in a museum or science 
centre setti ng . Howev e r , it has b een carri e d out in studi e s relat e d to the public awaren e s s of 
scien c e , and ?maki n g learn e r s  aware of their own learni n g ? has proved to be a useful 
techniq u e in some studies (e.g. Alsop, 2000;  Alsop & Watts, 1997). Principles 7 and 8 
have also gained suppor t from resear c h  since 1988. Princip l e 7: ?Episte mo l o g i c a l 
commit me n t s of studen t s influe n c e learni n g ? has been exami ne d from the point of view of 
41 
the Nature of Science (e.g. Lederman, Abd- El - K h a l i c k , Bell & Schwar t z , 2002) as well as 
concept u a l change researc h (e.g. Tyson et al . , 1997) and also by Steve Alsop in the area of 
informa l scienc e learni n g (Alsop & Watts , 1997). Accordin g to evidence presente d by 
Weinbu r g h in her extens i v e review of attitud e s toward s scienc e (Weinb u r g h , 1995), 
Principle 8 ?Thinking, feeling and acting are integra t e d ? had been researc h e d extens i v e l y 
prior to Novak?s paper and has formed an importa n t compone n t of underst a n d i n g learnin g 
in scienc e centr e s (e.g. Jarvi s & Pell, 2005; Rennie & McClafferty, 1996). 
Since his 1988 work, Novak and his associa t e s have further elaborated huma n 
constr u c t i v i s m and its asso ciat e d pedagogi c a l procedur es of concept mapping, vee 
diagrams and semanti c network s (Mintze s et al ., 1997). This view  of learning is 
summar i s e d in Figure 2.2, and demons t r a t e s th e relati o ns h i p of four cognit i ve proce s s e s 
from meani n g f u l learn i n g : subsu mp t i o n , supe rordinate learning, progr ess i v e differen t i a t i o n 
and integra t i v e reconci l i a t i o n . I describ e each of these main proce s s e s as I use them in 
subsequ e n t analysi s . 
42 
 
Figure 2.2 Concept Map of Human Constr uctivism (after Mintzes and Wandersee 1998) 
2.8.2.1  Subsumption 
I n his theory of meani ngf u l learning , Aus ube l ? s origin a l (1968) and revised (Ausubel, 
Novak & Hanesia n , 1978) explan a t i o n of subs ump t i o n refers to ?the process of linking 
new infor ma t i o n to pre-ex i st i n g segmen t s of cognit i v e struct u r e ? (1978 p. 58). The 
impor t a n t aspec t is that new meani n g s refle c t a subordinate  relationship to an existing 
cognit i v e config u r a t i o n . He identi f ied two types of subsumption: derivative subsumption  
and correlative subsumption . In derivati v e subsumpt i o n , ne w material is understood as a 
specifi c example of, or is illu strative of, a previously learne d proposition. For example that 
the colours scarlet and lavende r are names for colou r s , altho u g h they are muc h less 
common than red or purple . In correl a t i v e su bsump t i o n new materi a l is an elabo r a t i o n , 
modifica t i o n or extensio n of previous l y le arned knowledge. Although ?it is incorporated 
by ?. more inclus i v e subsume r s , ?? its me an ing cannot be adequate l y represen t e d by? 
43 
them (Ausu b e l et al., 1978 p. 59). For example understanding that displaying a country?s 
flag is an act of patriotism.  
Since these origin a l defini t i o n s , subseq u e n t schola r s have placed less emphas i s on 
identif y i n g differe n t types of subsump t i o n , and have used the model as an expla n a t i o n of 
the gaining of new specifi c concept s , linked to more genera l and inclu si ve conce p t s withi n 
a perso n? s cogn i t i ve struc t u r e (Pear sa l l et al ., 1997). An example of subsumption would be 
a studen t learni n g the names and cha r ac t e r i s t i c s of the nine plane t s of the solar syst e m, and 
it is regar d ed as by far the most common of the four proce s s e s of meanin g f u l learni n g , 
involv i n g a ?weak? form of knowle d g e restru c t u r i n g in the brain, as well as an incremental 
change in conceptual understanding. 
2.8.2.2 Superordinate Learning 
A u s u b e l et al. explai ned super o r d i n a t e learn i ng as being ?when one learns an inclus i v e 
new proposi t i o n under which several establi s h e d ideas may be subsum e d ? (1978 p. 59). 
Pearsa l l et al. regard supero r d i n a t e l earning as being less common in school science, and 
regard it as ?a signif i c a n t and rapid shift in conceptu a l  understanding? involving strong 
knowled g e restruc t u r i n g (1997 p. 196). The example they give is very perti n e n t to my own 
rese ar c h 5 : if an indiv i d u a l learn s th at scient i s t s have decide d that Pluto should no longer be 
considered to be a planet (o n the basi s of revi s e d scient i fi c notio n s of the conce pt planet ) , 
the conceptual change required to make sense of this new information involves 
supero r d i n a t e learni n g , result i ng in a student?s changed u nderstanding of the concept 
planet.  
2.8.2.3  Progressive Differentiation 
Both subsumption and superordinate learning  resul t in the clari fi ca t i o n of conce p t 
meani n g s , and, accor d i ng to human const r u c t i vi s m, a person ? s knowl e dge base become s 
more hierar c h i c a l and comple x . Such increa si ng structural complexity  is refer r ed to as 
progres s i v e differe n t i a t i o n (Mintze s et al . , 1997; Pearsa l l et al . , 1997). In his original 
theory, Ausubel seems to link progressive di fferentiation specifically with subsumption, 
where b y ?the proce s s of subsu mp t i o n ? leads to progressive  differentiation of the 
subsumi n g concep t or propos i t i o n ? (Ausub e l et al . , 1978 p. 124 emphasis in original ) . 
                                                 
 
5  It is also top ical. As I revised my thesis in Augu st 2006 the Intern ation al Astron o mical Union deb ated the 
issue of Pluto as a plane t; a topic which made head lin e s inter n a tion a lly. Pluto was ?demo te d ? to become a 
dwar f plan e t. 
44 
Howev e r later author s such as Pearsa l l and Mint zes and associa t e s indi cate that progressive 
differ e n t i a t i o n occurs either throug h subsump t i o n or superor d i n a t e learni n g . 
2.8.2.4  Integrative Reconciliation 
T h i s occurs alongs i d e progre s s i v e differ e n t i a t i o n , as struc t u r a l compl e x i t y incre a s e s , and 
refer s to ?the expli c i t delin e a t i o n of si milar i t i e s and di fferences among closely related 
concep t s? (Pear s a l l et al., 1997 p. 196). The example given by these authors, again related 
to astronom y , is when students learn about sim ilari t i e s and differen c e s in the atmos phe r e s 
of differe n t planets , ?their knowledge structures become more  interconnecte d, integrated 
and cohesive? (p. 196). Again, while later authors regard it as a result  of both subsumption 
and supero r d i n a t e learni n g , Ausube l ? s origin a l theory refers to integrative reconciliation 
occurr i n g as a result of superor d i n a t e learning, whereby new meanings arise and 
confli c t i n g meanin g s become resolv e d . 
One further cognitiv e process is referr e d to by Ausube l in his origin a l theor y of 
meaning f u l learnin g alongsi d e that of supe ror d i n a t e learnin g , name ly combina t o r i a l 
learni n g . In this type of lear ni n g , he sugges t e d that the new propos i t i o n s being learnt could 
be classif i e d neither as subordi n a t e (as in s ubsumption) nor superordinate. However, this 
type of learnin g has not been referr e d to by later author s ,  and has fallen into disuse. 
I have shown how the core of Ausubel? s th eory of meaningful learning has been 
assimi l a t e d into the notion of human constr u c t i v i s m , and how aspec t s of th e orig i na l theo r y 
have been adapte d as furthe r evidenc e has become availa b l e . While me anin g f u l learni n g 
has broadly influenced severa l resear c h e r s of out-of - s c h o o l learning (Falk & Dierking , 
1997; Orion, 1993), only Anderso n has used human  constr u c t i v i s m as a basis for specifi c 
empiri c a l study (Ander s o n , 1999; Anders o n et al . , 2003). In his study of a school visit to a 
science centre he identifi e d seven categor i e s of knowled g e tr ansformation, three of which 
he relate d direct l y to pr oces s e s descri b e d by human constr u c t i v i s m: subsump t i o n , 
progres s i v e differe n t i a t i o n and integrative reconcilia tion. However, Anderson?s 
interpre t a t i o n is somewhat at variance w ith human constru c t i v i s m as propose d by Mintzes 
et al. (1997, 1998). In his concept map of HC he indicate s a link showing that subsumpt i o n 
result s in supero r d i n a t e learni n g (Ander s o n et al ., 2003 p. 180), yet he found no evidence 
of superordinate learning in his study.  
In my own study , the cogni t i ve aspec t s of learni n g were exami n e d from a human 
constructivist viewpoint, taking into account both Novak?s and Mintzes? theories , as well 
45 
as Anderson?s interpretation and his own cat egories of ?knowledge transfo r ma t i o n ? . In 
Chapt e r 6 I will show how these cogni t i ve proce s s e s can be applie d to the data collec t ed 
during the fieldwor k in my study. 
2.8.3  Conceptual Change 
M y study howev e r , is not merel y a study in cogni t i o n . There is ample evide n c e to sugge st 
that other proces s e s are taking place in people? s minds when they visit a scienc e centr e or 
museum (e.g. Jarvi s & Pell, 2005; Short l a n d , 1987) . Return i n g to the en tert a i n me n t versu s 
educatio n debate in section 2.5, the affectiv e dime nsion of learning has been stressed as 
being impor t a n t by numero u s resea r c h e r s (Osb or n e , Simon & Colli n s , 2003) . While some 
resear c h e r s have been concer n e d that ?having fun? in a scienc e centre do mi na t e s over real 
learni n g , I have shown that there is consid e r a b l y evide n c e that th ey can both occur (e.g. 
Falk & Dierkin g , 2000; Gilbert , 2001). Due to the under-theori sed nature of learning in 
infor ma l envir o n me n t s , I found a pertin e n t conceptu a l fra mewor k that includes the 
affect i v e dime ns i o n of learni n g elusiv e . Even t u al l y I disco v er e d it in  an unexpec t e d place: 
within the theory of concep tual change. In the 1980s Posn er and colleag u e s (Strik e & 
Posner, 1985) developed the theory of conc eptual change, in which they show how 
learne r s trans fo r m their conce pt i on s durin g th e proces s of learni n g ,  firstly by Piaget?s 
assimilation, ?where a major concept u a l revi sion is not required? (p. 215) and 
accommodation, in which the student replaces or re organises his or he r centra l conce p t s . 
The same researc h e r s also describ e d how the features of students? ?conceptual ecology? 
(cogn i t i v e resou r c e s) relat e to condi t i o n s n ecessary for accommoda t i o n to take place . 
These condit i o n s are dissat i s f a c t i o n with the ex istin g concept i o n , intelli g i b i l i t y , plausibi l i t y 
and fruitfu l n e s s of the new concept i o n . Critiqu e of this origina l theory resulte d in the 
revisio n i s t theory of concept u a l change (Str ik e & Posner , 1992) to accoun t for the roles of 
emoti o n and intui t i o n . Furth e r ?non cogni t i v e ? develo p me n t of the model was propos e d by 
David Treagust and colleagu e s (Tyson et al ., 1997) in which they suggested a 
multi d i me n s i o n a l inter pre t i v e frame w o r k . In this  model they ident i fi e d three dime n s i o n s 
which influe n c e d the concep t u a l knowle d g e  of the learne r over time: episte m o l o g y , 
ontolo g y and social / a f f e c t i v e . 
Alsop and Watts (1997), in th eir study of adults? informa l learning about radiation, 
have adapte d the Treagu s t concep t ua l change m odel (CCM) to suit their resear c h conte x t . 
Alsop and Watts? revisio n identif i e d four lens es (cognitive, affective, conative and self-
46 
estee m) throu g h which to view the scienc e le arning taking place. Their research had shown 
them that neithe r the revi se d CCM, nor even  Treagust?s further modification was entirely 
suita bl e for learni n g withi n an infor ma l cont e x t . The cogni t i ve lens ?reta i n e d the basic 
elements of the Strike and Posner m odel? (Alsop & Watts, 1997 p. 638), name ly 
intelligible , plausible  and fruitful . The Strike and Posner  mo del also included 
dissatisfaction but Alsop and Watts omit it from thei r frame wo r k , possib l y becaus e in an 
infor ma l envir o n me n t it is diffi c u l t to show th at the lear ne r is dissat i s fi ed with his or her 
existing knowledg e . 
For the affec t i v e lens, Alsop and Watts id enti f y three elemen t s of how learner s 
engag e with the topic . These are germane , referrin g to personal relevanc e ; salient referr i ng 
to the promi n e n c e of the topic in  the learning environment; and palatable  (how agreeabl e 
the mater i al is). Alsop and Watts stre s s the im port a n t nature of the affecti v e lens and how 
it can affect learn i n g . This can be in a positi v e way whereb y learn e r s are motiva t e d to 
engage with the topic when they are intere s t e d  in it. This has been demo ns t r a t e d nume ro u s 
times in museu ms (e.g. Csiks z e n t mi h a l y i & Hermans o n , 1995; Ra me y- G a s s e r t , Wa lber g & 
Walberg, 1994). It can also negatively aff ec t how learni n g occurs , whereby learners 
disen g a g e with a topic they are not inte res t e d in (e.g. Jarv is & Pell, 2002, 2005). 
Resear c h e r s such as Suzann e Hidi distin g ui s h betwe e n perso n a l inter es t , relat i n g to the 
indivi d u a l and situat e d intere s t which relat e s to conte x t u a l facto r s in the envir on me nt 
which promot e intere s t (Hidi & Harach i e w i c z ,  2000). While it is diffic u l t to change the 
long-t e r m prefer e n c e s of person a l interest, the context of a cl assroom or science centre can 
manip u l a t e the envi r o nme n t to try to devel o p situa t i o n a l inter e s t . Both motiv a t i on and 
situat i o n al inter e s t relat e to Shen? s no tio n of cultu r al scienc e liter a c y , where the 
achiev e me n t s of scienc e can be recogn i s e d . 
Alsop and Watts refe r to their third dime n s i o n as the ?cona t i v e? lens which relat es 
to the ways in which a learne r mi ght take  action to use the knowledg e in a practica l 
manner. They suggest that it follows from Strike and Posner?s notion of fruitfulness , and 
that the learne r then asks ?H ow can I use that knowled g e ? Does  it empower me to act? ? (p. 
639). Withi n this dimens i o n they  identif y three element s : how actionable  is the knowledge, 
what control do they have over its use, and the extent to which they can trust their 
understa n d i n g . The fourth lens (self-es t e e m) refer s to the learne r s ? own confidence, self-
 image  and autonomy within the framew o r k of scienc e l earni ng and relie s heavi l y on meta-
47 
cognit i v e views of the student s ? learni n g . My interviews questions were not devised to 
bring this out, so I did no t use it in my analys i s . 
The Alsop and Watts model has many adva n t a g e s for a study such as mine. It 
stresses the affectiv e nature of learning , but  not at the expense of the cognitive, and 
introduces a new component: the conative  aspect . Howev e r , unlik e the human 
const r u ct i vi st model I have descr i b e d, it was not ident i fi e d prior to the data colle ct i o n , and 
therefo r e the questio n s asked of the student s were  not tail or e d to ?suit ? the mode l . This has 
both advantages and disadvantages: an advantag e is that questio n s asked of the student s 
were not set up to in some way ?pr ove ? the mo del. A furthe r advant a g e is that the initia l 
analys i s of the questi o n s was not biased by the model? s featur e s . A disadv a n t a g e is that 
some questi o n s which might have been asked, in  line with the chara ct er i s t i c s of the model 
were not asked, and therefor e some aspects of the model may go unconf i r me d . Howeve r , 
by using this and a human constr u c t i v i s t framew o r k for the analys i s of indivi d u a l learni n g 
(see Chapte r s 6 to 8) and using ?botto m up? or induc t i v e codin g to demon s t r a t e how the 
students learnt as a group (see  Chapter 5), I hope to  show I have capture d the major i t y of 
the learni n g that took place during the visit by the sample  of student s in my study. 
2.9  Summary 
T h i s chapt e r has descri be d the resear c h relat e d to scien c e educa t i o n , astro n o my educa t i on 
and informal learning. While the section on  scienc e educa t i o n set the scene for my 
resea r c h , findi n g s from both the other secti o n s of  the revie w have a numbe r of impli c a t i o n s 
for my study. First, my analys i s  of the astronom y educatio n rese arch over the past 30 years 
shows that the majori t y of st udies have examine d people ? s views of day and night, the 
Earth-S u n - M o o n system and the phases of the Moon. Althou g h I includ e d questi o n s on 
these in my interview questions, the literature suggested that other, less researched areas 
were importa n t for a study such as mine. Hence,  I inclu de d the topic s of gravity, stars, the 
Sun, the Solar System and size and scale as ar eas for investigation, and identified Big Ideas 
in astronomy as areas of focus.  
Secondl y , much of the astrono my educat i o n research reported uses questionnaires 
and tests to collec t data from subjec t s . Al so, much of the discus sion is under-theorised, 
with lists of misconceptions being one of the princ i p al outco me s . Furt her analysis of the 
findi n g s sugge s t s that the more intere s t i n g re sul t s whic h have impli c a t i on s for learnin g and 
teachi n g are where resear c h e r s have used in ter v i e w s and intro d u c e d model s into the 
48 
discuss i o n . I therefo r e used a structu r e d interv i e w to collec t my data, and used a model of 
the Earth- S u n - M o o n syste m for clarifi c a t i o n . 
Thirdl y , very little resear c h about astron o m y has been carried out in South Africa. 
Given the import a n c e the govern m e n t is placin g on astron o m y curren t l y (see Chapte r 1), it 
is certainl y a worthwhi l e area  of study. Related to this is the dearth of research in 
planetaria over the past deca de; given the inconclu s i v e re sults of much of the early 
experi me n t a l resear c h , it shoul d be an area worthy of furthe r study, especia l l y within an 
interpr e t i v i s t paradig m. 
Fourth l y , althou g h a conside r a b l e litera t u r e of studies of infor mal education has 
been publi s h e d over the last 15 years , the va st major i t y has been withi n devel o p e d 
count r i e s , mostl y in the Northe r n he mi s p h e r e . Very little empiri c a l resear c h relate d to 
museu ms and scien c e centr e s has been condu c t e d or publish e d in South Africa. The very 
differe n t priori t i e s of the S outh Africa n educat i o n system compar e d with those of 
develo p e d countr i e s me an that resear c h in the contex t of a developing country will be a 
contri b u t i o n to the liter atu r e . 
Fifth l y , seve r a l studi e s have empha s i s e d  the import a n c e of teache r s prepar i n g 
studen t s for the visit and follow i n g up with activ i t i e s after the visit .  However, throughout 
the world this seems to happe n only to a limit e d extent, with most outings taking place as 
?stand- a l o n e ? trips. Given this  reality, I therefor e thoug h t it impor t a n t to deter mi n e how 
much learnin g takes place in the visit al one, with no referenc e to teache r input. 
Finall y , mu ch of the resear c h in museums and scienc e centre s elsewh e r e has been 
under-t h e o r i s e d . Models for learnin g have b een put forward , but few have been tested 
empir i c a l l y . Relat i v e l y few st udie s have examin e d how learni n g occur s in an informa l 
enviro n me n t , and my study is an opport u n i t y to  provide an empiric a l basis on which future 
resea r c h can build in this area. 
49 
Chapter 3 
I present a justification for the research design of the study, the instruments 
used for data collection as well as a description of how data was collected. I 
also discuss issues of ethics and credib ility, and the limitations inherent in the 
study. 
3  Research Design and Methodology 
3.1  Introduction and Overview 
In this chapter, I discuss the methodology wh ich underpins my empiri cal study, and show 
how it relat e s to the theor e t i c a l frame w o r k and my resea r ch quest i o n s . Furth e r , I descr i be 
the method s used in the study, the ration a l e for using such methods, the data collection 
proces s , the select i o n of study sites and partic i p a n t s , the instru me n t s used to gather data, 
the develop me n t and pilotin g of the instrume n t s , as well as issues of ethics, credibil i t y and 
trustwo r t h i n e s s and the li mit a t i o n s of the study .  
Over the perio d July to Decemb e r 2003 the Johan n e s b u r g Plane t a r i u m and 
Hartebe e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Observatory visitors? centre hosted nume rous  visits by 
school and other groups from grade R (kinde r g a r t e n ) to universi t y students . Prior to my 
for mal data collection I attend ed five shows at the planetar ium and four visits by school 
groups at the observ a t o r y , made field notes a nd collec t e d pilot data for the study. From 31 
July to 7 Novembe r I observ e d and video/ a udio recorded seven sc ho o l s visiti n g the study 
site s , and coll e ct e d data from 57 stude n t s ag ed 12 to 15 who completed Personal Meaning 
Maps and whom I interviewed about their expe rience of the visit. Ov er the course of 2003 
there were a few newswo r t h y items relat e d to  astron o my and space scienc e that studen t s 
might have been exposed to at school or in the media. The first of these was the Columbia 
Space Shuttl e disast e r of 1 Februar y 2003, when  the shuttle disint e gr a t e d on its re-entr y 
into the Earth? s atmos p h e r e , ki llin g all seven crew me mber s . Later in the year (late August 
2003), the planet Mars was at its close s t to Eart h in 50,000 years, appearing six times 
large r and 85 times brigh t e r than norma l . Th is generat e d consid e r a b l e media attent i o n , 
includ i n g severa l miscon c e p t i o n s , such as the idea that the planet would be as big as the 
Moon in the night sky. In Oc tober, China?s first astronaut  was launched into space, an 
event which again made it into  the news headline s . A prior event, in Decemb e r 2002, was a 
50 
total eclip se of the Sun visib l e over the northe r n areas of S outh Africa , which was visible 
as a partial eclipse in the Gauteng area. All these phenomen a caught the attention of at 
least some of the stude n t s in my study, as th ey referred to them dur ing their intervie w s . 
The conclu s i o n s and recomme n d a t i o n s from this study which I discuss in Chapter 9 
are releva n t to presen t a t i o n s at both the study si tes and more genera l l y to scienc e centre s in 
South Afric a but are limit e d to simil a r c ohort s of studen t s to those who were the 
parti c i p a nt s in this study. 
3.2  Methodology  
3.2.1  Paradigms of Research 
R e s e a r c h in much pure and appli e d scien c e follow s a fairl y consi s t e nt patte r n in which the 
research e r sets up a hypothes i s which he or sh e then tests by empir i c a l means to deter mi n e 
whethe r the premis e was correc t or not. Peru sal of science journal s and higher degree 
these s shows that scienc e resea r c he r s do not norma ll y consid e r their own ontolog i c a l 
assumpt i o n s . They regard their resea r c h as a means of discover i n g the nature of the real 
world, and that the role of scie nc e resear c h is to uncover real truths. Re sea r c h into lear n i n g 
(even if it is science learning)  is usual l y rega rde d as bein g differ e n t from researc h in 
science (Cohen & Manion, 1994). Educat i o n a l resear ch is normally regarded as a form of 
social science, in which the position of the resear c h e r has a strong influe n c e on the claims 
made (Henni n g , 2004). Most te xts on educational research  methods (e.g. Opie, 2004; 
Tobin, 2000) therefor e advise the research e r to  make clear his or her own ontological and 
epist e mol o g i c a l assumpt i o n s , and to expla i n care fully what paradigm of research he or she 
is working with. I gave an account of myself as  resea r c h e r in Chapt e r 1, which laid out my 
views on teachin g and learnin g . 
The three principa l paradigms or overarch i ng frameworks in social science research 
are positi v i s t , inter p r e t i v i s t and critic a l (Henni n g , 2004). They are regar d e d as parad i g ms 
in that they each have under l yi n g philo s o p h i c a l assumpt i o n s about how the world exists . 
The posit i vi s t or norma t i v e paradi gm regar d s huma n behav i o u r as being subje c t to rules 
and somethin g that can be investig a t e d us ing methods used in science, such as 
experime n t a t i o n . In contras t , the interpr e t i v i s t framew o r k takes an anti-po s i t i v i s t stance , 
exami n e s huma n s as indivi d u a l s , uses desc ript i v e methods to find out about these 
individ u a l s and interpr e t s meaning in the fi nding s . The critica l pa rad i g m attem p t s to 
quest i o n how power relat i o ns and the polit ical nature of human society affects 
51 
relati o n s h i p s . Instea d of measur i n g or interp r e t i n g huma n be havi o u r , th e critica l framewo r k 
works toward s decons t r u c t i n g and reconst r u c t i n g our world, usually involving some for m 
of critic a l reflec t i o n and action . My study lies in the interp r e t i vi s t paradi g m, as althou g h 
my own posit i o na l i t y has been influ e n c e d by the posit i vi sm of natur al scien ce , I belie ve 
that the proces s of educat i o n a l researc h involve s working with  people as indivi d u a l s . Such 
persons are best studied in de pth rather than by using experiments and surveys, which may 
only acquire data at the level of the co lle ct i v e rathe r than the indiv i d ua l .  
Anothe r divisi o n in social sc ienc e resear c h is betwee n quantitative and qualitative 
studies , which norma ll y (but not always ) follow from the paradi g m adopte d by the 
research e r (Opie, 2004). Opie suggests that a positivistic approach take n by a researcher 
will tend to lead to quanti t a t i v e resear c h  techni q u e s being adopte d , while an anti-
 p o s i t i vi st i c appro a c h will lead to quali t a t i v e metho d s . Chapt e r 2 descr i b e d that over the 
past half- cent u r y , the posit i vi st i c para d i g m  in scienc e educa t i o n has declin e d , and 
interp r et i v i s t and critic al appro a c hes have  beco me more do mi na nt (section 2.2). The 
follow i n g sectio n lays out my ration a l e for using qualitat i v e methods. 
3.2.2  A Case Study 
S i n c e the 1970s, studie s of museum learni n g have adopted a variety of methods for 
exami n i n g the exten t to which learn i n g takes place in the scien c e mu seu m and scien c e 
centr e envir o n me n t s . In commo n with the rest  of the scienc e educ ati o n field, since the 
1990s, there has been a shift away from e xperi me n t a l and quasi- e x p e r i me n t a l method s 
towar d s more inter p re t i v e , quali t a t i v e metho d s (Renn i e et al., 2003; Rennie & 
McClaffe r t y , 1996). The principa l reaso n for this chang e in empha s i s withi n the wider 
scienc e educat i o n commun i t y has be en the real i sa t i o n that soci a l and cont e x t u a l issue s play 
a greater role in learning than had been previously t hough t . (Duit & Treagus t , 1998). Key 
researc h e r s who have been influen t i a l in sh ift i n g the museu m resear c h commu n i t y towar d s 
more natural i s t i c studies are John Falk and Lynn Dierkin g . They have carried out 
numerou s studie s and reviews over the past  15 years (Dierk i n g & Falk, 1994; Falk & 
Dierking, 1992; Falk & Dierki ng, 2000), as well as collaborated with  many others (e.g. 
Adelma n , Falk & James, 2000; Falk et al . , 1998) using a variety of qualitative (and 
quanti t a t i v e ) experi me n t a l de signs which have added consid erably to our knowledge of 
learni n g in the infor ma l envir o n me n t . Other mus e um resear c h e r s have experi me n t e d with 
variou s visito r studie s techni q u e s such as time at exhibit s , pre- and post-vi s i t interv i e w s , 
52 
convers a t i o n s between visitor s , and the like. As a result of these efforts over the past 
decad e , speci a l issue s of leadi n g scienc e educat i on journa l s ha ve been devote d to informa l 
learni n g (Inter n a t i o n a l Jour nal of Science Education 1991, Science Education 1997, 
Journal of Research in Science T eaching 2003, and Science Education 2004) 
demons t r a t i n g the variet y of natura l i s t i c ap proaches and data collection methods now 
being used in the field. 
In line with this tr end, I decide d that my resear c h  study design should be along 
qualit a t i v e , rather than quantit a t i v e lines.  Accordin g to Tesch (1990) and Henning (2004), 
a quali t at i ve appro ac h allow s researc h e r s to co ndu c t more detai l e d and in-de pt h studi e s . 
Henni n g (2004 ) and Hollida y (2002 ) state that  a true or ?progre s si v e ? qualit a t i v e study 
does not use pre-de t e r mi n e d instru me n t s to captur e the data, but instea d relies upon 
observa t i o n (withou t using an observa t i o n sche dule), artefact and document studies, and 
intervi e w s (which may be semi -st r u c t u r e d ) . Hennin g (2004) is wary of qualita t i v e studie s 
which use pre-d e t e r mi n e d instr u me n t s , such as  observat i o n schedule s or questionnaires, as 
?na?v e natura l i st i c? studi e s , and treat s th em as falli n g betwe e n the two parad i g ms of 
posit i vi st and inter pr et i vi s t , captu ri ng neith er the numbe r s neede d for quant i t a t i v e work, 
nor the thick descrip t i o n s require d for effecti v e qualita t i v e study. Othe r researchers dispute 
this, for example Gorard and Taylor (2004) main t a i n that it is perfe c tly possible, and even 
advisab l e , to combine qualita t i v e and quant itative methods, and produce research of high 
quality . I decided that my own study should use predet e r mi n e d instr u me nts, but that these 
would be devised during the pilot data collecti o n phase. 
One of the featur e s of an ethnog r a p h i c qualit a t i v e resear c h design is that it involv e s 
very small numbe r s of parti c i p a n t s , who ar e studied in great depth (e.g. Boaler , 1998; 
Brodie, 2005; Wolcott, 1988). Accordin g l y , such  a study should give rise to a detail e d 
descri p t i o n and interp r e t a t i o n of the partici p a n t s ? ?lived experi e n ces? (Henning 2004). 
Althou g h this has become an accept e d and effec tive way of carrying out research, I had to 
consider the fact that the ?intervention? wh ich for ms part of my study is very short, 
confined to the one to three hours that student s spend at the study site . In some cases there 
may have been additi o n a l work done at school ,  but this was not the norm in my study. This 
is in cont r a st to the sort of inter v e nt i o n whic h is more norma l l y studi e d in research projects 
involving students and/or teachers. In these studie s (e.g. Moolla , 2003; Trundl e et al ., 
2002) the inter v e nt i o n can consi s t of a speci a l teachin g project , which is tracked over a 
period of weeks so that a detail e d pictur e of student s ? learnin g (and/o r beliefs, attitudes, 
53 
intere s t ) can be built up over a period of time. A study of info r ma l learn i n g such as mine 
exami n e s the effec t of a singl e ?eve n t ? whic h is not necess a r i l y relate d to classr o o m 
learni n g ; I found early on in the pilot phase of  my study that most teach e r s takin g a class to 
the planet a r i u m or Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astr o n o my Obser v a t o r y did not link the visit to 
what they were teachi n g in class. In my cas e therefo r e , I conside r e d that it would be 
impor t a n t to follo w the exper i e n c e of museu m  resear c h e r s such as Allen (2002) , Falk et al. 
(1998) and Paris and Mercer (2002) who used 49 pairs, 40 and 100 partici p a n t s 
respecti v e l y , when conducti n g studies. Instead of  acquiri n g limite d data in the form of a 
questio n n a i r e or survey, such studies obtai n detai l e d infor ma t i o n by inter vi e w i ng the 
particip a n t s . In this way, qualit a t i v e method s of analys i s can be applie d to the data, which 
can also be analys e d using descri pt i v e statis t i c s to help confir m the findin g s . 
My study followed a case study design (Stake, 1995; Yin, 1994). Althoug h I was 
initia l l y intere s t e d in follow i n g Yin?s m odel of ?multi p l e ? case studie s , I found his 
recom me n d a t i o n s for analy si s appro a c h t hose of quantitative research design. Yin 
recomme n d s that theore t i c a l propo si t i o n s be se t up as part of initial research design 
(simil a r to hypoth e s e s ) , which can be tested against the findings . Given my preferen c e for 
a qualita t i v e resear c h design , with my researc h  questi o n s attemp t i n g to find out the extent 
of learning and how it occurs at  science centres, I considered the setting up of propositions 
to be prema t u r e . Instea d I worked with St ake?s notion of a collect i v e case study (Stake 
1995) where a number of cases are selecte d in order to understa n d a particul a r situatio n 
being studie d . Stake regard s th e collect i v e case study as a special case of  an instr u me n t a l 
case study , in which a case (e.g. a teache r ) is being studie d to unders t a n d some th i n g in 
addition to the case itself (e.g. the way the teacher works with assessme nt). Stake then 
recommen d s interpre t i v e methods  of analys i s to unders t a n d mo re about the cases select e d , 
which may lead to limit e d gener a l i s a t i o n s . This , howeve r , is where great care needs to be 
taken on the part of the researc h e r . General i s a t i o n s are usually made by research e r s using 
quant i t a t i v e metho d s in their res ear c h , and even then only when very strict parame t e r s have 
been set up in the resear c h design , such as randomi s a t i o n of the subjec t s and carefu l 
atte nt i o n to validi t y and relia b i l i t y issues . Howeve r , Bassey ( 1999 ) introd u c e d the idea of 
?fuzz y gene ra l i sa t i o ns ? into educat i on a l case study research. These generalising statements 
are suggested  by the results, and although the researcher  cannot be certain that his or her 
findin g s are comple t e l y valid or reliab l e , (s)h e can make such fuzzy generalisations so as 
to gener a t e limit e d claims as the basis for fu ture resea r c h . Stake himse l f prefe r s to limit 
54 
case study resear c h to finding out as much as  possib l e about the case or cases under study, 
with the ?empha s i s on unders t a n d i n g the case itself ? (1995 p. 8), though he also accept s 
that asser t i on s can resul t from the under s t a ndings, and possibly even  the modi fica t i o n of 
generali s a t i o n s . 
As I will show in s ubsequent chapters, my own study  gives a detailed look at both 
what and how student s learn about astron o my in  science centres, and fits with Stake?s 
descri p t i o n of a collect i v e case study. In addi t i on , in Chapt er 9 I s how how some of my 
findi n g s resul t in limit ed clai ms . These claims have implica t i o n s for the study sites and 
student s who partici p a t e d in my study, as well as other scienc e centre s and student s more 
general l y and therefo r e form fuz zy genera l i sa t i o n s based on my colle ct i v e case . My 
collective case consisted of a total of 34 students from four schools who visited either the 
Johannesburg Planetarium or Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory. The research 
instru me n t s I used and how I selecte d the st udy sites and students are described in the 
followi n g section s . 
3.3  Research Instruments 
T h e princi p a l data collec t i o n device s I used  were Personal Meaning Maps drawn by 
participants, field notes taken during observ a t i o n of the partici p a n t s during their visit to the 
study site, and pre- and post-vi sit interviews with the part icipants using a structured 
inter v i e w sched u l e . In a ddition, field notes were taken duri n g the visit s to the schoo l s , and 
the class visits to the study sites were audi o-r e c o r d e d (planet a r i u m ) or video-r e c o r d e d 
(HartRAO ) . 
In the early stages of the study, I cons idered using questionnaires to elicit 
infor ma t i o n from the partic i p a n t s , along the lines of ma ny resear c h e r s in the area of 
astronomy education (e.g. Baxter, 1989; Jarm an & McAleese, 1996; Kikas, 1998b; 
Lemmer et al . , 2003; Trumpe r , 2001a) . I develo p e d such a questi o n n a i r e informe d by the 
literature (see Appendix A), and administered it on a pilot ba sis to school students visiting 
the planet a r i u m and to approx i ma t e ly 90 students at two township 6  schools. The results of 
this pilot survey demons t r a t e d  that severa l of the questi ons were not properly understood 
by the studen t s . Many studen t s left some of th e ?short answer ? questi o n s blank and guesse d 
                                                 
 
6  In South Africa, the term town s h ip usua lly refe r s to the (ofte n under d ev e lop e d) urban resid e n tial areas that, 
under Apar th e id , were rese rv ed for non- wh ites (Afr ic a n s, Colou r ed s and Indian s ) who liv e d near or work ed 
in areas that were designated  ?wh ite-on ly? (W ik ip ed ia, see 
http ://en . wik iped ia.org /wik i/Town sh ip_ %28 So u th _Africa%29 ) 
55 
at what was expec t e d in other s . These resu lt s echoed the ?empty senten c e s ? referr e d to by 
Wagenschein (in Engestr?m, 1993). It is highl y likel y that the stude n t s? abili t y to 
underst a n d the questio n s (or lack thereof) had a part to play in their diffic u l t y with them. 
Sever a l resea r c h e r s in South e r n Afric a have sh own how langu a g e issue s have a role to play 
in learning. Rollnick and colleagues (Blo ck & Rollnick, 2003; Rollnick, 2000) have 
investigated how second language  learne r s acquir e the langua ge of science, while other 
resear c h e r s such as Setati and colleag u e s (S etati, 2003; Setati, Ad le r , Reed & Bapoo, 2002) 
have examined how code-switching and discou r s e - s pe c i fi c talk infl u e nc e the learn i n g and 
teachi n g of mathe ma t i c s and scienc e . Langua g e  was not a focus of my study, and I decide d 
that the deepe r , probi n g natur e of inter vi e w s would be a better way of exploring students? 
knowl e d g e and unders t a n d i n g of issues in basic astro n o my . In additi o n, it appear e d that in 
doing a study based on questionnaires, I would be replica t i n g previo u s  research such as 
that critiqued in Chapter 2. Fo r these reasons I chose to use methods more appropr i a t e to 
gatherin g data related to my researc h questi o n s , and decide d on a combinat i o n of Personal 
Meaning Mapping and intervie w i n g . 
3.3.1  Personal Meaning Mapping 
Personal Meaning Mapping (PMM) is a techni que developed specif i ca l l y for muse u m 
learnin g , in which a subject ? s knowled g e a nd views about a particu l a r subject are 
invest i g a t e d prior to the subj ect enterin g the mus eum and ag ain after the visit (Falk , 2003) . 
Speci fi c a l l y , PMM is carr i e d out in the follo w i n g manne r : 
1.  P r i o r to the visit to the museu m, the subje c t is given a sheet of paper, in which a 
word or phrase is written in the centre. Th e subject is then asked to write or draw 
anythi n g that comes to mind in relati o n to the word or phrase. This can be factual 
infor ma t i o n , ideas , belie fs , or any other rela ted opinion s , and is written in a specifi c 
colour on the paper (e.g. blue). 
2.  T h e inves t i ga t o r then has a short intervi e w with the subject , and, investi g a t e s the 
ideas the subjec t has alread y writt e n on the paper, record in g the subject? s 
elabo r a t i o n of their ideas in a different colour in k from the origina l (e.g. red) 
3.  A ft e r the visit , the subject is given their origina l paper, and asked to make changes 
or addition s to what they have already writte n on the paper. Accordin g to Falk 
(pers. comm.) and Luke (pers. comm.) , it is crucia l that the origin a l paper is given 
back to the subje c t , rather than aski ng them to fill a new one. It ensur e s that they do 
not feel that the investigat o r is ?wasti n g their time? by asking them to repeat what 
they have already done, and it allows them  to alter their origina l ideas. These 
correcti o n s and addition s are made us ing another colour  ink (e.g. green). 
56 
4.  F i n a l l y , the investi g a t o r carri es out another intervie w , based on the alterat i o n s and 
addition s carried out in step 3. The inve sti g a t o r writes these (again using the 
subject? s own words) in a diffe r e n t colour ink (e.g. yellow ) . 
 
A sa mple PMM is shown in Figure 3.1. 
 
Figure 3.1 A Personal Meaning Map constructed by Bhekiwe 
T h e techni q u e is based on the concep t maps developed by Novak and collaborators in the 
1980s (Novak & Gowin, 1984). In concept mappi n g , a subject is taught how to map out 
their own understa n d i n g of concep t s on a sheet of paper, and relate concep t s to each other 
with appropr i a t e connec t o r s . In the analys i s of concept mappin g , there is sometime s a 
?corre c t ? conce p t map, drawn by an expert, against whic h the subject?s map can be 
compar e d and scored . Much of the concept map analy si s that has been devel o p e d over the 
past 20 years is based on this  type of compar i s o n (McClu r e et al . , 1999) and it has proved a 
useful techni q u e for both pedagog y and the study of concept u a l develo p me n t , especi a l l y at 
the school and terti ar y educa t i o n leve l . There has been a number of variants of concept 
mappi n g since the techn i q u e was first deve lop e d by Novak. Techniq u e s used by Morine-
 D e r s h i me r (1993) and Leinhar d t and Gregg (2002)  are probab l y the closes t to PMM. In a 
57 
study of concept u a l change , Morine - D e r s h i me r  asked studen t teache r s to make a concept 
map depicti n g their view of the importa nt compone n t s of teacher prepara t i o n by providi n g 
the phrase ?teac h e r planni n g ? . Two semest e r s late r, the studen t s repeat e d the task, and then 
compa r e d their post- c ou r s e map with their origina l map. Leinhar d t and Gregg used a 
simil a r met ho d with pre-s e r v i c e teach e r s visiting a museum. Ot her mapping techniques 
have been used in educat i o n a l resear c h , such  as ?flow diagr ams ? (Davi d o w i t z & Rolln i c k , 
2005) and ?vee diagra ms ? (Trowb r i d g e & Wande r s e e , 1998).  
Critiq ue s of concep t mappi n g have been made by Kagan (1990) and Ruiz-P r i mo 
and Shavelso n (1996). Kagan noted that they were used to asse ss short-t e r m change rather 
than long-t e r m gain and remark e d that studie s often compa r e d subjec t maps with a target 
?mast e r ? map. Many studi e s made claims that  the map reflect s an individ u a l ? s actual 
cogniti v e structu r e , while Kagan conside r e d that the maps may reflec t their abili t y to 
?reprod u c e the structu r e of th e discipline? (p 451) rather than show real changes in 
stude n t s? cogni t i ve struc t u r e s . Ruiz- P r i mo a nd Shavels o n (1996) sounded warnings about 
using concep t maps for assess me n t purpos e s , an d stressed the need for furt her research on 
the relat i o n sh i p betwe e n the maps and stude nt s ? cogni t i ve sche ma . Apart from the fact that 
I used PMMs to demons t r a t e short- t e r m rather  than long-te r m gain, these critici s ms do not 
apply to my study, as I used the ma ps principa lly as a basis for further questioning rather 
than analys i n g their struct u r e . 
One key differe n c e between many analyse s  of concept mappin g and PMM is that 
there is no ?corre c t ? map develo p e d at any stage, agains t which the PMM is scored . In fact 
Falk (2003) maint a i n s that such  a form of analysi s would be  counter to the philosophy of 
PMM in the context of museum learnin g . For Fal k, there is no ?correct? answer or series of 
answe r s that a museu m visit or can be expec t e d to come up with in re lat i o n to their visit . 
Unlike the school classro o m, or the univers i t y lecture, where the students would be 
expec t e d to learn parti c ul a r sc ien t i fi c conce p t s or facts , the learn i n g which takes place in 
museums is person a l , contex t - bound and idiosyncratic. A PMM is  there f o r e an indiv i d u a l ? s 
perso n a l const r u c t of whate v e r learn i n g took pl ace as a result of their visit . As Perso na l 
Meaning Mapping is a relativ e l y new techniq u e , and has mainly been carried out by Falk 
and collabo r a t o r s , no analysi s  or evaluati o n of the techni que has yet been published. In 
making use of the techniq u e , I comme nt on its usefuln e s s in Chapter 9. 
In my study, the enviro n me n t for data colle ct i o n was quite differe n t to many studies 
using PMM. Like Luke (1998), my data was collected in the school classrooms of the 
58 
select e d partic i p a n t s in the study. I initia l l y gained permis s i on of the school Principal and 
releva n t class teache r s , and obtai n e d infor me d  conse n t from the stude nt s and their paren t s 
(see sectio n 3.5). I then addres s e d the student s in cla ss and explain e d that I am researc h i n g 
their forthcoming visit to one of the study si tes. After handing out blue pens to each 
student, I explained that  I wanted them to write whatev e r  they think about when seein g the 
words in the centre of the sheet of paper. Befo re giving them the paper, I then showed an 
examp l e on the chalk b oa r d , using the word ?Joh annesburg?. I asked th e class what things 
came into their heads when they saw that wo rd on the middle of a piece of paper. Using 
examp l e s fr om the class , I then wrote their sugge s t i o n s on the chalkb o a r d , linkin g the 
words they sugges t e d to the centra l word ?J ohan n e s b u r g ? , or to words they have alread y 
put forward). 
 
Figure 3.2 Example of initial PMM drawn on chalkboard 
Once I had answered questions, and considered  that stude n t s had got the idea of the 
technique, I would hand out the PMM sheet that I had prep ared in advance for the study. 
The ?promp t ? words in the middl e of this sheet were ?spac e , stars and plane t s? . Falk 
recomme n d s that thoroug h piloti n g of the promp t is necessary (Falk 2003), and I did this in 
one of my pilot schools , using a combin a t i o n of words inclu d i n g space, Earth  and stars 
before decidin g on the final wording , whic h elicit e d the most fruitf u l respon s e s . 
I then asked the stude n t s to write what they could tell me about these words . I stres se d the 
follow i n g , that:  
59 
? E v e n if they were not sure about a parti c u l ar issue , they shoul d feel free to write about 
it. 
? T h i s was not a test.  
? T h e y could use words in their home language if they wanted. 
? T h e y could do drawings . 
? T h e y could write about th eir feelin g and beliefs 
I then gave the stude nt s time to comp l e t e the PMM. This varied from about 5 minutes, to a 
maxi mu m of about 30 minutes . Most student s would complet e the map within 15 to 20 
minut e s . In order to ensur e anony mi t y , I wrote a number on the PMM as each student 
compl e t e d it, and compi l e d a class list with the studen t s ? name s and the PMM number s . I 
could then cross refere nc e each studen t agai ns t their own PMM, but anyone seeing a map 
would not be able to ident i fy which stude n t had compl e t e d it. As they compl e t e d their 
PMMs, stude n t s hande d them to me. I then sele cte d which studen t s I wanted to intervi e w , 
as described in Section 3.4.2. 
3.3.2  Interview Schedules 
S e v e r a l resea r c h e r s in the area of both astron o my educat i o n (e.g. Summe r s & Mant, 1995) 
and museum learni n g (Ander s o n et. al. 2000) ha ve found that interviewing subjects can 
elici t more infor ma t i o n from them, as well as allowing probing, through which the salience 
of the subject s ? belief s can be examine d mo re carefu l l y . In the light of this and my 
exper i e n c e with pilot i ng a que stionnaire, I decided to de si g n a struc t u r e d inter v i e w 
schedule which would allow me to probe student s ? thinkin g more deep ly, both prior to and 
after their visit to the study site. The resul ti n g intervi e w schedul e s were piloted with a 
small group of students in an  independent school East of  Johannesburg visiting the 
Planetarium during August 2003. Further develo pme n t of the intervi e w schedu l e s result e d 
in four diffe ri n g versio n s , two pre-vis i t a nd two post-visit, (Appendices B and C), each 
tailored to the study site the studen t s were due to visit.  
3.3.3  The pre-visit interview schedule 
Each pre-visit interview schedule (Appendix B) is divided into four sections, A to D. 
Section A, identic a l in both st udy site versions, is introdu c t o r y , dealing with informe d 
conse n t and demog r a p h i c infor ma t i o n about th e interviewee. Section B, also common to 
both version s , concern s the propos e d visit, and what the stude nt s expect from the visit. 
60 
Resea r c h exami n i n g the effec t of orien t i n g st uden t s regard i n g ?what to  expect? during their 
visit to a museum or science centre (Ande rson & Lucas, 1997; Griffin & Symi ngton, 1997; 
Kubot a & Olstad , 1991) has shown that studen t s  benef i t both cogni t i v e l y and attit u di na l l y 
from such orien t a t i o n . The S ectio n B questi o n s were develo p e d using methods describe d in 
such research . 
The Sectio n C questi o n s diffe r accor d i n g to which study site is due to be visite d . 
During the early months of the study prior to formal data co llection, I visited each study 
site several times, observing groups  of students as they particip a t e d in the ac tivities. I took 
field notes during these visits, and also audi o- or video- r e c o r d e d the procee d i n g s . I then 
analysed the astronomy content that each of th e study sites was aimi ng to impar t to their 
visitor s , and identif i e d a number of key con cepts that a visitor to the study site was 
exposed to. These concepts are shown in Ta ble 3.1, and are developed into Big Ideas in 
astron o my in Chapte r 5. 
Table 3.1 Key concepts in astronomy identified at each study site 
Planetarium HartRAO 
S t a r s and plane t s in night sky Sun and stars : si mil a r i t i e s and diffe r e n c e s 
Solar sy stem: what it is, its shape Sun temp era t u r e , other sun facts 
Sun and its movem e n t acro s s sky Sun and its movem e n t acro s s sky 
Relat i v e size of the sun and moon Relat i v e size of the sun and moon 
Stars: what they are, how far away Stars: what they are 
Moon phas e s Moon phas e s 
 Satell i t e and satell i t e dish 
 Gravity , with probes 
Astro n o my in the news Astro n o my in the news 
 
S e c t i o n C of each pre-vi s i t interv i e w asks ques ti o n s relat e d to the content shown in Table 
3.1, accordi n g to whether student s were visitin g the Planeta r i u m or HartRAO . For 
example , questi o n C2 asked at the planeta r i um was ?Wha t is the sola r syst e m? What 
[thing s ] does it consis t of? What shape is it? How do you know? ? Question C5 asked at 
HartRAO was ?Stars at night  look like pinprick s of li ght . Wh y? What are stars ? ? 
Finall y , Sectio n D of each pre-vi s i t inte rvie w (common to both study sites) asks 
questio n s relate d to student s ? intere s t s a nd attitud e s toward s sc hool, recreation and 
astron o my , as well as studen t s ? belief s rega rding extra-terrestrial life, astrology and 
61 
religio n . Studies of peoples ? attitu d e s toward s phenome n a have been carried out by 
psycho l o g i sts for decade s , and have tended to use quant i t a t i v e measur e s (Crawl e y & 
Koballa, 1994; Osborne et al ., 2003). More recently, there ha s been interest in school 
student s ? attitud e s towards va riou s aspect s of scienc e , includ i n g astron o my (Jarma n & 
McAlee s e , 1996; Jarvi s & Pell, 2002, 2005). However, these studi e s have also been mainly 
quantit a t i v e in design, and their instrume n t s were not helpful in my study. The questions in 
Section D were adapted slightly over the course  of the study, as my intervi e w i n g techni q u e 
impro v e d . The intent i o n of the Secti o n D quest i o n s was to ident i fy wheth e r a stude n t ? s 
attitud e s or interes t s could be related to his or her learnin g at the study site. The pre-vis i t 
intervi e w schedul e was piloted in a priv ate mission school on the East side of 
Johannes b u r g , and question s were revised on the basis of the responses by students. 
3.3.4  The post-visit interview schedule 
A ft e r the visit , stude n t s were asked questi o n s (see Appendi x C) relat e d to their visi t to the 
study site, as well as a repeat of the content questions asked in the pre-vis i t intervi e w . 
Section A questions, common to both study sites, were again relate d to informe d consen t 
and identif i c a t i o n of the student . Accord i n g to Anderso n et. al. (2000), very little research 
has been carri e d out on post- v i s i t activi t i e s carrie d out by teache r s who take groups of 
studen t s on visits to scienc e centre s . The S ection B question s made a brief follow-u p on 
whether the students knew where they had vi sited , and why. The Section C questio n s were 
ident i c a l to those asked in the pre-v i s i t inte rvie w , and we re differen t for the two study 
sites . Final l y , quest i o n s in Sectio n D relate d to their attit u d e s towards the visit, and 
attempt e d to identi f y me mor i e s from the vi sit which could be construed as learning. In 
severa l articl e s and chapte r s , Falk and Dierking (1992, 1997, 2000) have indicate d that it is 
better to try and identif y learnin g not by asking ?What did you learn in the Science 
Centre? ? (e.g. Cox-Petersen, Marsh, Kisiel  & Melber, 2003) but instead by asking for 
examples of aspects of the visit which interested  stude nt s , or that they told to others . The 
questions on learning in Section D draw upon me thods used by Falk and Dierkin g (1997). 
3.3.5  Interviews 
I condu c t e d each intervi e w at the school s vis ite d . After obtaini n g permis s i o n from the 
Princi p a l and releva n t class teache r s , and arra nging for consent forms to be signed by the 
studen t s and their parent s / g u a r d i a n s (see sectio n 3.5), I reques t ed the teacher to arrange a 
room in whi ch I could interv i e w any selecte d  studen t . The room varied accord i n g to the 
62 
school visited, but was usually an office or clas sroom not being used by others at the time. 
The inter v i e w e e and I were ther efor e accorded privacy, and I wa s able to ask quest i o ns and 
audio - re c o r d the inter vi ew . Inter v i ew s follo w e d a standa r d i s e d format , in which I would 
first confi r m infor me d conse n t , and ask biographical questions to try and set the subj ec t at 
ease. I would then go through the interview sc hedule from Section A to D, ending the 
interv i e w with questi o ns on the subjec t s PMM th at they had previ o u s l y writt e n for me. At 
the end of the intervie w , I would thank the stud en t , and reques t him or her to call the next 
select e d stude n t to come to the room. All in terv i e w s were audio- r e c o r de d on to casset t e 
tape, and transc r i b e d to a word-p r o c e s s o r file, which could th en be analysed.  
3.4  Data Collection 
3.4.1  Selection of the Study Sites 
D e s p i t e Sout h Afric a ? s incre a s i n g promi n e n c e as a hub for astron o my and space scien c e , 
there are relati v el y few sites promo t i n g astro n om y educa t i on in the countr y . The main ones 
include planeta r i u ms in Johanne s b u r g and Cape Town, the South African Astronomi cal 
Observat o r y in Cape Town, the newly ope ned South African Large Telescope in 
Sutherl a n d , the Boyden Observ a t o r y in Bloe mf o n t e i n and astron o my - r e l a t e d sectio n s of 
museu ms and scien c e centr e s . 
The logistic s of my study m eant that I prefer r e d to work in the provin c e based 
around Johann e s b u r g and Pretor i a . Within this region , the number of sites that combin e 
both astron o m y and informa l learn i n g was limite d . I could have chosen sites of intere s t for 
amate u r astro n o me r s , such as the Obser v atory in Johannesbur g , or other private 
observ a t o r i e s open to the public on a limite d ba sis . Howev e r , my princ i p a l inter es t was 
combin i n g infor ma l learni n g with the school educati o n system, hence I needed to locate 
sites which allow e d or encou r a g e d acces s for school groups . On this basis, one obviou s 
choice was the Johanne s b u r g Planeta r i u m, located  on the premi s e s of the Unive r s i t y of the 
Witwatersrand, and run by the un ivers i t y as part of its science aw areness outreach 
progra m me . Large number s of school groups vi sit the planetar i u m on a monthly basis, and 
it appea r e d to be an ideal site to base my study. When I approa c h e d the Direct o r for 
permis s i o n to use the planet a r i u m as one of  my study sites, I was welcome d , and given 
permi s s i o n , with the provi s o that I feed back the result s of my resear c h to the staff. 
Originally conceived in 1956 as pa rt of the celeb r a tions for the 70 th  anniversary of the 
foundi n g of Johann e s b u r g , the planet a r i u m wa s transfe r r e d by the Ci ty Council to the 
63 
Univer s i t y of the Witwat e r s r a n d and opened in 1960, housing a recondit i o n e d star 
project o r from Ha mburg , Germa ny (J ohann e s b u r g Planeta r i u m, 1999). 
The disadvantage of the planetarium is that  its educat i ve experi e nc e is in the form 
of a present a t i o n only which, even if made inter a ct i v e by the prese n t er quest i o n i ng the 
audie n c e , is a mainl y visua l and audit o r y experience. In looking fo r an additional out-of-
 s c h o o l astro n o my exper i e n c e , I met an as tron o me r fr om the Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio 
Astron o my Observ a t o r y . He descri b e d their science awareness programme and explained 
that they encour a g e schoo l s to visit the tele s c ope and adjac e n t visit o r s? centr e . Since it is 
locate d in Gauten g provin c e less than 60 km from Johanne s b u r g , HartRAO appeare d to be 
an ideal addit i o na l site at which to base my study . HartR A O is an examp l e of a ?large 
objec t with a small mu seu m attac h e d ? (Gre gor y & Miller, 1998 p. 204) similar to the 
Jodre l l Bank Visit or Cent r e attac h e d to the radio teles c o p e in the UK (Chap l i n & Graham-
 S mi t h , 1992). Gregor y and Miller consid e r such types of museum as typical of the later 
20th century, as artefacts of  science and technology whic h now concern themsel v e s 
increasingly with both the ?cosmic and microsc opic?. This contrasts wi th previou s decades , 
in which the emphasi s was on relativ e l y small artefa c t s in large  museu ms .  
Situat e d in a hollow in the Magali e sb e r g hills West of Pretori a , HartRA O was built 
in the early 1960s as a trackin g statio n for th e United States? NASA Apollo and associat e d 
missio n s . Even in those early days, there was a for m of ?scienc e awaren e s s ? progra m m e 
run by NASA, which included lectures to inte rested groups and visits by schools. The 
facility was handed over to the South Afri ca n govern me n t in 1975, and was managed by 
the Nationa l Institu t e of Telecom mu n i c a t i o n s Re se a r c h , a part of the Counc i l for Scien t i fi c 
and Industr i a l Researc h . For the next few y ears, HartRAO accept ed one visit per month 
from schoo l s and unive r s i t i e s . In 1988, respo n s i b i l i t y for the observ a t o r y  was transf e r r e d to 
the Foundation for Research and Development (now the NRF), and in 1990 a me mb er of 
staff was recruited to spend 50% of her time on a science awareness programm e . The 
facilit y is current l y ma nage d by the Nati ona l Researc h Foundat i o n (NRF) as a radio 
telescop e , and has expanded its science awaren e s s progra m me consid e r a b l y. The 
program me encour a g e s visits by schools , clubs  and the public to learn about astron o my , 
space and the resear c h carrie d out using the radio teles co p e . In addit i o n , the scien c e 
awarene s s staff (two full-ti me educato r s , under the guidance of the programme manager) 
run teache r s ? worksh o p s and partic i p a t e in re levant science communication events in the 
region. (Gaylard , undated) 
64 
A visit to the planeta r i u m typic a l l y invol v e s a 50 to 80 minute ?show? in which an 
astron o my educat o r demons t r a t e s the sola r system, astron o mi c a l distan c e s and an 
indicat i o n of the night sky constel l a t i o n s vi sib l e the same night. A visit to HartRA O 
typica l l y lasts up to 4 hours, and entails intera c t ive activities such as water rockets, whisper 
dishes and star spinnin g , and de monstrations of the solar system and the radio telescope. I 
descri be the experi e n c e s of a student visiting each of the s ites in detail in Chapt e r 4. 
3.4.2  Selection of the Participants 
In the original research design for my study, I planne d to exam in e how both studen t s and 
their teachers experie n c e d astron o mi c a l phenome n a  at a scien ce cent r e . I felt this 
import a n t , as much of the lite rature related to astronomy edu cation showed that teachers? 
unders t a n d i n g of astron o mi c a l experiences is ofte n as limited as that of their students 
(Summers & Mant, 1995; Trundle et al ., 2002) and it is unlikely that the situation in South 
Africa is differe n t . As the re search proposal prog ressed, and the focus mo ved towards the 
infor ma l learn i n g of astro n o my and ?muse u m lear ning?, the role of teac her s was less of a 
priority , but I still consider e d it impor t a n t , as many stud ies have shown (e.g. Anderson & 
Lucas, 1997; Griffi n & Symi ngt on, 1997; Storksdieck, 2004). However, once I began to 
examine how school visits to my study sites we re being arranged, it be came apparent that 
only in a very limited number of cases would it be possibl e to collec t data on teache r s , as 
well as the stude n t s . This is becau s e in most insta nc e s , the vi sit being arranged was 
unrela t e d to the actual teachi n g curren t l y ta king place in the school, and the accompan y i n g 
teache r was often not teachi n g the subjec t of as tro n o my to the stude n t s parti c i p a t i n g in the 
visit . I therefo r e decid e d that it was more appr o p r i at e to asses s the le arning of the students, 
who were full partici p a n t s in every visit to a study site, rather than include the teacher s , 
who rarely participated in  the study site activit i e s . 
The select i o n of school s and grade level of  the student s for the study was careful l y 
consi d e r e d . Revie w s of museu m and scie nc e centre learni n g (F alk & Dierkin g , 2000; 
Rennie & McClafferty, 1996) show that studies ha ve been made with participants across a 
wide age range, from pre-sc hool to adults. Revi ew s of astron o my learni n g (Table 2.1) 
show a si mila r wide range of partic i p a n t s , but includ e fewer adults , and larger number s at 
the level of college and univers i t y student s (Bailey & Slater, 2003; Dunlop, 2000). I chose 
grade s 7 to 9 (mainl y 13- to 15-yea r - o l d s ) for my study because these grades form the 
Senior Phase of the General Educati o n a nd Training (GET) Band of South Africa?s new 
65 
Curric u l u m 2005. This curric u l u m was phased in from 1996 to repla c e the ?inte r i m core 
syllab u s ? , in place since 1994. Grade 7 is the fi nal year of primary school , while (in most 
schools ) grades 8 and 9 for m the first two y ears of high school, so my sa mple spanned the 
primar y - s e c o n d a r y school border . 
My origin a l intent i o n was to select school s princ i p a l l y from the publi c syste m of 
school i n g in South Africa , as they form the bulk of the school s in South Africa , and any 
finding s from the study would be most valuabl e  if relevan t to them. Howeve r , having 
chosen the two study sites as being ideal locations at wh ich astronom y and informal 
learning are combined, and havi ng a limited time fo r data collec t i o n , I was restricted to 
school s that had alread y plann e d to visit one of these two sites. In order to select school s , I 
contac t e d each study site and obtai n e d lists of schoo l s with class e s in grade s 7 to 9 visiti n g . 
I then approa c h e d the teache r s and princi p a l s  in these school s to enquir e whethe r they 
would allow me to conduct my research with  students in their schools. This usually 
entailed visiting the school, expl ai nin g my resear c h proj ec t , a nd leavi n g them with a set of 
informa t i o n sheet s and infor me d conse n t forms .  After a period of days , I would contact the 
school again, and, if permis s i on was granted (it was in every case), I would make 
arrang e me n t s to visit the school to conduc t th e first phase of my study: the admi ni s t r a t i o n 
of Persona l Meanin g Maps. 
As a result of this ?conveni e n c e samp ling ? (Cohen & Manion, 1994) of schools and 
classes , the schools shown in Table 3.2 for med  the basis of my study, from which studen t s 
were selected . 
66 
Table 3.2 Participant schools in the study 
Name 7  Type of School Location Visit to Classes 
L o u r d e s Girls 
Scho o l 
Indep e n d e n t Catho l i c 
Girl s ? Scho o l 
Subur b, West 
Rand 
Plane t a r i u m 1 Gr 8 
Revel a t i o n 
Scho o l 
Smal l inde p e n d e n t 
Chris t i a n schoo l 
Subur b, East 
Rand 
Plane t a r i u m 1 mixed Gr 
7-9 
Achiev e me n t 
Scho o l 
Smal l inde p e n d e n t 
schoo l 
Subur b, West 
Rand 
HartRA O Gr 7 
St. Augus t i n e? s 
Scho o l 
Inde p e n d e n t Miss i o n 
Scho o l 
Town East of 
Pretor i a 
HartR A O 2 Gr 7 
Balfou r Fores t 
Scho o l 
Publ i c scho o l Subu r b, 
Johan n e s b u r g 
HartR A O 2 Gr 7 
Boka mo s o 
Scho o l 
Publi c schoo l Towns h i p West 
of Preto r i a 
HartRA O Gr 7&8 
The select i o n of studen t s who compl e t e d the PM M was straight f o r w a r d , and is describe d in 
section 3.3.1. In total I obtained 145 PMMs fr om student s in seven school s . The actual 
number was closer to 170, but some of these studen t s either did not return their informe d 
conse n t forms , or did not actua l l y go on the visi t, and were theref o r e not used in the study. 
My study being princip a l l y qualita t i v e , I needed  to selec t a small e r number of student s to 
intervi e w , both with referen c e to their PMM and in relatio n to thei r astronomy knowledg e 
(section 3.3.2). Selection of whic h students to interview needed a set of criteria which I set 
up based on my researc h questi o n s and on similar studie s in the literature (e.g. Anderson et 
al . , 2000).  
My research questio n 5 asks ?How do stude nts ? interest s and prior knowledg e affect 
the learning experien c e of a school visit?? On e of my criter i a for select i n g studen t s for 
interview was therefore identifying students who had particul a r l y strong interests or beliefs 
which may have affecte d their learni n g at th e study site. Simil a r l y , if a studen t eithe r 
showed consid e r a b l e prior knowle d g e or c onvers e l y lacked any prior knowled g e in the 
area of astron o my , I could select them. Ande rs o n et al . (2000) , select e d studen t s who 
provide d a range of scienti f i c and alter n at i v e conce p t i o ns in their concept maps, as well as 
roughly equal representation of boys and girls; criteria that I al so applied in my selection. 
In addit i o n to this, where the classes I sele ct e d includ e d both black and white studen t s , I 
                                                 
 
7  In ord er to main tain anon ymi ty, all sch oo l na mes are fictitio u s. 
67 
attempt e d to obtain repre s e n t a t i o n from all popula t i o n group s 8 . A further criter i o n used by 
Anderso n et al . (2000 ) was the recomme n d a t i o n by the teache r that a studen t would be able 
and prepare d to communi c a t e effect i v e l y with th e resea r c h team. I consi d e r e d that this ma y 
introdu c e possib l e bias into the selecti o n , and preferred to obtain a ?veto recommendat i on? 
from the teach e r in cases where s/he consi d e r e d the studen t unsuit a bl e for interv i e w . 
Selectio n of my final research sampl e of 34 students is described in section 3.4.2. 
3.4.3  Triangulation 
T r i a n g u l a t i o n refers to the process of obtaini n g two or more for ms of evidenc e to improve 
the validity of the findings (Cohen & Manion, 1994). I collecte d  evidence in the form of 
PMMs, interv i e w s and field notes, as well as an  audio or video recor d of the class visit . 
The process of data collect i o n is summari s e d  in Figure 3.3, and issues of validity are 
discuss e d in section 3.6. 
 
Figure 3.3 The sequence of events in data collection 
                                                 
 
8  In South Africa, repo r ting of statistics often inclu d es refe ren c e to black , Indian , colour ed and white 
indiv id u a ls in orde r to demo nstr a te redr e s s. 
68 
3.5  Ethical issues 
I s s u e s of ethics are promin e n t in all the researc h literat u r e (e.g. Cohen and Manion 1994, 
Henning 2004). In my study, prior to starting my data colle c t i o n , I had to satisfy the 
Universi t y ? s Huma n Ethics Research Committ e e (Non-M e d i c a l ) that I had done everything 
possible to ensure proper informed cons en t , anonymi t y and confid e n t i a l i t y for the 
participants. To start with I obtained the perm iss i o n of my two study sites to use them in 
the study, and also to use the real names of th e sites in repor t i n g on the researc h . Prior to 
starting data collecti o n in a sc hool I provided all the research particip a n t s (student s ) with 
information sheets on the proposed study, as we ll as consent forms (see Appendix D) for 
themse l v e s and their parent s to sign if they  wished to partici p a t e in the study. I did not 
consid e r it necess a r y to obtain infor me d cons en t from the teache r s , as they were not 
participants in the study. 
Table 3.3 shows the retu rn rate of infor med consent fo r the 6 schools in which the study 
was conducte d . 
Table 3.3 Informed consent returns 
Fictitious Name Code Classes No. of 
students 
approached 
No. of students 
who consented 
% who 
consented 
L o u r d e s Girl s Scho o l scf 1 Gr 8 21 16 76 
Revel a t i o n Schoo l sri 1 mixed Gr 
7-9 
8 8 100 
Achi e v e m e n t Scho o l vho Gr 7 16 14 88 
St. Augu s t i n e? s Scho ol sth 2 Gr 7 45 32 71 
Balf o u r Fore s t Scho o l swo 2 Gr 7 75 37 49 
Boka mo s o Scho o l tsw Gr 7&8 18 17 94 
 
T h e r e was a relat i v el y high level of retur n of infor me d cons e n t from the majo r i t y of the 
schools. As could be expected , when the num ber of students approach e d was relative l y 
small (25 or fewer ) , the rate of conse n t was highe s t . In all cases I explai n e d the issue of 
infor me d conse n t to the stude n t s , but did obt ai n assist a n c e from the teach e r s to colle c t in 
the forms. In two cases, I found that the teach er s wanted to coerce the student s to sign, for 
examp l e by telli n g them that they would not be able to go on the trip unles s the infor me d 
consent form was signed and returned. I expl ai n e d to the teacher s who did this that this 
69 
was neither necessar y nor ethical, but they did not always appear convinced by my 
argume n t and I did not interv e n e furthe r betwee n the teache r and studen t s in this matter . 
The consent forms were provided only in E nglish . I conside r e d th is appr o p r i at e as 
all the school s which parti c i p a t e d in the st udy used English as their language of learning 
and teaching. Of the students in t ervi e w e d , 38% (n=34) said th ey used English as their 
home langua g e , and a furthe r 38% used it as  an additional language  spoken at home. So 
from the point of view of the studen t s and their parent s ? unders t a nding and signing the 
for ms , I consi d e r e d that the use of Engli s h was appropri a t e . However, it could be argued 
that in the two publi c schoo l s ,  while the stude n t s ? level of  English was suffici e n t to 
underst a n d the for ms for the purpose of being able to give their infor me d  consen t , this ma y 
not have been the case for their parent s / g u a r d i a n s who were required to sign. Discussi o n 
with the stude n t s howev e r , assur e d me that thei r pare nt s / gua r di a ns level of English was 
suffici e n t to underst a n d what they were signing . 
Although students were told that, both orally  and in writing, they  were entitled to 
revie w the trans c ri p t s of their intervi e w s , I did not actual l y offer this to them onc e the 
inter v i e w s were trans cr i b e d . This was partl y due to the fact that the inter v i e w s were 
trans cr i be d seve r al mont h s afte r the tapes we re made, and also because the logistics of 
arranging a review of the tr anscription would have been difficult. Also, a number of 
quali t a t i v e metho d o l o g y texts note that this ?re spo n d e n t feedba c k ? involv e s power relati o n s 
which make it unlike l y that the interv i e w e e wi ll make signific a n t changes (Univers i t y of 
Huddersf i e l d , 2006). This is particul a r l y releva n t in the case of childre n who have been 
told that the trans c ri p t is a re cord of what they said record e d  in the intervi e w . Howev e r , I 
recogni s e that the fact that the students were not given the opportun i t y to revie w  thei r own 
transcri p t s could be a limitati o n in my study. 
One aspect of ?self? select i o n by student s (or their parent s) is parti c ula r l y worth 
commen t i n g on here, as it demons t r a t e s how the context of a school visit in a develop i n g 
country is very differe n t from those in mo re develop e d contex t s . In schools where the 
socio - ec o n omi c level of the parent s is relat i ve l y low, the issue of affo rd a b i l i t y of visits 
affects student s ? partici p a t i o n in school trips, and therefo r e in my study. In both the public 
school s , as well as in schools which were part of the p ilot study, only students whose 
paren t s can affor d to pay are able to parti c i p ate in school visits. In  effect, this selects out 
the studen t s whose home circums t a n c e s precl u d e s payme n t for visit s to sites such as those 
used in my study, and therefore biases the re sults in favour of stude nts from higher socio-
70 
economi c status homes. Since the entran c e fees ar e subsidi s e d , the biggest  cost of visits to 
either of my study sites is the cost of trans po r t . Unless the school is able to pay for such 
students, which Balfour Forest School did in  the case of some students in my study, 
students whose parents who cannot or are no t prepared to pay ar e excluded from my 
sample . This may well accoun t for less than 50 % of student s / p a r e n t s signing the infor med 
conse n t forms in this schoo l . If a stude n t or parent knows they are unlike l y to be able to 
attend the visit, there is little reason to agree to part ic i p a t e in the st udy. Students from 
Boka moso were in a slightly differen t position , in that they were part of a scienc e club for 
whom the visit was being organis e d . Being a relatively small self-selected group, there 
would have been more incent i v e to sign the conse n t forms . It is impo r t a n t to note that the 
two public schools are represen t a t i v e of the va st majority of schools in South Africa, and 
that, like the student s in Jrene Rahm?s study  in the USA (Rahm, 2004), such student s will 
norma l l y have littl e acces s to science centre s and museums . 
Adler and Lerman (2003) stress that rese arch, particularly in  developing country 
conte x t s , has a multip l i c i t y of respon s i b i l i t i e s to and owners h i p by the partici p a n t s, the 
researcher and his or her community, and the public. My study attemp ted to address these 
issues. Although my research di d not directly benefit the students who participated, I 
consi d e r that it does benefi t fu tur e gener a t i o n s of stude n t s at  the parti ci p a t i n g schoo l s . I 
have kept in touch with the schoo l s , and inten d to make prese n t a t i o n s on schoo l visit s to all 
staff once the study is comple t e . Other indire c t benef i c i a r i e s are teachers and school 
children througho u t South Africa and the region, as I have already presented interi m 
findin g s to confere n c e s , and intend to contin ue this. The princi p a l benefi c i a ri e s of the 
resear c h are the planeta r i u m and HartRAO , who will recei v e copie s of the thesi s and I will 
make prese n t a t i o n s to their educa t o r s an d stres s the impl i c a t i on s for their own 
progr a m me s . In addit i on , I will prese n t my findi n g s to the scien c e centr e commu n i t y in 
South e r n Afric a at their annua l confe r e n c e . In these ways I believ e I have addres s e d issues 
of respon s i b i l i t y to th e participants, the research comm un i t y , and to a lesser extent the 
public. However , aspects of ownersh i p by thes e stakeholders are less clear, and I did not 
identi f y them as priori t i e s when the study was for mul a t e d . While the resear c h acade my 
will have a degre e of owner s h i p as the topic of rese a r c h is impo r t a n t in the infor ma l 
learni n g commun i t y , I need to de vis e ways of great e r owner s h i p by the partic i p a n t s and the 
public in future research projects . 
71 
3.6  Issues of credibility and trustworthiness 
U n l i k e quanti t a t i v e resear c h , a qualit a t i v e study cannot rely solely on the concep t s of 
validi t y and relia b i l i t y as crite ri a for a ssess i ng the qualit y of the researc h . Inst e a d , 
quali t a t i v e resea rc h texts ident i f y a variety of ?qualit y checks ? to determi n e the exte n t to 
which the resear c h can be regard e d as credib l e and trustw o r t h y . The follow i n g sectio n s 
discu s s how I attempt e d to build check s of quali t y into my resea r c h throu g ho u t the 
planning, data collection and analysis.  
3.6.1  Validity 
A great deal has been writte n about the concep t s of validi t y and relia bi l i t y with respe c t to 
qualita t i v e resear c h . Some case study resear c h deals direc t l y with is sues of validity, for 
examp l e Yin (1994 ) recom me n d s using multi p l e case studi e s to impro v e exter n a l validi t y 
and matchin g predic t e d patter n s with empiric a l l y based patte r n s in the data to impro v e 
internal validity. In my own case I asked co nt e n t specia l i s t s to examin e my interv i e w 
schedu l e s for face validi t y , and made change s accord i n gl y . This was not howeve r possib l e 
for the constr u c t i v i s t tool of Person a l Mean in g Mappin g , where the concep t of validi t y has 
little meanin g . Many schola r s who work qualitat i v e l y (e.g. Henning, 2004; Miles & 
Huberma n , 1994) have questi o n e d the use of valid i t y in quali t a t i ve resea r c h as it is a 
const r u ct of the posit i vi s t paradi gm. An alterna t i v e is the conce pt of trust w o r t hi n e s s 
(Linc o l n & Guba, 1985) which refe r s to the exten t to which the reade r can trust the 
findin g s , interp r e t a t i o n s and claims as bei ng ?worth paying attenti o n to, worth taking 
accoun t of?? (p 290). Elliot Mishle r goes even furthe r , and asks whethe r subseq u e n t 
researc h e r s will value the finding s and clai ms enough to use as a basis for their own 
researc h (Mishle r , 1990). Instead of using ?the static propertie s of instruments and scores? 
(p 419) invest i g a t o r s would examin e the met hod s used in the resear c h and base their 
judgeme nt on this. Other resear c h e r s have describ e d the need  for an ?audit trail? in 
qualit a t i v e resear c h which docume nt s the steps taken and decisi o n s made during the 
analy si s to demon s t r a t e how the resea r c he r mo ved from data to the final findi n g s and 
conclus i o n s (Miles & Huberma n , 1994). 
I have attempt e d to lay out in this ch apter the detaile d proced u r e s by which I 
colle c t e d my data, as well as the vario u s assu mp t i o n s I made. Chapte r s 5 and 6 explain the 
proces s e s by which I analys e d my data. More im por t a n t l y , attac h e d to this thesi s is a CD 
contai ni n g the Hermen e u t i c Unit (HU) of the quali t a t i v e analy si s soft w a r e I used to 
72 
analys e all the data (ATLAS . t i versio n 5.2) , which provides my audit trail. The HU 
inclu d e s all prima r y docume n t s (PMMs and trans c r i pt i o n s of intervi e w s ) , coding of these 
docume n t s , code famil i e s , comme n t s on codes and famil i e s , inci d e nta l me mos I made 
while exami n i n g the data as well as a journa l recordi n g all my thought s as the analysi s 
progress e d over the period 2004-200 6 . For example,  a reader can decide to exami ne the 
data on which one of my portrait s in Chapter 7 or 8 is based. If  he or she chooses to look at 
Brend a ? s data they can open the HU, selec t the primary documen t fa mi ly for Brenda, and 
exami n e all the data and how it was coded . Ex tracts from the HU are shown in Appe ndix 
E. 
Toget h e r , these provi d e a satis fa ct or i l y de tailed description of the basis for my 
claims in Chapter s 9 and 10, and in this way satisfy the criteria fo r trustwo r t h i n e s s as 
detaile d by researc h e r s such as Henning (2004) and Opie (2004). 
3.6.2  Reliability 
R e l i a b i l i t y refer s to the abilit y  to replica t e the finding s of th e study if repeat ed by another 
research e r under similar conditi o n s . As with validit y , a qualita tive research approach does 
not lend itsel f to repli c abi l i t y , given the fact that the data are colle ct e d under very speci fi c 
condi t i o n s from a relat i v e l y small numbe r of partic i p a n t s (Bass e y , 1999). For this reason, it 
was not possib l e to use one of the normal crite r i a for reliabi l i t y , which is to examine the 
consi s t e n c y of resul t s in a test over a perio d of  time. Inst e a d , I used the noti o n of inter - r at er 
relia b i l i t y to asses s the degre e to which othe r resea rc h e r s would classi fy my data into 
catego r i e s , and compar e those with my own (Ruiz- P r i mo & Shavel s o n , 1996). As I 
descri b e in Chapte r 6, I asked two collea g ues using my Big Ideas typology to group 
studen t s into catego r i e s . The result s of their classi f i c a t i o n were cons i st e n t with my own, 
and demon s t r at e d that my resul t s in this sect i on of the thesi s are as relia bl e as could be 
expec t e d . 
3.6.3  Reflexivity 
R e fl e x i v i t y or Refl e x i v ene s s is the exten t to which a resear che r is aw are of his or her own 
assumpt i o n s and biases in constr u c t i n g meani n g from the data acquir e d during the resear c h 
proce s s (Ryan & Weisne r , 1997) . In Chapte r 1 I descri b e d the positi o n from which I 
approa c h e d my resear c h, a point which I tried to  be aware of during th e data collec t i on and 
analysi s proce s s e s . As analys i s proce e d e d  I kept a journa l (avai l a bl e withi n the 
Hermen e u t i c Unit of ATLAS. t i attach e d to this thesis ) which shows my though t s and 
73 
inter p r et a t i ons of the data. In these ways I attempt e d to ensur e that bias was kept to a 
mini mu m in the analys i s, and where prese n t th at I explaine d my th inki n g to the reader . 
3.7  Reflection 
M y resear c h method s were design e d to cap ture student s ? prior knowled g e of basic 
astro n o my as well as their relat e d inter e s t s and be lief s before they partic i p a t e d in the visit 
to the scien c e cent r e . I need to accept that th e process of the pre-vis i t PMM and intervi e w , 
as well as my presence on their visit set up a pedagogi cal frame around the visit and 
probabl y height e n e d studen t s ? awaren e s s of th e trip and the topic of astrono my . In this 
sense my data collection could be regar de d as a form of intervention which would be 
absent in most school visits . Al thou g h I tried to remain a non-pa rticipant in the visits made 
by the school classe s to the study sites, at leas t some studen t s would likely have viewed me 
as part of the educa t i ve team based at the science centre. Data provided in subsequent 
chapt e r s needs to be viewed with this limita t i o n in mind, and I will discuss it further in 
Chapte r 9. 
This chapte r has descri b e d my resear c h design and method s of data collec t i o n in 
some detai l in an attem p t to provi d e the r eader with a full account of how the study was 
conduct e d . Chapter 4 provide s a narrati v e of two studen t s ? visits to the study sites to show 
their experi e n c e as partic i p a n t s  in the out-of-school trip. 
74 
Chapter 4 
4  Setting the Scene ? a narrative of the visits 
This chapter shows the reader what it is lik e to visit the sites at which the study 
was based. I decided to present it as findi ngs from the research, in the form of 
narratives of two students? visits. This serves two purposes: to enable the 
reader to experience a visit through the eyes of a child, and for me as 
researcher to share findings from in terviews and observation and in doing so 
?open a window on the mind?(Cortazzi, 1993). 
4.1  Introduction 
N a r r a t i v e is used in the quali t a t i v e resea r c h lite r at u r e to refe r to a vari et y of prose texts , but 
in this study I am using Polkin g h o r n e ? s (1995) description of narrative as a text which is 
organi s e d thema t i c a l l y by plot. In my case the pl ot refers to the experie n c e s that the student 
has during the visit to the astrono m y - b a s e d science centre.  Polking h o r n e identif i e s two 
types of narrative study, name d rather confus i n g l y as ?analys i s of narratives? and ?narrative 
analysi s ? . In the former , a study examin e s pe ople?s stories and life hi stories and comes up 
with categor i e s or groupin g s on the basis of such narratives. In narrative analysis a 
resea r c h e r uses data colle c t e d to produ c e a na rrat i v e or narrat i v e s as the resul t of the study. 
The findin g s prese n t e d in this chapte r repre se n t the latter . 
Dollar d (1935) develo p e d a set of criter i a  which he ?viewed as indispe n s a b l e for 
judging a life history techni que? (p 8). In his paper on narrative configuration, 
Polkinghorne (1995) adapted Dollard?s seven cr iteria as guidelines for narrative analysis 
writin g . I have follow e d Polkin g h o r n e ? s advice a nd attempt e d to use these guide l i n e s in the 
develo p me n t of the narrat i v e in Sectio n s  4.2 and 4.3. The seven guidelin e s can be 
summar i s e d as follow s : 
? Attention must be given to the cultural context  in which the story is  set. The charac t e r s 
in the narr a t i v e intera c t withi n  a set of norms and values de veloped as the result of the 
cultur e in which they exist .  
75 
? T h e protagonists  (main charac t e r s ) in the story should be clearl y descri b e d in terms of 
how they are embodied : age, physic a l featur e s , me ntal capaci t i e s and emotio n a l 
respon s e s are some of the ways in which this can be expressed. 
? Relationships  between the people in the narrat i ve need to be clearl y explai n e d in the 
developme n t of the plot, so that the effect  on the main charac t e r s can be brough t out. 
? A narra t i v e that involv e s a main charac t e r (a s both of mine do) ?needs to concentr a t e 
on the choice s and action s of this centra l  person? (Polking h o r n e 1995 p 17). Such a 
person does not only react to events but also  shape s these event s , and the resea r c he r 
needs to describ e how this interaction occurs . 
? A l l characte r s have a history , and the describer of the narr a t i v e needs to be able to 
relate the protagonist?s ac tion s in relatio n to his or  her past experie n c e s . 
? T h e story that results from narrat i v e analys i s should be time bounde d and presen t e d in 
enoug h detai l to demon s t r a t e that it is a unique description, not merely an averag e 
accoun t abstra c t e d from a series of  observat i o n s of differen t people. 
? Finally, the outcome of the analys i s needs to be plausi b l e , unders t a n d a b l e and 
believab l e , often with a clear conclusi o n whic h is a configuration of the various data 
elemen t s into a well-r o un d e d systema t i c whole. 
 
I n additio n to Dollard ? s criteri a , Polking h o r n e  has developed a further guideline which he 
consi d e rs impor t a nt . This is the need for the res ea r c h e r to share with the reade r the role the 
researc h e r played in the constru c t i o n of th e narrati v e , and how he or she has shaped the 
resulting story (Polkinghorne, 1995) . I consider that this guidelin e is relat e d to  the analysis 
rather than the data, and have attempt e d to do so in this chapte r .  Wolco t t (1988 ) makes 
recommendations for narrative accounts in his story of a ?sneaky ki d?. He suggests that 
?the story should make a point that transc e n d s its modest or igins? , and ?the case mu st be 
partic u l a r , but the implic a t i o n s broad ? (p. 246). In the narrativ e s portraye d , what the 
studen t s experi e n c e before , during and after th e visit s to Harte b e e s t h o e k Radio Astro n o my 
Observatory and the Johannesbur g Planeta r i u m are typical of the exper i e nc e s of all the 
school visits observe d in my study, includi n g those observ e d during the pilot phase. 
Zeller (1995a; 1995b) recommends a particu l a r style for a na rrative analysis, that of 
?new journalism? as described by Tom Wo lfe in 1973. The writing device s recomme n d e d 
for new journal i s m and espouse d by Zeller are ?the  telling of a story in scenic episod e s ? , 
?charac t e r develop me n t through dialogu e ? , ?expe r i e n c i n g an event thro ugh the perspective 
76 
of one of its parti c i p a nt s? , and ?the full detai l i ng of the ? status life? ? or rank ? of scene 
partici p a n t s ? (Zeller , 1995b p. 79 after Wol fe 1973).  
Each of these techn i q ue s has impli c a t i o ns for a school visit narra t i ve . Scene - b y-
 s c e n e const r u c t i o n consi s t s of a series of event s , each with its own ?stor y ? , and in a 
museum or scien c e centr e cont e x t can be equat e d to the main charac t e r visiting a series of 
exhibi t s with a narrat i on of the experi e nc e s he or she has at each. Accor d i n g to Zeller the 
development of character through dialogue presents particul a r challeng e s for the 
qualit a t i v e resea r c h e r regar d i n g the extent to which he or she should report actual dialogue 
or an impres s i o n (and theref o r e an interp r e t a t i o n ) of the data. Si mila r l y , the actual proces s 
of obtain i ng conve r s a t i o n s in museum setti n g s is diffi c u l t due to the natur e of the 
interac t i v e experi e n c e (Allen , 2002). Zeller ? s recomme n d a t i o n of  a subject i v e viewpo i n t by 
tellin g the story throug h the eyes of one of its parti c i p a nt s is to show  that no account can be 
truly object i v e . An attemp t to presen t an obj ectiv e descript i o n of a science centre visit 
could result in a mechan i s t i c description of the exhibits a nd presenta t i o n s which would not 
prope r l y captu r e the natur e of the exper i e nc e fo r the majorit y of visitor s . The final writing 
device (the provisi o n of deta il ) is sugges t e d to make the story and characters believable, 
and is found in many ge nres of writing .  
In the case of a school visit, how the ma in chara c t e r inter a c t s with his or her 
surrou n d i n gs , fellow studen t s , teache r s and scien ce centre staff is importan t to provide the 
reade r with evide n c e that the narra t i v e prese nt e d is plausi b l e , and relat es to sever a l of 
Dollar d ? s crite r i a descr i be d above . I demons t r a t e how each of the criter i a relate s to my data 
sources from the study in Table 4.1.  
Table 4.1 Relationship of data sources to criteria for constructing narrative 
Criterion (from Polkinghorne 1995) Data Source 
C u l t u r a l conte x t Inter v i e w s wi th stude n t s , field notes from 
schoo l visit s 
Embo d i e d prot a g o n i s t Inte r v i e w s with stud e n t s , fiel d note s and 
vide o from study site visi t 
Relat i o n s h i p s betwe e n peopl e Field not e s from schoo l visi t s , fiel d notes and 
video from study site visit 
Main charac t e r s intera c t i o n with events Inter v i e w s with stude n t s , field notes and 
vide o from study site visi t 
Prota g o n i s t? s histo ry Inter v i e w s with stude n t s 
Time boun d ed story Fiel d note s and vide o from stud y site visi t 
77 
In the narrat i v e s I have attempt e d to follo w the guidel i n es of Polki n gh o r n e , Zeller and 
Wolc o t t in order to creat e a rich and text u r e d narr at i v e anal y s i s of what it means for a 
student to visit either HartR AO or the planetarium. In doing so, I have drawn mainly on 
field notes and video- and audio- taped recordings of school vis its to the sites, as well as 
field notes made durin g my visit s to the sc hools and intervi e w s with the student s and 
teacher s . Howeve r , what I?m present i n g is not a life hist o r y , but a snapsho t of an event in 
life, so some of the guideli n e s , such as 4 (t he central charac t e r ) , 5 (the charact e r ? s life 
experien c e s ) and 6 (the unique descript i o n of th e event ) are more relev a nt than other s such 
as 2 (the charact e r embodi e d ) and 3 (how  the characte r relates to others). 
In the follow i n g narra t i v e s , the ficti o n al st uden t s Kaelo and Kits o are based on two 
individuals, but are a synthe sis of several students who visited Hartebeesthoek Radio 
Astronomy Observat o r y and the Johanne s b u r g Planet a r i u m respec t i v e l y . 
4.2  A visit to Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
4.2.1  Before the visit 
K a e l o was very excite d to be visiti n g Hart e b e e st ho e k Radio Astro n o my Obser v a t o r y. His 
science teacher, Mr. Maoto, had told hi m that all thre e class e s in the 7 th  grade would be 
able to go, but that they had to pay R90 fo r the trip. His exciteme nt was tinged with a 
concern that his parents would not be able to pay, and that  he would be left behind. He felt 
it was a bit unfai r that if this was a school tr ip related to the topic they were doing in 
Natural Scienc e (?NS? as they all called it at school) , they should all get to go. When he 
told his paren t s and gave them the infor ma t i o n sheet and indemn i t y form for their signat u r e 
he feared the worst, but when he remi nd e d his Mom the follow i n g week, he was deligh t e d 
that she signed and gave him the mone y to take to school the next day.  
Kaelo had celebra t e d his 13 th  birthd a y in June at his hom e in Alexand r a (?Alex? ) , a 
township in northern Johannesburg. It wasn?t  a big party, just a few friends round. His 
parents had long ago expl ained to him that they were prep ar e d to have small celebr a t i o n s 
for Kaelo?s and his siblings? bi rthdays, although in their ow n SeTswana culture birthdays 
weren? t really observ e d . Kaelo was the third of four children: two boys and two girls, 
which his mother always said made a nice even nu mber . Until recent l y , he had been going 
to school in Alex, but his mother was concer n e d that the high school he was likely to go to 
after complet i n g his grade 7 was ?not doing well ?. Kaelo didn?t unders t a nd what his mother 
was referr i n g to, but he didn?t compl a i n when she moved hi m to Balfour Forest School , as 
78 
he knew he could make new friends quite quick l y . For Kaelo, that was the main reaso n for 
tolerati n g school: his friends. 
In NS, all the grade 7 classe s were doing the same topi c in term thre e : stuff to do 
with energy . Kaelo accept e d that they were  going on a trip to an observ a t o r y , which wasn?t 
to do with energ y . He didn? t think much about it; he knew that an obser v a t or y was 
somethi n g to do with the stars and astrolo g y (or was it astr ono my ? ) , and the idea of going 
on a trip was cool. In prima r y schoo l they had once been on a trip to Johan n e s b u r g Zoo, 
which he still thought about. A fe w days before this observatory  trip a tall white man (?Mr. 
Lelli ot t ? ) had come to the schoo l and expla i n e d th at he wanted them to draw and write for 
him. Before they did so, he drew a sort of  spider diagram on the board and asked them 
questio n s about Johann e s b u r g . He then said he?d  like the class to draw their own diagra m, 
but based on ?space, stars an d planets?. Kaelo didn?t mind doi ng this ? at least it was 
different from his norma l class ? but he found he couldn?t wr ite much on the diagram. He 
mainly put down words he was familiar with ? the names of the planets he knew, galaxy, 
come t, asteroids and suchlike. He didn?t rea lly know muc h about the words, but he did 
know that they were in space, and that?s what the man seeme d to want. 
After the class Kaelo and his friends didn? t discu s s the man?s visit . Kaelo heard 
some of the girls sayi n g they should go a nd look up about space and planet s in books so 
that they could show the white man that they  knew a lot. Kaelo thought, as he had many 
times, that he would never under s t a n d girls ! A few days later Kaelo saw the man again, 
and some of the class were called, one-by - o n e , to see him. Kaelo wasn?t one of them, and 
he was a bit relie v e d . 
When the day final l y came , a Friday , Kaelo was almos t burst i n g with excit e me n t 
about the trip. But it was unfai r for his seven or so classma t e s who wouldn ? t be going. His 
best friend Karabo was amongs t them, and he a voided talking about the trip when Karabo 
was around . Still, nothin g in class had been done in prepara t i o n for the visit anyway . All 
Mr. Maoto had done was to collect the money a nd indemni t y forms , and tell the class what 
they should wear for the trip. In fact Mr . Maoto told them that  he wouldn? t be going 
himself, but he didn?t say why. Kaelo was plea se d , as he didn? t like his teache r ? s sharp 
tongue, especial l y when they were out of  class, playin g footba l l for exampl e . 
At seven o?clock Kaelo arrived at school by minibus taxi as usual, met up with his 
friends , and they climbe d on the bus togethe r . His mother had given him a packed lunch, 
and he decided to eat the chips straigh t away. The bus left at seven-thirty and Kaelo and his 
79 
friends recited some hip-hop songs on the jour ney . There were three teacher s on the bus, 
and as Kaelo had joined the school in January  he only knew one of them. He was pleased 
that the teach e r s kept mostl y to thems e l v e s , and that he and hi s friends could enjoy 
themsel v e s . After about 80 minutes Kaelo saw th at they were among hills, and there were 
some ?satel l i t e s ? in the distan c e , like the ones people had on their houses to watch DSTV 9 , 
only much bigger . Then the bus  turne d on to a narro w road and began to descend steeply. 
They suddenly saw a really big ?satelli t e ? , as well as a number of buildin g s , and Kaelo 
realised that they had arrived. 
4.2.2  ?Planets and stars and like, well, space? 
T h e students all got off the bus chatteri n g lo udl y . Kate and Daisy met them, introd u c e d 
thems e l v e s as ?educ a t ors ? , and said that th ey would be showing the students around the 
observa t o r y for the next few hours. The stude nts were split into two groups, and Kaelo 
started off with Kate, who act ed just like a teacher: ?Wha t is astronomy? Can anyone tell 
me what it is??  
It?s spac e ma?a m 
The study of the stars 
It?s like when people tell your future 
...Plane t s and stars and like, well, space 
After a few answer s from the class and an explanation from Kate, Kaelo heard that 
astrono my and astrolo g y were two differe n t thin gs, although he still wasn?t quite sure of 
the diffe r e n c e betwe e n them. He wasn? t surpr i se d to hear that HartR A O (as Kate called the 
obser v a t or y ) was built as long ago as the 1960s ; Kaelo though t that they looke d prett y old. 
His mind be gan to wander a bit when Kate went on to descri b e why the observ a t o r y was 
built for severa l of the space missi ons to the Moon, Venus and Mars, and how the South 
African governme n t now owned it. He did no tice that Kate mentio n e d some th i n g called 
?Nasse r ? , which he knew was involv e d with space travel . 
They went inside a large hall which had some pieces of weird appara t u s , some 
exhibi t s and poster s on the walls, as well as a very large model of wh at Kaelo thoug h t must 
be the Moon. The students sat on  the floor, and Kate began by asking them questio n s about 
the solar syst e m and plane t s . Most of the stude nts called out planet na me s, as well as other 
words like comets, asteroi d s and galaxie s . Ka te stressed that the visit today was about 
                                                 
 
9  The main satel lite pay telev isio n chann el in Sou th Africa 
80 
havin g fun and learn i n g at the same time, and that  they shoul d try to think as well as play 
with the exhibit s . Kate then talked to them about the sun and stars. Kaelo knew some of 
what she was saying, about how the sun is a st ar, but he had thought that the sun is the 
bigge s t star. He heard that the stars are mostl y  the same size as or bigge r than the sun and 
that it is only becaus e they are far away that they seem so small. Kaelo under s t o o d when 
Kate said that the tree in the car park looks big compa r e d to the trees on the hill, but they 
are actual l y the same size. His mind wander e d a bit when Kate start e d talking about light 
trave l l i n g from the stars to Earth , and he star ted whisper i n g to Sipho about the big satelli t e 
when Kate talke d about light years . He liste n e d again when Kate started to talk about the 
death of the Sun when it runs out of fuel to  burn. Kaelo thoug h t the idea of the Sun dying 
was cool. She said that stars spin, and asked if it will spin fast er when ?livin g ? or when it is 
?dead and collap se d ? . Kaelo then jumped up when  Kate called out for volunt e e r s to sit on a 
sort of turnta b l e . He shot up his hand and laughe d when he was called first, wonder i n g 
what he was going to have to do on this rotating disc. To him it looked a bi t like a giant 
version of a turntab l e that DJs use to play vi nyls at the commun i t y hall back home ? he?d 
been to a very loud party there once. 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.1 The Turntable  
K a e l o kneel e d on the turnta bl e , and Kate gave  him weight s to hold in his hands. He held 
them out while Kate spun the turnta b l e sl owly , making him rotate. Then on Kate?s 
instru c t i o n he pulled his arms into his si des and found that he immed i a t e l y spun much 
81 
faster and nearly fell off; everybody laughed. He got off the turntable and other students 
tried , with simil a r resul t s . Feeli n g dizzy but excit e d , Kaelo didn? t reall y liste n to Kate 
explaini n g why he spun faster  with his arms pulled in, bu t heard her saying somethin g 
about the energy needed to spin a large star (arms out) was convert e d to speed in a small 
star (arms in). He just liked spinning and feeling the difference in  speed, and wasn?t too 
disturbed by the shouting of th e other studen t s outsid e . He couldn? t see what they were 
doing, but it sounded fun as they  were making a lot of nois e and he could hear water 
splatt e r i n g around . Kate then pointe d to the e normo u s model of the Moon in the hall, and 
asked Kaelo? s group what you see when you lo ok at the Moon over a few nights.  
It changes shape ma?a m, some ti me s it?s round like a plate, and 
somet i me s looks like a banana . 
It?s a crescen t or a half moon. 
Kate said ?Yes, that?s right, does anyone know why it does that?? No one was sure, 
so Kate took the group to a dark room at the back of the hall to demo ns t r a t e 1 0 . One lamp 
was set up in the room to repres e n t the sun;  each student was provided with a ping pong 
ball (?the Moon? ) on a stick, an d Kate showed how to turn round and round on your feet 
(?rotate 360 degrees? Kate ca lled it) and observe what was happening to the lit and unlit 
sides of the ball. Kaelo tried it out, and saw that the light from the lamp lit diff e r e nt 
amoun t s of the ball, depe n d i n g on its positi o n in relat i o n to the lamp. For examp l e , when 
he was stand i n g with his back to the lamp, th e ball was fully lit, and Kate said that this 
repre s e n t e d the full moon, and when he was si de- o n to the lamp only half the ball was lit, 
represen t i n g a half moon. Kael o realise d that the ball wa s meant to be the Moon going 
round the Earth and how the sun?s light fell on the Moon caused the differe n t Moon 
shapes. So it wasn?t the Earth?s shadow fa lli n g on the Moon making it look someti m e s half 
and someti me s full! He wanted to ask Kate why someti me s there is no Moon in the sky at 
night, but Kate said it was time to move on, so he didn?t get the chance .  
                                                 
 
10  Not all group s who visited HartRA O during my stu d y looked at Moon phases 
82 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.2 Moon Phases  
4.2.3  The Sun, Whispers and Rockets 
K a e l o and his group then moved outside , and Da isy calle d them over to a small teles c o p e 
pointi n g up to the sky, with a r ound white ?shade? att ached to it. Daisy said that she was 
going to show the m the sun. First, she told them someth i n g Kaelo alread y knew: never to 
look at the sun direct l y , especi a l l y with a telesco p e or binocul a r s . Then she told them how 
hot the sun is: 5000 degree s at its surfac e and 15 millio n degree s at its centre . Kaelo tried 
to think about this, but could n ? t reall y imagi n e that sort of heat; he knew that water boile d 
at just under 100 degree s at school as they had measured it once, so the idea of thousands 
or milli o n s of degre e s didn? t reall y make sens e . Befor e Kaelo could puzzl e this out, Daisy 
arrange d the telesco p e so that a round white  patch was project e d on to a small card under 
the teles c o pe . Kaelo saw that the white patc h was actua l l y the sun (Dai s y said the ?sun? s 
image ? ) and notic e d that it moved slowl y acro ss the card even though the telescope wasn?t 
moving. Daisy said that this wa s due to the ?rotation of the Earth?, and the ?sun?s apparent 
move me n t acros s the sky? was becaus e the Ea rth spins on its axis. Kaelo had heard about 
this in primar y school and seen a video of it on television, so he knew what Daisy was 
talking about: day and night caused by the Eart h spinning with differen t sides facing the 
sun and getting daylight. Then Daisy pointed out some tiny black pa tches on the sun ?Does 
anyone know what these are?? 
83 
?Some dirt on the micro s c o p e glass ? sa id Tebogo and severa l boys laughed. Daisy 
said that these were sunspo t s . Kaelo had never heard of these , and heard Daisy saying they 
are cool er areas of the surface due to magne ti c storms on the sun blockin g out the light . 
Kaelo wasn?t really sure wh at a magneti c storm was, but he thought they looked pretty 
cool anyway. He also heard Daisy say that 100 Earths could fit across the image of the sun 
they were seeing, and he thought how enor mo u s the sun must be. Da isy asked the group 
?Would you like to live on the Sun??  
No! 
Why not? 
It?s too hot! 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.3 Projection of the Sun  
After a few more questions from the group wh ich Kaelo didn?t rea lly listen to, they 
went to the car park, where there was a sort of  figure - o f - e i g h t and a curved line painte d on 
the ground . Daisy explai n e d that this is a sundi a l , used for telli n g th e time, and tha t the 
time is paint e d in white on the curve d secti o n and the date is the yellow writi n g on the 
figure - o f - e i g h t . She then asked the studen t s the date (24 October ) , and got one of the girls 
(Nnaniki) to stand on the yellow line at th e right position. Nnanik i then put her hand up 
and Daisy explai n e d that wher e the shadow of her hand cros sed the white circle was the 
time of day. It wasn?t very cl ear to Kaelo, but when Daisy used a long pole instead of 
Nnani k i , Kaelo could see that the pole? s shad ow cross e d the white line at just after the 
84 
?10? mark. Lookin g at his watch, he could see that the real time was ten past ten; this 
sundial was quite accurate! Da isy explaine d that before th e invention of watches this 
method was an import a n t way of tellin g the time. She went on to say that  as the position of 
the sun chang e s in the sky, the shadow it casts m oves, and can be used to tell the time. She 
then asked: 
What direction are you guys facing towards now? 
East! North! South! 
Daisy laughed.  
OK, which one is it? Look we are facing to war ds the sun now. Where 
does it rise? 
East ! 
OK, which direct i o n is that? Point to it. 
Several of the class pointed East, ot hers pointed in other directi o n s . 
Right. 
Daisy confir me d those who were correct .  
So if that directi o n is East, where is North? 
Final l y , the class was orient e d and Kael o saw that he was facing roughl y North 
while the sun?s shadow was poi nting roughly South. He got a bi t lost when Daisy began to 
talk about the yellow figure - o f -eight being called an analemma , and its shape being due to 
the Eart h? s tilt and orbit. Again , his mind began to wander a bit ? 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.4 Sundial  
85 
Kaelo ? s atten t i o n was jerke d back to the prese nt when Daisy then said that ?girls 
like to talk on the telep ho n e ? . This was somet h i n g that Kaelo knew, as his older sister often 
chatte d for ages on the phone to her boyfri e n d . Kaelo thought they were now going to do 
something with cell phones, but they walked ov er to what Kaelo thought  of as a satellite, 
point i n g side w a y s . Daisy calle d them dishes , and split the group into two; one group 
standing next to one dish a nd the other group at a dish about 20 metres away. The two 
dishe s were facin g each other and one perso n could then whisp e r into the dish in turns . 
Kaelo ? s class ma t e s showe d some surpr i s e when  whispe r i n g and listen i n g at the dishes , and 
when it came to his turn he found he could h ear the perso n whisp e r i n g from the other dish 
perfec t l y . Kaelo heard Sipho saying some th i n g rude about Bontl e in to the dish, and the 
boys near them laughe d , a little cruell y Kaelo thought . The group each had two turns at this 
?teleph o n e ? as Daisy joking l y called it, and then she asked them if they knew how it 
worked.  
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.5 Whisper Dish and Telescope 
T h e r e are wires running under the ground ma?am 
It?s like a cellph o n e ma?am 
No miss, it?s the shape of the satel l i t es 
Daisy picked up on this last suggest i o n a nd ignored the others. She compared the 
dish shape to a reflec t or on car headli g h t s or a torch. She said that the shape concent r at e d 
86 
the sound , and the fact that the dishe s were lined up very carefu l l y with each other meant 
that the sound carried perfectl y between th e two. She said somethi n g about a parabol i c 
shape of the dishes, but Kaelo di dn?t quite get that part of it. But he did see the simil a ri t y 
betwe e n these dishe s and the enorm o u s dish poin t i n g at the sky. Daisy said that it is a radio 
teles c o p e , and that its shape conce nt r a t e d wave s comi n g fr om stars so that they could be 
detecte d by the astrono me r s studyin g them. Da isy also said that DSTV dishes did the same 
thing, and that they are pointi n g to a satell i t e  in the sky sendin g te levision signals. Kaelo 
wished that he had DSTV; he had watched th e cartoon channel at his friend?s house a few 
times. At this point the group stood under a tree, and Daisy answer e d lots  of their questi o n s 
on all sorts of aspects of astrono my . Kaelo aske d ?If the DSTV satellit e in the sky is going 
round the Earth, how come there is no break in signal when it goes the other side? ? 
?That? s a very good questio n . In fact the DSTV  satelli t e orbits th e Earth at exactly 
the same speed as the Earth spins . So it is  always in our sky, and there?s no break in 
signal ? . Kaelo heard Daisy, but he wasn?t sure he really got it. He wondered if satellites 
ever smashed into each other. But he didn?t ask Daisy. 
Daisy then walked the group to the other side of the car park where Kaelo had 
earlier noticed the shouting and sloshing-of-water sounds comi ng from the other group. 
Daisy told them that they were going to be la unch i n g rocket s into space! In fact they were 
using Coke bottle s and bicycl e pumps, but it still looked f un. Kaelo paired up with Ross 
and they fille d thei r bottl e half full of water fr om the tap, fitted it to  the ?rocket launcher?, 
and pumpe d it with the bicyc l e pump. Ross started pumping it, but got tired when nothing 
happen e d , so Kaelo took over the pumpin g and noticed air bubbling through the water 
inside the bottle. Suddenl y the bottle shot high into the air, showering them with water.  
87 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.6 Launching Rockets 
K a e l o thought this was cool, the best thing they had done here so far, and ran to the 
water tap to refil l the bottl e . Daisy came over and sugge s t e d that they exper i me n t with 
different amounts of water in the bottle. They tr ied with a full bottle, but it didn?t rise very 
high into the air, as it seeme d to be too hea vy . They found that a quart e r bottl e flew highe s t 
in the air. Kaelo found that if he held the bottle on the launche r with a finger while it was 
being pumped it flew higher . They then tried it with no water at all, but it hardl y rose off 
the launch e r . After mo re fun Daisy called th e group together and asked if they could 
explain what had been happening. K aelo wasn? t reall y in terested in this b it, he would have 
prefe r r e d to carry on launc h i n g rocke t s , and he di dn?t reall y liste n to what Daisy said. If he 
had been asked he would probably have said that rockets should only carry a small amount 
of fuel (water) in order for them to rise  highest in the sky. ?W hy as k us about this?? 
thought Kaelo, feeling frustrat e d . 
4.2.4  Break with Gravity 
A t about 10.40 Daisy said it was time for brea k. Kaelo and his friends sat outside to eat 
their food. Kaelo now wished he hadn?t eate n his chips on the bus, as he only had a 
sandwich and drink now. He did have some mo ney, and he went to the tuck shop to buy 
some Astros . The shop had posters of the planets a nd some booklet s , but he didn?t have 
enough money to buy these. On his way back out side, Kaelo saw some other ?exhibits? in 
88 
the hall he hadn?t notice d before . He called Ross over and they picked up Coke cans from a 
table. Each one was a differ e n t  weight, and they were labell ed with the names of planets 
and the Moon. The ?Jupit e r ? can was very heavy, while ?Plut o ? was very light. Was this to 
do with the size of the plane t or was it th at Pluto was very far from the Earth? 
Why?s Pluto so light? Kaelo asked Ross. 
I dunno. Probly an alien drank all the coke! 
They both laughed. There wasn?t anything explai ning what the Coke cans were for, and if 
there had been Kaelo probably wouldn?t have read it anyway. 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.7 Coke cans and gravity 
Kaelo then saw four bathroom  scales on the floor, marked  Earth , the Moon, the Sun and 
Jupite r . On the Moon scale he only weighe d about 8.5 kilogra ms , while on the Earth scale 
he weighed 52 kg, which he thought was about  right. He weighed 132 kg on Jupiter and 
1400 kg on the Sun, which was weird! Kaelo reme m b e r e d that the Jupite r Coke can had 
been heavy.  
Hey, you?re lighter on the Moon because it?s closer to the Earth said 
Ross.  
No, it?s not that. It?s because th e Moon has no gravity, that?s why. 
Kaelo chatte d about it with Ross, and they decide d that it was some th i n g to do with 
gravity. They knew gravity on the Moon was low,  in fact if asked, they would probably 
both have said that people float on the Moon.  Now it looked like gr avity on Jupiter was 
high and it was humung o u s on the Sun. Ross coul dn? t really unders t a n d this as he thought 
89 
both Jupiter and the Sun were made out of gas,  so how could they have a lot of gravity ? 
They both went away a bit puzzle d from the scale s , but with the im pression that gravity 
was different on different planets and the Sun. 
Kaelo also saw a ga me-t y p e exhibi t called ?Cosmi c Pinbal l - Can you make a comet crash 
into Jupite r ? ? Kaelo slid a ball beari ng (repres e n t i n g the comet) around a funnel 
(rep re s e nt i ng the sola r syst e m) that gets incr easi n g l y steep towards th e centr e (repr e se n t i n g 
the Sun). The trick is to ?orb it? the comet around the bowl so that it is  captured by a 
depressi o n (signif y i n g Jupiter ) . If you get it wrong the comet will be drawn by (actual) 
gravity towards the hole in the centre and fall through it. Kaelo tried this a few times and 
manage d to hit Jupite r twice, but he didn?t tr y and reflect on what the exhibit represe n t e d , 
he just treat ed it as a game. 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.8 Cosmic Pinball 
4.2.5  Taking the Solar Sy stem for a Walk 
T h e whole class joine d toget h e r after break, and sat down in the hall toget h e r for a talk by 
anothe r educat o r called Musi. He starte d by showing the students photographs and models 
of all the plane t s in the solar system, each of which was mounted on a stand. Musi 
expla i ne d that these were scale model s of the real planets, and that they had been reduced 
four billion times. Kaelo didn?t really know what that meant, but it sounded quite 
impre s s i v e . Musi then talke d brief l y about th e Sun and gave a few facts about each of the 
90 
planet s in turn, from Me rcur y to Pluto. Kaelo knew some of wh at Musi was saying , as they 
had cover e d each of the plane t s in class , such  as the large s t , smal l e s t , clos e st to and 
furthes t from the Sun. For Kaelo, the most intere s t i n g part of the talk was the bit about the 
asteroi d s , and how one day an as tero i d might collid e with Eart h. He was fascin at e d to hear 
that one had crashed near Pretor ia hundreds of thousands of y ears ago, as well as another 
to the South of Gauten g , and that it may have  cause d all the miner a l s that were being 
mined now. Kaelo ? s mi nd wande r e d a bit, and he thought of that film where there was a 
rock smashing into the Earth, causing floods and all the people runn ing to the mountains. 
He quite liked the idea of te rribl e devast a t i o n ? he wondere d what he?d have done if it 
happene d to him? 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.9 Taking the Solar System for a walk: the Sun 
H i s mind jerke d back to heari n g Musi speaki n g again. Another fact th at struck Kael o was 
that Jupit e r had about sixty moons ; he could n ? t really believe that was possible . One of his 
classma t e s asked whethe r it could ever be da y on Jupiter with so many moons orbiting the 
planet. Kaelo though t this was an odd questio n , but  Musi explain e d pati en t l y to the studen t 
that it wasn? t the Moon that cause d night , but the sunlig h t fallin g on the planet as it rotate d . 
The class asked Musi lots of ot her questi o n s during th e talk, especi a l l y a bout the asteroid s , 
Jupite r and Pluto.  
91 
Musi then hande d each plane t to a studen t , and took the whole class outsid e , saying 
that the solar system could do w ith some exercise, so they should take it for a walk. Just 
outside the hall Musi got a stude nt to place the Sun on its sta nd. He told the class that the 
model was reduced four billion times from rea lit y , and said that they  would need to pace 
the distanc e from the sun to each of the clos er planets . They set up the Mercury stand 20 
paces from the Sun, while Venus and Eart h were placed 36 and 50 paces respect i v e l y. 
Kaelo didn?t pay much attentio n to these dist ances, but he was carrying Mars, and he knew 
that he had to pace 27 steps from Earth . He set up the Mars stand and looke d back towar d s 
the Sun. Although he hadn?t fully understo o d what  Musi had said about  the four billion, 
Kaelo though t there mus t be an awful lot of space in space. The pl anet s (big though they 
appear up close) were tiny in  relat i on to the Sun and sitti n g at enor mo u s dista n c e s from 
each other . He would have thoug h t about it for a bit longe r , but the class was now chant i ng 
?15-16 - 1 7 ? as they paced the asteroi d belt beyond Mars. Wh en  they reached Jupiter, 190 
paces from Mars, Musi said that they needed  to stop, as the planet s beyon d were too far to 
pace. Kaelo couldn ? t believ e that Pluto woul d be about 1.5 km from the Sun. He looked 
towar d s the horiz o n and tried to imagi n e that this dista n ce repre s e n t e d the size of the solar 
system, with these tiny models repres e n t i n g th e plane t s . When Musi said that the neare s t 
star (Alpha Sen, or someth i n g like that) w ould be 10,000 km away on this scale it almost 
hurt his brain just to think about  it, so Kaelo ran into the bu ilding when Musi said they?d 
now be having a slide show. On the way, Kaelo punched Ross on the arm. 
4.2.6  Landing on the Moon 
T h e room for the slide show was cool and dar k, and Kaelo got a seat at the back with a 
group of his friends. Daisy began by asking que stions: ?Who was the first man in space? ? 
?Who was the first man on the Moon? ? 
Kaelo didn?t know the answer to the firs t one, but shouted out ?Mark Shuttwer t h ? 
to the second , as did ma ny of his class ma t e s . Daisy said that while Mark Shutt l e w o rth was 
the first Sout h Africa n into space, it was an American called Neil Arms tron g who was the 
first man on the Moon. 
When Daisy asked who wants to be the s econd South African into space, the class 
called out ?Me! Me!? Daisy quickl y chose a small boy called Titus, and said ?Today I?ll 
take Titus, and I?ll take anot her tomorr o w ? . She went on to say ?OK, he?s been trained , can 
he go dressed in uniform?? Th e classe d chorus e d ?No!?  
92 
Why not? 
He?ll bounce 
There ? s a specia l suit 
For protect i o n 
Titus then dress e d in a white overa l l ,  with a NASA logo, and Daisy some ti me s 
referred to him as Titus Shuttlew o r t h or Tito Ar mstrong. She then suggested he would 
need boots, ?Why? ? 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.10 The Second South African in Space 
? S o that he doesn?t float?. Dais y said that the gravity on the Moon is one sixth that of the 
Earth, and that he needs some th i n g heavy to  keep him on the ground . This fitted with 
Kaelo?s own idea that people float on the Moon, and that they need to be held down with 
somethi n g heavy. Titus then donned rubber boots as  well as a pair of gloves. Again, Daisy 
asked the class why he would need gloves, and settled on th e idea of protection against 
extreme s of hot and cold. She then handed Titu s a helme t to wear, a nd asked the class why 
he would need one. 
No atmos p h e r e . 
Dangerou s gases. 
 ?were ideas offered, and Kaelo heard Dais y saying that the helmet was needed to 
protec t his head from the Sun?s rays and extr eme s of temperat u r e . Finally, Titus was given 
93 
an ?oxygen tank? (which Kaelo could see was just a plastic 2 litre Coke bottle) and was 
told he had a microp h o n e inside the helmet as there is no air in space, and that sound 
cannot travel. 
Daisy then introduced a slide show, saying  that Titus had taken the photographs 
during his trip to the Moon. K aelo saw a series of photographs  projected on to the screen, 
and Daisy quest i o ne d the class as each one was shown. Some of them, such as the 
photog r a p h s of the Saturn 5 rocket , the Earth from space and the Moon from space didn?t 
make much impres s i o n on Kaelo, but when it ca me to the first steps taken on the Moon he 
paid more attent i o n . He reme mb e r e d he had heard the phrase ?One small step for Man, one 
giant leap for mankin d ? before and could now  unders t a n d what it was talkin g about when 
he saw the black and white photos of the as tron a u t s walki n g on the Moon?s surfa c e . Daisy 
said that the Moon was like a desert: it has no water, also no air, and although there is soil 
it is not fertil e , and temper a t u r e s are so ex treme that you can?t grow anything . Kaelo was 
surpr i s e d to see what Daisy calle d a ?luna r buggy vehicl e ? which the astrona u t s used to 
explo r e the surfa c e . Danie l sugge s t e d ?Their vehicl e help them to not float. Maybe their 
vehicle have gravity? and everybod y laughed, though Kaelo wasn?t sure why. Daisy ended 
by showing a footprint in the dust of the Moon, and asked if it is going to stay there 
foreve r . ?Yes? they chorus e d , and Daisy emphas i s e d that there is no wind to blow it away, 
no water to wash it away and no weathe r . It would stay ther e unless a meteorite hits it. 
Kaelo wasn?t so intere s t e d in the last few phot os , which showed the sp ace ship returni n g to 
Earth and landing in the sea. The lights were  then switched on and cl ass then asked a few 
questio n s before they went ou tside . Kaelo was getting a bit restless by this time, and was 
pleased to be back in the open air. 
4.2.7  The Telescope and Control Room 
O u t s i d e , Musi stood the class in front of th e telescope. Kaelo was amazed how big it was. 
From where they had been at the Visitors? Cent re it hadn?t looked so large. To start with, 
Kaelo listen e d to Musi, and heard that the te lescop e once monitore d Apollo missions to the 
Moon, and that it was the only one of its size in Africa . As he was talkin g , the dish began 
to move, and Musi explain e d that it is because  they are studying stars. Now Kaelo wasn?t 
quite sure about this, as it was daytime, and he knew that  we see stars at  night. He was 
pleas e d that one of the class asked the quest i o n in his mind: ?How can it see stars in the 
daytime ? ? Musi explain e d that the dish was lis tening to the sound that stars make, and that 
94 
the sound will bounce on the dish into a receiv e r . They stayed outsi d e for anothe r 10 
minutes or so, and Musi answere d lots of other questi o n s from the class while Kaelo 
drifted to the edge of th e group with Ross and they ch atted about other things. 
 
Copyright ? Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observatory  
Figure 4.11 The radio telescope at HartRAO 
T h e class was then usher e d into the build i n g ne xt to the dish and K aelo was excite d to see 
lots of elect r o n i c equipme n t with light s flashi n g , rathe r like in a scienc e ficti o n movie . In 
fact he was so overwhe l me d that he didn?t real ly listen much to what he was told in the 
short time they spent there . Th en Musi played a tape whic h sounded like someone beating 
a drum, and said that this is what some star s sound like. Kaelo then realise d that the big 
dish was someho w heari n g stars rathe r than  seeing them, so he now underst o o d how the 
dish could be used in the daytime . 
It wasn?t long before they were back in the sunshi n e and walkin g back to the 
visit or s ? centr e . As they walke d with Dais y and Musi, some of the very keen student s 
carried on asking questions. Kael o had one or two he would ha ve liked to as k, but he got 
talki n g with Ross, and didn? t reall y get the ch anc e to do so. In the visit or s ? centr e they sat 
aroun d for a short time, then the class monit o r gave a vote of thank s to Kate, Musi and 
Daisy for showing them round the site. Kaelo join ed in the clapping enth usiastically, as he 
had really enjoye d being there. 
95 
4.2.8  After the visit 
T h e trip back to school passe d quite quickly for Kaelo, a nd although they sang songs and 
messe d aroun d on the bus he felt quite tired . He  caught the minibu s ta xi from school and 
rushed home to tell his mother what they  had done at HartRAO. His mother listened 
patie nt l y as the words tumbl e d from his m out h: pumpin g the rocket s , spinni n g on the 
turntab l e , picking up Coke cans, talking through the whisper dishes, hearing stars 
drummi ng and seeing the big dish moving. Af ter the evening meal he watched some 
televis i o n and went to bed ener vated, but sti ll quite excite d . 
Over the weekend, Kaelo thought about the trip from time to time, and wonde r e d 
wheth e r he might ask his fathe r to take hi m back to HartR A O some t i me . But his fath e r 
always seemed to be so busy, even at weeken ds , that he wasn? t sure wheth e r to or not. On 
Monday, back at school, Mrs. Kathrada talked to the class about the trip , and said that they 
ought to carry on studyi n g about space, and she gave them a piece to read about Neil 
Ar mstrong?s first footprint on the Moon. Kaelo like d this, as he could re late the article to 
what he had seen on the slide show at HartR AO. However, Kaelo had to be a bit careful 
around Karabo, who hadn?t been able to go on the trip. Kaelo held back  talking about the 
trip too mu ch, so that Karab o would n ? t feel  bad about it. During the week, Mr. Lelliot t 
came back and all the studen t s who had been on the visit went to an empty classr o o m with 
him. Mr. Lelli ot t gave the stude nt s the spide r diagr a m each one had compl e t e d befor e the 
trip, and Kaelo was pleased to be able to add to his, as well as  cross out 1 or 2 things that 
he now decide d were not right. In fact Kaelo ha d so much to add that he wrote some things 
on the back of the sheet . Later on Mr. Lelli ot t asked the same students he had seen before 
the trip to speak with him again, but as before, Kaelo wasn?t one of them. 
Over the next few weeks they didn?t do anyth i ng relat e d to the trip in class either 
with Mr. Maoto or any of the other teacher s , and the visit sl owl y faded from Kaelo ? s mind. 
One day, when he happene d to see the televi sion news his father wa s watching, there was a 
report of a Chinese astrona u t going into sp ace and orbiti n g the Earth. Kaelo starte d 
chattering to his father about how they had seen pictures like this at HartRAO and his 
father appe ared quite interest e d . But then ther e was a programme that his father wanted to 
watch in peace , and Kaelo left the room to do some homew o r k . Over the next mont h s the 
visit faded furth e r in Kaelo ? s me mor y , and in January he started grade 8 at the high school 
near to Balfou r Forest . Howeve r ,  he did find that even over the next year, he was remi nded 
from ti me to time abou t the visi t , by scho o l w o r k topic s (in Geogra p h y )  by occasi o n a l items 
96 
on the televi s i o n and in talki n g with his friends. For Kaelo, th e relat i v e l y brief visit had 
made a lastin g impre s s i o n . 
4.3  Discussion 
Like Falk and Dierking?s (2000 ) descri p t i o n s of museum vi s its, this narrative shows what 
school childr e n experi e n c e at the Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Observa t o r y . While 
Kaelo ? s expe r i e nc e s were a compos i t e of what the whole group experie n c e d , each event 
describ e d was based on data from my study that  involve d indivi d u a l st udents. It is worth 
noting here some of the experi e n c e s , how they re la t e to theme s in the liter a t u re revi ew, and 
how they are drawn from the data gathered in  the study, ma ny of which will be described 
in subsequ e n t chapter s . Five issues are describ e d here: studen t miscon c e p t i o n s ; socio-
 e c o n o mi c const r a i n t s on museu m visit s ; inade q uate preparation; inappropriate follow-up; 
and enjoym e n t . 
First, Kaelo was present e d with quite a lo t of informa t i o n about astron o m y , and the 
narrat i v e shows that he entere d the centre with sever al misco n c e p t i o ns . I will illus t ra t e this 
with three vignet t e s from Kaelo? s story, and show also how the centre either change d or 
reinfor c e d his ideas .  
? Kaelo originally thought that the Sun is the biggest star, but the explanation he was 
given together with the anal ogy of the distance of trees de monst r a t e d to him that the 
Sun only appears big because it is close. In  this case the explana t i o n by the educato r 
appeared to change Kaelo?s underst a n d i n g of the Sun in re lation to stars. Data for 
this observa t i o n was gathere d from student s who changed their opinion that ?the Sun 
is the bigges t star? to ?the Sun is the close st star? in thei r pre- and post-vi s i t 
interv i e w s . Althou g h it is likely that in some  cases they just reme mbe r e d this as a 
fact, in other cases the stude nt explai n e d that their unders t a n d i n g of star distanc e in 
relatio n to the Sun had changed . 
? K a e l o and Ross had severa l miscon c e p t i o n s  about gravit y demons t r a t i n g a limite d 
unders t a n di n g of it: they though t there was no gravit y on the Moon and that the Sun 
or Jupite r would not have gravit y as they are made of gas. By interacting with the 
Coke can exhibit and the gravity scales Kaelo and Ross came away with sligh t l y 
changed knowledg e of gravity: that grav ity is differe n t on diffe r e nt plane ts . 
However, the gravity exhibits at HartRAO were not thems e l v e s suffi c i e nt to resul t 
in a major shift in their unde rsta n d i n g of gravity. Some interve n t i o n by an educato r 
97 
was necessary to explain what the displays  represe n t e d . Althou g h there were several 
explanat i o n s of other exhibits as describe d in the narrat i v e the centre did not addre ss 
gravit y as a central issue. In fact dur ing the Moon landings slide show some 
students ? concepti o n s of gravity were re inforced, by the presenter suggesting that 
heavy boots need to be worn, and not dispel li n g the suggesti o n that they are needed 
to prevent the wearer from floatin g . 
? T h e narrativ e does not show us what K aelo?s percepti o n of space was, although 
many studen t s in their Pers ona l Meaning Maps describ e d it as empty , lacki n g air 
and water. The descrip t i o n of Kaelo?s expe rience of ?taking th e solar syste m for a 
walk? tries to demon s t r a t e that this activi t y did indee d have at least some effec t on 
his unders t a n d i n g of the emptin e s s of space. The data for this claim was gather e d by 
watchi n g and listen i n g to studen t s as th ey paced out the distan c e betwe e n the 
planet s . There was genuin e amaze me n t that the Sun could be so small and far away 
by the time stude n t s reach e d Mars, in contr a st to Musi? s stat eme n t that the model s 
are reduc e d four billi o n time s from their actual size. This figure appeared to be 
incompr e h e n s i b l e to most student s , who never referr e d to it in their subseq u e n t 
inter v i e w .  
The miscon c e p t i o ns demons t r a t e d by Kaelo were si mil a r to those reco rde d in the liter a t ur e 
regar di n g the Sun, stars, gravi t y and space (Bail e y & Slater , 2003) . Altho u g h museu m 
resear c h e r s are usuall y carefu l not to make claims about subs tantial change in visitors? 
knowledge as a result of a vis it to a museum, the evide n c e fr om Kaelo?s narrative suggests 
that there were some shift s in his knowl e d g e of astrono my and concept i o n of gravity and 
space . 
Second l y , the narrati v e brings out a contex t u a l issue regardi n g the visit. As 
discuss e d in section 3.5, only student s whose parent s could a ffor d to pay for the trip 
actual l y took part in it. Out of a total of 75 studen t s in the two grad e 7 class e s at Balfo u r 
Forest School, only 37 (49%) completed cons ent for ms and 30 (40%) went on the trip. The 
percen t a g e s of studen t s who par tic i p a t e d in the trip were mu ch larger in the other school s 
as they were mostly private schools where th e socio- e c o n o mi c status  of the parents was 
likely to be much higher . Howeve r , Balfou r Fo rest School is more repres e n t a t i v e of the 
majori t y of school s in South Af rica , althou g h it too is privil e g e d , being locat e d in a former 
?white? suburb of Johannes b u r g . Kaelo?s mother ? s rema rk , that the local townsh i p school 
was ?not doing well?, is a remi nd e r to the reader of this fact.  
98 
Thir d l y , the narrat i ve demo n s t r a t e s the limit e d extent to which Kaelo and his 
class ma t e s were prepa r e d for the visit . In his school they did no classro o m prepara t i o n for 
the visit, and in most schools the students were currently st udying another topic in science 
(or geography in the case of Lourdes Girls Sc hool). No preparation for the visit was the 
case in all schools except Bokamos o , whose scie nc e club visited HartRA O . Six individ u a l s 
acros s the whole study did some person a l prepa r a t i o n , and gave specifi c exampl e s of what 
they did. There was so me evide nc e that indiv i d u a l s did the prepa r a t i o n becaus e they 
wante d to ?impr e s s ? me with th eir knowle d g e , and my visit did have some effe c t in raisi n g 
studen t s ? awaren e s s and the profile of the t opic . These issue s are discu s s e d under the 
limit a t i o n s of the resea r c h in secti o n 9.8. The na rrati v e also notes that the student ? s science 
teache r , Mr. Maoto, does not attend the visit with  his student s , and instea d other teache r s 
accompa n y the class. This too was a common o ccur r e n c e in my study, and is one of the 
reason s I did not includ e  teache r s in the study.  
Fourt h l y , the issue of follo w - u p after th e visit. Kaelo?s narrative shows that a 
teach e r did get the stude n t s to think about the trip by givi ng them a readin g about the 
Moon landin g s . Howeve r , althou g h this is what was stated by the teache r , Kaelo did not 
relate what Mrs. Kathra d a did with studen t s in her Englis h class when I asked him in the 
intervie w , possibly because nothi ng was done in his science cl ass. No students worked on 
astron o my betwe e n the time of the trip and wh en I visite d to inte rv i e w them, and this 
varied from 1 to 35 days. What did happen after the visit was that Kaelo told his mothe r a 
lot about what he had experi enced at HartRAO. Except for Nonkululeko, all st udents told 
someon e , us uall y a me mber of their fa mily of the experi e n c e s they had during their visit. 
This question was a useful method of elici t i n g what stude nt s found excit i n g or impor t a nt 
for them at the visit . Ther e is subst a nt i al evide n ce in the litera t u r e that teach e r s fail to use 
school visit s to museums as effec t i v e l y as  they might (e.g. Gri ffin, 2004; Storksdieck, 
2004), and exhort a t i o n s from resear c h e r s to corr ect this situati o n (e.g. Falk and Dierkin g 
2000). The narrativ e shows that th ere is considerable potential for teachers to work with 
their classes after a visit, both from the viewpoi nt of learning about subject matter as well 
as using the visit as a focus for other work, in languag e classes for exampl e . 
Last l y , it is clear from the narr at i ve that Kaelo regarde d the trip as an enjoya b l e 
event . I showe d in Chapt e r 2 that the resear c h litera t u r e on museums and scienc e centre s 
has tended to dichot o mi s e fun and learni n g , with several opinion pieces decrying the 
hands- o n experi e n c e as being ?minds - o f f ? (P arkyn, 1993; Sanders, 1998). Kaelo however, 
99 
appea r e d to learn some t h i n g about astro n o my as well as have fun at HartRA O . As Falk and 
collea g u e s have demons t r a t e d (Falk et al . , 1998; Griffin, 2004) and I show in subsequent 
chapt e r s , educa t i o n and enter t a i n me n t are not li kely to be on a contin u u m , but comp le me n t 
each other in a scienc e centr e envir on me n t . 
Kaelo?s narrati v e describ e s experie n c e s that are common to many South African 
learn e r s on school visit s to museum s . These include difficulties with funding and lack of 
prepar a t i o n for the visit, dise n g a g e me n t of  teache r s and the didact i c nature of the 
experie n c e during the visit, and a lack of follo w - u p after the visit when back at school . The 
narrati v e also demonst r a t e s that in a few cases  a student?s idea about a scientific concept 
might be comple t e l y change d as a result of th e visit . For examp l e K aelo ? s reali sa t i o n that 
the Moon?s phases are caused by sunlight fa lling on different amounts of the Moon?s 
surfa c e , rathe r than his previ o u s idea that  the phases are caused by the Earth? s shadow . 
Howeve r , they also demons t r a t e that previo us l y held misco n c e p t i o ns can remai n unalt e red 
or be further reinforced during the visit . For examp l e Kaelo ? s id ea that in order not to float 
on the Moon one needs to wear heavy boots. It also demo ns t r a t e s that the majori t y of 
learni n g is likel y to be small and incre me n t a l , a notio n that is explor e d more fu lly in 
Chapters 6 and 7. 
4.4  A visit to the Johannesburg Planetarium 
Welcome to the Johannesburg Planetarium , to the Wits Un iversity.  I?m 
Caroline.  I?m doing a show for you today.  What are we going to look at 
today?   
S p a c e ! 
Star s ! 
The Moon! 
Plane t s! 
O kay I?ve got bad news for you guys.  Your teachers booked you into the 
solar system show.  Okay so what is in  the solar system?  Planets, the sun.  
How many stars in the solar system? 
Student 1:   Millions. 
Caroline  No sorry 
Student 2:  One. 
Caroline:  One.  Okay.  So tough luck.  We can do 8 000 stars with our 
projector here, but we?re booked into the solar system show. 
K i t s o was final l y sitti n g in th e rather uncomfortable seats of  the Johanne s b u r g Planeta r i u m, 
lookin g up at the dome which was covere d with st ars. She had been lookin g forwar d to this 
100 
visit for some ti me, so now what was the pl an et a ri u m lady talki n g a bout? Had there been 
some sort of mistake in their booking ? 
That?s one way we can get around that.  If we?re going to lo ok at the solar 
system we might as well l ook at where the solar syst em fits in, in space and 
we are doing solar system here but we ?ll start by looking at the stars. 
 
Figure 4.12 Zeiss star projector in the planetarium 
O K , that sounded better, and Kitso realise d that maybe Car oline had been joking. Kitso 
had always been interes t e d in the stars and planets , though she wa sn?t that keen on 
geograph y (which is where the topic fitted in at her school ) . Kitso atten de d Lourd e s Girls 
School in the western suburbs of Johanne s b u r g, and was unaware that the astronomy topic 
had now been changed in the cu rri cu l u m to fall withi n the new Natur al Scien ce s learn i n g 
area. She just knew it as part of geogr a p h y . She had heard from friends that the 
planeta r i u m was really cool, that she would actual l y see the planets and stars and stuff. 
Caroline: I just want to start about, w ith those particular stars.  Does 
anyone know the names of any of those stars up there that you can see now? 
Student:  I can see Orion 
Caroline:  Okay, right.  Southern Cr oss.  Can you see the Southern Cross 
there? 
Students:  [Some say yes, others say no]. 
Caroline:  No?  Some of you can.  Yeah.  Here it is. 
101 
Kitso had no idea of the names of any stars.  She just knew them as stars, that they 
consist e d of burning gases, were small objects (say the size of the Earth ) and far away.  
People looked at the sky thousands of years ago, it?s only in the last couple 
hundred years that we have the tools for understanding the universe.  But we 
know that people have always been intere sted in it, mainly because they gave 
names to things. That one there Betelgeux, which is an Arabic word that 
means the armpit of the giant. (Laughter) Look here is the giant, his belt ? 
these three stars in a row - What are they called?  Three sisters some people 
called them.  Some people called them Orion?s Belt and some people used to 
call them dikolobe.  What does that me an?  Pigs.  Ja.  So that?s Tswana for 
pigs.   
K i t s o could see what Caroli ne meant, as she wa s pointin g out the stars with a sort of light-
 p o i n t e r torch . Kitso had some vague me mor y of being at her grand mo t h e r ? s house sitti n g 
round a fire at night, and one of her cousin s mentio n i n g dikolobe  in the sky. She wondere d 
if any of the really bright stars had names, but Carolin e was moving on???. 
Nowadays we can do more than just nam ing stars and looking at them.  We 
actually know what they are about and th e reason we could do that is we?ve 
got better equipment. What?s that?  That?s a telescope.   
N o w Kitso saw a picture projec t e d on to the planet a r i u m dome. Wo w, this show wasn?t 
just going to be about stars and space. Th ey?d be seeing ?real things ? too, like on TV. 
Ja.  That is quite a small telescope.  It?s the kind of thi ng that people who?s 
really into astronomy will go out and th ey?ll buy. Now the size of that we 
know will be about 10, 15 centimetres wh ich means already that is about 400 
times better than your eye.  
Anyone know the name of South Africa?s big new telescope? 
Students:  SALT 
Caroline:  Yes, it is called SALT - Sout h African Large Telescope.  It is 
going into that building there, the bu ilding is finished, the telescope should 
be finished some time next year.  Ther e is one obvious thing you can do with 
the telescope, you can see more.  That makes people realise that the universe 
is much bigger than we thought.  
C a r o l i n e went on to explai n how astron o me r s look at the colour of stars, which helps them 
work out the hotter ones (blue) and the cooler  ones (red), and how you can use a sort of 
spectru m of the stars to work our their composi t i o n . Kitso found this all a bit confusing. As 
far as she was concer n e d stars were these l ittle dots in the sky, and just as she was 
wonderi n g what they?d be like if you got close to one, Caroli n e did just that??. 
So that particular star, bright stars in fact, are mainly hydrogen and a bit of 
helium then a couple of other gases as well. A star is a ball of burning gas.  
Right, so if you went up close to a star, about 150 million kilometres away 
102 
?.. What?s that? 
Students:  The sun. 
Caroline:  The sun.  Yah, what is the sun?  A star? 
Students:  Yes. 
Caroline:  It is the which star? 
Students:  The biggest! 
Caroline:  It is not the biggest star.  The? 
Students:  Closest. 
Caroline:  Closest  Thank you.  It is the one that we live close to, it is not 
bigger at least not of all the stars,  it is a very ordinary type of star . 
K i t s o heard Caroli n e talkin g , but she didn?t really  take it in, as Caroli n e seemed to be 
saying so many things : the Sun is 6000 degree s , UV-rays , X-rays, the ozone layer. Anyway 
Kitso knew  the sun is the biggest star. It?s obvious  anywa y , as it?s much bigge r than the 
other stars in the sky. Then a video fil m st arted playin g on the pl aneta r i u m dome , which 
Caroli n e said was the Sun, taken with a special camera ? ? . 
Now one thing to notice is that the sun is turning.  Everything in the 
Universe is spinning. If y ou look at the top where you can see it against the 
black of space, you can see jets of gas being thrown off.  If you ? just watch 
and tell me if it goes away from the s un or back to the sun?  It?s going back.  
Right.  So how does it pull it back?  Gravity.  Ja.  The sun has about 28 times 
as much gravity as the Earth which it? s useful. If it didn?t had enough gravity 
it would just sort of blow  itself apart from heat.  He at makes things expand.  
Gravity holds things like the sun togeth er.  We wouldn?t have a star if it 
wasn?t for gravity. 
K i t s o though t the video was really cool. She knew a bit about gravit y : she knew that in 
space there was a lot of gravit y, and that with a lot of gravity you could float around. She 
tried to reconci l e what she knew with lots  of gravity on the sun, and thought maybe 
?.ugh , she couldn ? t reall y think about it, as a very big number then flashe d on to the 
plane t a r i u m dome : 
40,000,000,000,000 km 
C a r o l i n e said that was the distan c e from the Sun to the neares t star: Alpha Centa u r i : 40 
milli o n milli o n kilome t re s . Kitso wasn? t great at Maths and the numbe r on the dome was 
mind-b o g g l i n g . 
How far is it? How would you describe that?   
Student:  Far. 
Far.  Very, very far.  Light years aw ay.  Right the problem with a number 
like that is no human being has ever  experienced that number, not 
physically, not themselves.  You never had to walk that distance or travel 
that distance.  Our brains can?t real ly understand stuff that we haven?t 
actually had to deal with physically. 
103 
Caroline then tried to make the distanc e to Alpha Centau ri more understandable by 
compa r i n g it with the dista nc e acros s the solar syste m, which is 10 thous a n d milli on 
kilome t r e s . She then sugge s t e d that if we could trave l across the solar syst e m in a very fast 
rocket in one year (20 times faster than we can), then we can do a su m to show how long it 
would take to reach the nearest star: 
40,000,000,000,000  
10,000,000,000 
 
40,000,000,000,000  
10,000,00 0,000  
 
Which gives us 4000 years. 
 
Okay, so if you could travel twenty tim es faster than what we can now then 
you will be able to get from the Earth to the edge of the solar system in half a 
year, then you travel for 3 999 years and you get to the to the first star.  
What would you pass on the way? Noth ing. It would be the most boring 
journey imaginable.  
K i t s o found this a bit confusi n g . Even with Caroli n e ? s attempt to try and make the distan c e 
unders t a n d a b l e , Kitso didn?t really identi f y with the ?years ? expl anation. She did though 
reme mbe r the ?sound ? of the distan c e : forty milli o n milli on was quite easy to remembe r , 
even if she was not sure what it referr e d to. When she was asked later , she said it?s the 
distanc e from the Earth to the Sun. 
The next part of the planetarium show was about stars being born. Caroline pointed 
out Orion?s cellpho n e (Kitso like d that idea - a cell phone in the stars) which was really a 
nebula, or cloud of gas and dust. She then showed how the cloud is ?lumpy? and that 
gravi t y pulls the gas and dust toget h e r until it become s hotte r and begin s to burn, for mi n g a 
star. She then demon s t r a t e d this with a vi deo animat i o n of the forma ti o n of the solar 
syste m, and stres se d that our solar syste m is plate- s h a p e d, with the sun in the middl e , 
surrou n d e d by the planet s . This didn?t really m ean much to Kitso . She though t of the solar 
system as some sort of belt of planet s surr ounding the Earth, and so couldn?t relate what 
Caroli n e was showin g to her own concep t of a so lar system . Kitso confus e d this furthe r in 
her mind with a system of heatin g water by  using a solar panel on the roof of her 
neighb o u r ? s house. Wasn?t that a solar system? 
Right, let me get closer to Mars. Yo u can see Mars in the evening sky now. 
Have you all seen Mars?Those who have seen it was it easy to find? 
Students:  Yes. 
It looks like a star, million kilometres away into space and it looks tiny but it 
104 
is very bright.  Okay have a look at that  sky there.  Okay.  Right, have a look 
especially at these two here.  Jupiter and Mercury. 
Kitso was amazed. She didn?t realise that planet s were visi bl e in the night sky. OK they 
weren? t very impres s i v e in size, as they looked like bright stars,  but the fact th at they were 
other worlds far out in space, and we  could see them was reall y cool.  
Right let?s go look for th e stars, but to see stars we are going to have to 
speed up time now. then the sun will move across the sky.  Okay, we will go 
from there all the way across to that side and it will go underneath the 
horizon there and the elephant will come  and collect it and carry it all the 
way around there and put it right there for tomorrow morning. (Laughter). 
What?s really happening? 
Students:  The Earth is moving. 
Caroline:  The Earth is? 
Students:  Rotating. 
Caroline:  Rotating or spinning.  The stars looking like they are moving over 
in fact it?s just the Earth spinning. 
K i t s o didn?t underst a n d what Carolin e was doi ng. As far as Kitso was concer n e d the sun 
did move across the sky, as did the Moon at ni ght. She had never really noticed the stars 
much. Carol i n e then got the class to use star ch arts to try and identif y the planets and some 
specif i c stars . The idea of this was so that th e studen t s could use the chart at home and look 
at the night sky, but Kitso got confused by th e chart, and how to hold it. She was also 
confu s e d that East and West seeme d to be mi xed up, but she got a bit more intere s t e d when 
Caroline pointed out some of the horoscop e constell a t i o n s ? 
Let?s go now back to the Southern Cr oss and Antares,  the heart of an 
animal.  It?s very well known.  Right can you see a scorpion there?  I mean 
look at the shape and the si ze of this scorpion if I take it away you can then 
see the sting at the end of the tail. This  is Libra.  Used to be part of the 
Scorpion.  There?s Virgo and you?ve got Leo down at that part of the sky 
over there?  Capricorn is just down he re.  You?ve got Capricorn but the rest 
of them you?ll have to wait ? okay  you have to come back to the 
planetarium in the summer for Taurus and in spring for Pisces. 
Wow, this was really interesting! Kitso alwa ys looke d at her horosc o p e in her mother?s 
magazi n e . She was a Le o, and now she could actual l y see the Leo stars in the sky. She 
might try and use that star chart after all and see if she could make it out in the real sky, 
though she knew that the street lights around he r home were reall y brigh t . What Kitso 
didn?t notice was how Caroline was trying to point out that the st ar signs were the 
conste l l a t i o n s throug h which the planet s and th e moon passed over the c ourse of the year. 
105 
How many planets? 
Students:  Nine  
Nine okay that is not very helpful, th en move on from that if you actually 
want to know what is going on in the solar system. Could anything be living 
on other planets? It?s unlikely on Mercury and Venus. Why? Too hot.  
C a r o l i n e then went throug h the planet s of the solar system, describi n g and showing slides 
of each briefly in turn. The one  that caught Kitso? s imagin a t ion was Mars. She saw that it 
had ice caps, that it had been visite d by ma ny space craft , that it had mount a i n s , and 
erosion channels which looked like they had been carved by  water. She had never thought 
about it befor e , but Kitso reali s e d that the planet s out there in th e solar system were 
actuall y a bit like Earth,  with ground and soil and stuff. She was disa ppointed to hear that 
the temper a t u r e on Mars never really went abov e zero , as well as the fact that it took so 
long to get there. 
Jupiter, Saturn, Uranus and Neptune. They? re huge.  They?ve made of gas.  
You cannot live in a gas cloud.  They ar e very far from the sun.  They should 
be really, really cold. Does anyone k now now how many moons Jupiter has?   
Twenty something. Twelve.  Sixteen.   
Okay none of those are the correct answer .  Let me just tell something if you 
go and look in the books you will see that Jupiter has 16 Moons.   
We put together a show about planets  and about Jupiter and I think when we 
started we were saying it had 51 moons.  By the time the show finish about 2 
months later ? we were up to 61 moons.  So when anyone asks you how 
many moons Jupiter has?  So at le ast 61 is about the best answer. 
There?s another of Jupiter?s moons.   Th is one is called Europa because that 
white stuff is ice. 
There?s our Pluto.  Right you were supposed to say ?what about Pluto??  
We left out Pluto because no space craft has been there.  So we don?t have a 
decent picture.  There?s the best pict ure of Pluto.  They?re talking about 
sending a space craft there next year. 
K i t s o was really enjoying the pi ctures of the planets, and then  suddenl y Carolin e said they 
had run out of time.  
O kay now, we?ll get it really dark bec ause actually the other thing I want to 
show you is the Milky Way.   The M ilky Way right now is going right over 
your head more or less over there but  we need a nice dark sky to see that 
and then the other thing we have to do in a planetarium like this is play 
music which has to be educational music.   So this one is for the teachers.  
This one is called the astronomy song. 
A n d that was the best bit. Carol i ne speed e d up the stars spinni n g overhe a d , she made it as 
dark as the darkest night a nd played the music loudly . Kits o thought she could watch this 
106 
for hours, and then sudden l y the lights came up a nd the class all start e d talking animatedly 
as they walked out of the planetar i u m. 
A few days later Kitso found her star char t crumpl e d up in her pocket. She did go 
outsid e with it one evenin g , but it was cloudy and she didn?t try again. She did tell her 
cousin about the trip, especia ll y the bit about the photos of Mars? surface, and how it looks 
as though there is anothe r Earth up there somewh e r e . During break the follow i n g week 
some girls from Grade 7 asked Kitso and he r friends how the trip had gone, and Kitso 
explained how everything looke d so real. She also saw a brief clip from the News on 
telev i s i o n which showe d a planet in the sola r syste m, but when the next item refer r e d to 
Mount Ararat and that the ark in the Bible mi ght not have existe d in realit y , Kitso mixed 
the two items in her mind, and didn?t see how one relate d to the other. 
Over the subseq u e n t months , much of what  Kitso had seen blurred together in her 
mind, but she still reme mbe r e d the photog r a p h s of Mars and how cold it was in the 
planet a r i u m! 
4.5  Discussion 
This narrative shows some of Kitso?s thoughts as she watched the planetarium 
presentation, and is a composite  pictu r e , based princi p al l y on inter vi ew s with stude n t s . 
Althou g h Kaelo had a number of scienti f i c ideas before he  visited HartRAO, Kitso?s 
scient i f i c knowle d g e is  conside r a b l y less, which result s in less learni n g and unders t a n d i n g 
of what is being presented at the planetarium.  Her idea s about the star s as relat i v el y smal l 
(the size of the Earth), a nd the Sun being the biggest st ar did not change during the 
presen t a t i o n , even though the educat o r specif i c a l l y refer r e d to it. Simila r l y , her na?ve ideas 
about gravit y , the solar system and day and ni ght rema ine d unchang e d . Like Kaelo, Kitso 
had no prepar a t i o n for the visit and no follo w- u p afterw a r d s (altho u g h her inter v i e w only 2 
days after the visit did not allow mu ch time fo r this). Also, she talked to her cousin about 
the trip, and discuss e d it with some othe r school students the fo llowing week, indicating 
that aspect s of it had ma de an impres s i o n on her.  In fact, the effect of  the visit on Kitso was 
more to do with emotio n s , the affect i v e aspec t of learni n g , than on cognit i ve chang e . This 
is exami n e d in more detai l in Chapt e rs 7 to 9. 
The aim of this chapt e r was to show the r eader what it is like to visit the two sites 
throu g h the eyes of the parti c i p a n t s in the study. Althou g h ba sed on data collect e d during 
observ a t i o n s of the school visi ts , and intervi e w s with the student s , it is necessa r i l y 
107 
subjec t i v e , in that I have chosen which scenes  to highlig h t and which to omit. Howe ver , it 
is preci s e l y the subje c t i v e natu re of the narra t i v e that allow s me to illust r at e , in an 
ethnog r a p h i c style , impor t a n t point s which appea r  to be commo n to many schoo l visit s , not 
only in Johann e s b u r g but elsewh e r e . Severa l of these were ident i fi e d in Chapt e r 2, such as 
the disen g a ge me n t of teach e r s and the types of  learn i n g that occur in  scienc e centre s. The 
followi n g chapter s take a more objecti ve stance . Chapt er 5 shows the collec t i ve trend s in 
learn i n g acros s the entir e data set from the study, while Chapte r s 6 to 8 examine what and 
how individu a l students l earnt during their visit. 
108 
Chapter 5 
5  An Analysis of Collective Learning 
While Chapter 4 provided a narrative of what some students learnt in the 
process of a visit to a science centre, this chapter examines what the complete 
sample of students in my study learnt about astronomy during their visit to 
either the Johannesburg Planetarium or Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy 
Observatory visitors? centre. It attempts  to answer research question one using 
the concept of Big Ideas in scie nce as a framework for learning. 
5.1  Introduction 
O n e of the aims of my study was to examin e wh at the studen t s learnt from the visit to 
either the Johannesburg Planetarium or Ha rteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astro n o my Obse r v a t o r y . 
This chapter descri b e s and analyse s what the entire group of 34 student s collec t i v e l y learnt 
from their visit . It demo n s t r a t e s that cogn it i v e learni n g of astron o my topics occurr e d at 
differe n t levels across a range of student s . 
The chapt er is divide d into the follo wi n g secti o n s: 
? A descri p t i o n of the researc h sample and how the final 34 student s were selecte d 
from the origina l 57 from whom data were collect e d . 
? A reiteration of what I consider as  learnin g in the context of the study 
? T h e use of Big Ideas in astrono my as an organis i n g fra mewo r k for the analysi s of 
student s ? learnin g 
? What students collectively learnt from th eir visit to a study site, and how this 
relat e s to the liter at u r e on basic astr onomy and learning in science centres. 
? H o w Big Ideas are used with respec t to i ndividual learning in s ubsequ e n t chapter s . 
 
5.2  The Research Sample 
C h a p t e r 3 describ e s how I obtaine d the study sa mple of 145 Persona l Meanin g Maps fro m 
student s in seven school s (inclu d i n g a p ilot school ) . Howeve r , only 57 student s were 
inter v i e w e d both prior to and after their visit to  one of the study sites , and inter v i e w s from 
109 
one of these schools were used as pilot data . During transcr i p t i o n of  the data, and some 
initial analys i s , I decided to exclude two of the schools from the full analysi s . The first of 
these, Revela t i o n School , was exclude d beca u s e their visit to the planet a r i u m on 4 
Septemb e r was follow e d almos t immed i a t e l y by their schoo l vacat i o n which lasted over 
two weeks. Due to my absence from Johanne s b u r g later in Septemb e r , I was unable to 
compl e t e their follo w - up PMMs and inter vi e w s until 9 Octobe r , 35 days after their visit . 
The mean number of days after the visit when  data was collected in  the other four schools 
used was five (SD: 5). The princi p a l aim of  my study is to ascerta i n the learnin g that 
happen s durin g and as a result of studen t s ? visit to the planet a r i u m, and I consid e r e d that 
my follow-up data would likely have been influe n c e d by in tervening experiences, which 
may not have necessa r i l y happen e d at school .  
The other school exclud e d was St Augusti n e ? s School. In this ca se their visit to 
HartR A O took place on day one of a school ?tou r ? lasti n g five days and organ i s e d by a 
commer c i a l ?schoo l tours? compan y . I would not have had a probl e m with this, as it would 
have been intere s t i n g to determi n e what had b een learnt from the HartRA O visit as part of 
a longer tour involv i n g a consid e r a b l e num ber of new experi e n c e s . Howeve r , three 
incid e n t s resul t e d in my decis i o n not to analy s e the data from this schoo l in detai l . The first 
was that the school arrived ove r two hours late at HartRAO , potent i a l l y reduci n g the time 
they would have for the visit. The second in cide n t was that the ?tour guide? accompa n y i n g 
the classe s decid e d that they needed to le ave at midda y , thus cutti n g their visit even 
shorter , with the result that they spent only  sevent y minute s at HartRAO , compar e d with 
the 3 to 4 hours that other schoo l s spent . The teacher s accompa n y i n g the student s later 
expl a i ne d to me that this was out of their hands, th at they reach e d thei r dest i na t i o n for the 
day unnece s s a r i l y early, and should have spent at least a furth e r two hours at HartRAO . 
The third incident was at St Augustine?s School on 17 October, 11 days after their visit, 
when I had arran g e d for the stude n t s to re peat their PMM and be interviewed by me. 
Unfortunately this day was ?Civvies Day 1 1 ? , and the stude nts were in a genera l l y playf u l 
mood. In order for them to comp lete their PMM, they were as ked by their teac her to stop 
what they were doing (and apparen t l y en joyin g ; it was not nor ma l school work) and 
compl e t e their PMM. I noted at the time that most of the students tended to rush through 
the PMM to get back to the task they were doing before being inte rrup t e d . Further, during 
                                                 
 
11  A Civv ie s Day in South Afric a n scho o ls is a day on which the unif or m code is relax ed , and stud en ts can 
wear whatever clothing they like. It is of ten associated with an  even t or festiv al. 
110 
the inter vi ew s with each stude n t , sever al of th em appea r e d distr a c t e d , and wante d to get 
away to join the other stude n t s as part of the day?s festi v i t i e s . For these reason s I decide d 
not to use the data from this school, as I considered it would be  collected under very 
differ e n t circums t a n c e s compar e d w ith data from the other school s .  
In excluding these two schools, the fina l number of students interviewed was 34, 
from four schools, and I have summarised de mogr a p h i c and other releva n t informa t i o n 
about them (Tabl e 5.1). 
Table 5.1 Demographic information on sample of students 
Fictitious 
Name 
Student code School Age Gender Grade Home 
Language 
Population 
Group 
S ib ong ile scf06 Lourd e s 14 F 8 isiZu lu Blac k 
Susan scf08 Lourd e s 13 F 8 English White 
Lara scf10 Lourd e s 14 F 8 English White 
Helen scf11 Lourd e s 14 F 8 English White 
Sarah scf12 Lourd e s 14 F 8 English White 
Fati ma scf15 Lourd e s 14 F 8 English Colou r ed 
Kitso scf17 Lourd e s 15 F 8 seTs w an a Blac k 
Anto n ia scf19 Lourd e s 13 F 8 English White 
Sipho swo05 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 M 7 isiZu lu Blac k 
Zane le swo06 Balf o ur Fore s t 12 F 7 isiZu lu Blac k 
Bhek iw e swo07 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 F 7 isiZu lu Blac k 
Mpho swo14 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 F 7 seSo th o Blac k 
Banya n a swo20 Balf o ur Fore s t 12 F 7 seTsw an a Blac k 
Ntob e ko swo22 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 F 7 isiZu lu Blac k 
Nonk u lu le ko swo26 Balf o ur Fore s t 12 F 7 isiZu lu Blac k 
Doug las swo29 Balf o ur Fore s t 14 M 7 tshiV en d a Blac k 
Julius swo36 Balfour Forest 13 M 7 sePedi Black 
Neo swo42 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 F 7 sePed i Blac k 
Batsile swo53 Balf o ur Fore s t 14 M 7 seTsw an a Blac k 
Thap iso swo59 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 F 7 isiZu lu Blac k 
Phillip swo69 Balfo ur Forest 12 F 7 sePed i Black 
Brend a swo70 Balf o ur Fore s t 13 F 7 sePed i Blac k 
Tlotlo tsw02 Boka mo s o 13 F 8 seTs w an a Blac k 
Botho tsw04 Boka mo s o 13 F 8 seTs w an a Blac k 
Fane tsw08 Boka mo s o 13 M 8 seTsw an a Blac k 
Nnan ik i tsw15 Boka mo s o 12 F 8 seTs w an a Blac k 
Paul vho0 2 Achiev e me n t 13 M 7 English White 
Vick y vho0 6 Achiev e me n t 13 F 7 English White 
Ther esa vho0 9 Achiev e me n t 13 F 7 English White 
Judy vho1 0 Achiev e me n t 12 F 7 English Colou r ed 
Caro lin e vho1 1 Achiev e me n t 13 F 7 English White 
Rich a r d vho1 2 Achiev e me n t 13 M 7 English White 
Ross vho1 3 Achiev e me n t 12 M 7 seSo th o Blac k 
John vho1 6 Achiev e me n t 15 M 7 English White 
 
L o u r d e s Girls School visite d the planeta r i u m, while the other three school s visite d 
HartRAO. Of the 34 students, 74% (25) were gi rls , 62% (21) were bl ack and 6% (2) were 
111 
colour e d . The white and colour e d studen t s al l spoke English at home , while the black 
students spoke either isiZulu (7), seTswana (7 ), sePedi (4), seSoth o (3) or tshiVe nda (1 
stude n t ) at home. 13 stude n t s (38%) of the tota l sampl e state d that they spoke a second 
langu a g e at home, the commo n e s t ones being Eng lis h (7 student s ) and Afrikaa n s (4). 22 of 
the students (64%) were in grade 7, wh ile the remain i n g 12 were in grade 8. 
5.3  Learning in Science Centres 
I n Chapte r 2 I describ e d how learnin g can be underst o o d in the contex t of a school visit to 
a museu m or scien c e centr e . I stipu l a t e d a defi ni t i o n which I repeat here to set the scene for 
the breadt h of learni ng the students show.  
Learning is a process of active engage me n t with experie n c e . It is what 
people do when they want to make se nse of the world. It may involve the 
develop me n t or deepeni n g of skills, knowle d g e , unders t a n di n g , 
aware n e s s , value s , ideas and feelin g s  or an increa se in the capaci t y to 
reflec t . Effec t i v e learn i ng leads to ch ange , devel o p me n t and the desire to 
learn more (Braund & Reiss, 2004 p. 5)  
While few people would disagr e e with this , the idea of learnin g as concept u a l 
understa n d i n g or conceptu a l cha nge is held in high regard by  many South African teachers 
(e.g. Hlatshw a y o & Stanton , 2005; Potgiet e r , Engelbre c h t & Harding, 2006). From the 
point of view of many inte re s t e d partie s , from teache r s to funding source s aiming to 
increa s e the quali t y of scien c e and mathe ma t i c s progr a m me s , learni n g invol v e s an incr e a s e 
in knowle d g e and unders t a n d i n g on the part of the learne r . If studen t s go on a visit to a 
scien c e centr e , it might be expec t e d that th ey should improv e their knowledge in the area 
in which the scien c e cent r e speci al i se s . In orde r to compa r e this ?expe c t e d learni n g ? with a 
wider view of learni n g such as that expre ss e d by Braund and Reiss, I develop e d a set of 
categor i e s of learnin g using the notion of Big Ideas  to define key concept s in basic 
astro n o my for my study . 
 T h e notio n of Big Ideas come s from the Ameri c a n Assoc i a t i on for the 
Advanc e me nt of Scienc e ? s (AAAS) Projec t 2061 which develo p e d what it regard s as 
topics of import a n c e for liter a c y in scien c e , mathe ma t i c s and techn o l o gy . These theme s 
(importa n t concepts ) have been assigned be nchmarks (ideas and skills) which the AAA S 
conside r s all student s should be exposed to during their schoolin g as  far as grade 12 
(Ameri c a n Assoc i a t i o n for the Advanc e me n t of  Science, 1993). Project 2061 is a refor m 
initi a t i v e start e d in 1985 to improv e scien c e literac y among United States citizen s , and 
takes its name from the year in which Halle y ? s Come t will next be visib l e from Earth . In 
112 
their Atlas of Science Literacy  (Ameri c a n Assoc i at i o n for th e Advancement of Science, 
2001) the Project 2061 develop e r s map out differe n t  aspect s of science , such as the Nature 
of Scienc e , the Physic al Settin g and the Human Organ i s m, and ident i fy clust e r s and stran d s 
of scien c e withi n these categ or i e s . Their inten t i o n is to provid e a se ries of strand maps 
which educators such as teachers and curr ic u l u m devel o p e r s can  use to locat e the 
benchma r k s for science literac y within the bi gger picture . However ,  I was rece nt l y made 
aware by a planet a r i u m direc t o r that when th e nationa l standar d s were  drafted, astronome rs 
were only involve d at a late stage, and that  ?the astron o my portio n s were added as a kind 
of afterth o u g h t ? (Jim Beaber pers. comm.). 
Inste a d of the term ?theme ? or ?topi c ? the AAAS has started informal l y using the 
term Big Idea  (Americ a n Associ a t i o n for the Adva nceme nt of Science, 2005), a notion 
which I find useful to identif y key concept s for my study. The notion of Big Ideas has also 
been used by Loughr a n and collea g u e s in th eir develo pme n t of teache r s ? Pedago g i c a l 
Content Knowledg e (PCK), but they use the te rm to refer to scien c e ideas that teach e r s 
regard as being import a n t in the topic be ing studied (Loughran, Mulhall & Berry, 2004). 
Also, Big Ideas should not be confused w ith Big Science , a term coined by Alvin 
Weinberg in 1961 to refer to sc ience activities (such as space research or nuclear physics) 
which involv e expens i v e and elabor a t e equipme n t and large teams of scien t i s t s (Gold b e r g , 
1995). 
As my analy s i s proce e de d I indep e nde n t l y id entifi e d three Big Ideas which coinci de d with 
the strand maps of the AAAS unde r their cluste r ?The Univers e ?. The three Big Ideas are 
gravit y , stars and the solar syste m, and I also ident i fi e d a fourt h idea (size and scale) which 
the AAAS i ncludes in a separate theme co mmo n to other aspect s of scien c e , mathe ma t i c s 
and techno l o g y . I chose the Big Ideas for my study using three main crite r i a : (i) the exten t 
to which a concep t is fundame n t a l to the scie nce of astronom y ; (ii) the extent to which a 
concep t is refle c t e d in the rese ar c h litera t u r e on astron o my educa tion; and (iii) the extent to 
which either the planetarium or HartRAO a ddre s s e d the concep t in their exhibi t s and 
presentations. I chose two further S ignificant Ideas : the day/ni g h t cycle and the phases of 
the Moon. While these cannot be regarded as ideas fundame n t a l to astronomy , they have 
been extensi v e l y resea r c h e d and I regard them  neverth e l e s s as key concept s that were 
address e d , at least oblique l y , at my study si tes. The Big and Signifi c a n t Ideas I chose for 
my study are shown in Table 5.2, together with a summ a r y of how they ar e presented at the 
study sites and their frequenc y in the research literature. 
113 
Table 5.2 Big Ideas used in my study 
Big Ideas Peer-reviewed 
studies since 1975 
Presentation at 
planetarium 
Presentation at 
HartRAO 
Gravity 9 2 vide o clip s , 1 
referen c e 
4 demo n s t r a t i o n s , 1 
refere n c e . 
Stars (and Sun) 4 6 demo n s t r a t i o n s and 
expla n a t i o n s 
4 demo n s t r a t i o n s 
and expla n a t io n s 
Solar System 3 2 demon s t r a t i o n s 1 exten s i v e 
demo n s t r a t i o n 
Size and scale 1 1 expl a n a t i o n and 1 
extens i v e 
demo n s t r a t i o n 
1 expl a n a t i o n and 1 
extens i v e 
demo n s t r a t i o n 
Day and night 1 6 2 expla n a t i o ns for 
observ a t i o n s made 
Demon s t r a t i o n s of 
the ?sky movi n g? 
Phases of the 
Moon 
1 3 Moon land i ng slid e 
show; phase s not 
discuss e d 
Phot o s of Moon ; 
phases not discuss e d 
 
I also chose one item which I refer to as a Dominant Artefact : the satel l i t e dish. At 
HartRAO the 26m-dia me t e r radio telesc o p e do mi nates the whole site, and dishes are 
visib l e in a numbe r of place s , even on the ro ad leadin g to the observ a t o r y . Let me say a 
little more about the rationa l e fo r choos i n g each of these conce p t s . 
5.4  Big Ideas 
5.4.1  Gravity 
I chose the concept of gravity (as one of the four fundame n t a l forc es in the 
universe) as it is crucial in the understanding of astro n o my and cos mo l o g y . It is often 
cover e d at schoo l as a rather dry topic with i n the theme ?mech a n i c s ? , and as a resul t , 
student s ? underst a n d i n g of grav i t y in relat i o n to the Earth and the solar syste m is usual l y 
limit e d to solvi n g calcul a t i o n s and exam- s t y l e probl e ms . In the Atlas of Science Literacy  
(Ameri c a n Assoc i a t i o n for the A dvance me n t of Science , 2001) gr avi t y is regar d e d as being 
of suffici e n t import a n c e to be allocat e d it s own strand map. In the South African school 
system, gravit y has tradit i o n a l l y been covere d at senior primar y and junior second a r y level 
(grade s 7 to 8) as part of acceler a t i o n and forces. At higher grades , gravit y has been 
covered as part of Physical Science within  the grade 12 syllabu s  under Newton ? s Law of 
Gravi t at i o n . Sinc e the impl e me n t a t i o n of Curric u l u m 2005 and the Revise d Nation a l 
Curri c ul u m State me n t (RNCS ) gravi t y is now c overe d in the Senior Phase of the General 
Educat i o n and Traini n g band as ?The force th at keeps planets in orbit ? and governs ? 
114 
motio n in the solar syste m? (Depa r t me n t of  Education, 2002 p. 71). Within the newly 
imple me n t e d Furth e r Educa t i o n and Train i n g curric u l u m (Depar t me n t of Educat i o n 2003) 
gravit y is covere d under mechan i c s in grades 10 and 11.  
The concep t of gravit y was covere d quite differ e n t l y at the two sites in my study. 
At the planet a r i u m, where stude nt s are taken on a visual tour of the solar system, the 
prese n t e r used the word ?grav i t y ? 17 times during the visit by St Th eresa? s school (during 
the pilot study). This compare s quite favoura b ly with other astronomy-related words, for 
examp l e she used the word ?moon ? 52 times , ?p lan e t s ? 33 times , ?Plut o ? 22 times and ?gas? 
17 times . The conce p t of gravi t y was used in a variety of ways during the show, as follows.  
First the studen t s were shown a video clip  of a corona mass ejecti o n from the sun, 
and this was descri b e d as being rare, as the s un?s mass is held together due to gravity. The 
presen t e r state d ?The sun has about 28 times  as much gravity as the Earth which is 
useful.???If it didn?t had enough gravit y it would just sort of blow itself apart from 
heat.  Heat makes things expand.  Gravity ho lds things like the sun together.? (Theresa 
pltm 038).  
Secondly , student s were shown a video clip of how the solar system for med, 
whereby dust and gas accumulate together, an d are pulled by gravity to form a star and 
planet s . The presen t e r stated ?So you can progr a m m e a comput e r using maths to say what 
would gravit y do to all those bill ions of little bits of dust and gas in that ball over there ? 
and ?So what you end up with is huge lumps in the middle with a lot of gravit y and a lot of 
pressure.? (Theresa pltm 070). 
Thirdl y , with respec t to an artifi c i a l sate l l i t e , the prese nt e r descr i be d how it orbit s 
the Earth at speed, so that it can  remai n in orbit : ?On this pictur e here the inter n ati o n a l 
space station would be about th at di stance above the Earth which is why it has to keep 
moving.  If it stops mo ving what would happen to it?  Gravity will pull it down to the 
Earth.  It will fall out of the sky.? (Theres a pltm 122). 
Durin g the visit by Lourd e s Girls Schoo l , si milar explanations were used by the 
planet a r i u m presen t e r . She also ma de a stat em e n t that stres s e d the importance of gravity 
through o u t the solar system: ?Gra vity, okay so the sun has gravity the Earth has gravity all 
of these others have gravity as well so Me rcury pulls the sun, Mars pulls the sun, Jupiter 
pulls the sun hugely the sun isn?t standi n g still the sun goes entire l y in circle s so we can 
look at other stars to see if they are moving entire l y in circles and about hundre d s of the m 
115 
look like they are we assume beca us e they are pulled by the gravit y of planet s ? (Lourd e s 
pltm 353) 
These excerp t s show that studen t s are expos ed to the concept of gravity in the 
followin g four ways througho u t the show: 
? G r a v i t y is very high in a ma ssive object such as the sun, and it pulls not only material 
ejected from it, but also the ot he r bodie s in the solar syste m.  
? G r a v i t y is respons i b l e for ?pullin g togeth e r ? a proto solar system.  
? G r a v i t y pullin g an orbiti n g body is counter a c t e d by the body orbitin g at high speed,  
? A l l the solar syste m bodie s are pulli ng on each other due to the gravit y of their masses . 
At HartRA O gravit y was also referr e d to, but in a less didact i c and more ?hands - o n ? 
manner . Studen t s experi e n c e gravit y princi p a lly through an activity  using ?Coke bottle 
rocke t s? . Durin g this activi t y they attach two-l i t re plast i c bottl e s to a one-wa y valve withi n 
a metal frame , which acts as a launch e r , posit i o n e d vertic a l l y . The valve is attache d to a 
hand-operated air pump, which is used to pump  air into the bottl e . When the press u re 
inside the bottle reaches a critic al level, the bottle shoots out fr om the launc h e r into the air. 
Stude n t s then exper i me n t with diffe r e n t level s of water in the bottl e to see which will allow 
the ?rock et ? to fly to the highe st altit u d e . After the activi t y is compl e t e d the HartR A O 
educat o r would norma l l y discus s the studen t s ?  findin g s , referr i n g to gravi t y , the relat i ve 
amoun t s of water in the bottl e ,  and the altitud e reache d . 
A second way in which students ma y experien c e gravity at HartRAO is by handling 
Coke cans. Situa t e d on a table in the Visit or s  Centr e , there are ten 340ml Coca- C o l a cans, 
each label l e d with the name of one of the plane t s in the sola r syste m, as well as the Moon. 
The ?Plut o? can is empty , and a ll the others are filled with an  amount of sand such that the 
relative weights are in proportion to the weight they would be on the different planets. The 
idea is that by pickin g each one up and feel ing its weight , visito rs can understa nd the 
relative effect of gravity on each planet. Un lik e the rocke t activi t y  however, students are 
not directed by the HartRAO educator to carry out any activi t y with these cans. There is a 
label in the centre reading ?What would a C oke can weigh on each pl anet ? ? Like many 
exhibits in a science centre, vi sito r s can choose to intera c t w ith the displa y or not. All the 
groups in my study who visited HartRAO were  given time to wander around the visitors? 
centre and handle exhi bi t s . On each occasi o n , a number of student s chose to pick up the 
Coke cans. However, as there is no written explanation of the science involved students 
116 
have to use their prior knowl e d g e about weight, gravity and the planet s to unders t a n d their 
signi f i c a nc e , or they need to ask their peers,  the HartRAO educat o r or their own teacher. 
A third appr o a c h to studen t s ? expe r i e n c i n g the effect s of gravit y is by the use of 
calibrat e d ?bathroo m scales?. HartRAO has four scale s at the visit o r s? centr e , in the same 
area as the Coca-C o l a cans. As in the Coke can activi t y , stude n t s are not direc t e d to use the 
scale s , so a visit i n g stude n t may or may not get to inter ac t with the exhi bi t .  All scales are 
measu r e d in kilog r a ms and are calib r a t e d a ccord i n g to four bodies in the solar system: 
Earth, the Moon, the Sun and Jupiter. A student  standi n g on the ?Jupit e r ? scale would see 
what his or her weight would be  like on the planet , which is in tend e d to assist in a greate r 
unders t a n d i n g of the concep t of weight in re lati o n to gravit y . Again, like the Coke cans, 
there is no writt e n expla n a t i o n of the relat i o nsh i p of weight to gravity or size of the solar 
system body. The activit y theref o r e intend s to s timulate the student to find out more about 
the relatio n s h i p for themsel v e s . 
A fourth way of experiencing  the effects of gravity is by visitors ?trying to hit 
Jupit er with a comet ? . In this exhibi t , visi t or s slide a ball beari n g (repr e s ent i n g the comet ) 
around a flatten e d bowl-s h a p e d struct u r e (repre s e n t i n g the solar syste m) with a hole in the 
centre (repre s e n t i n g the S un) as explained in Kaelo?s narrative in Chapter 4. 
The fifth, and probably lesser, way in which gravity may be directly referred to at 
HartR A O is durin g the slide show, which in the case of the Balfo u r Fores t and 
Achievement School visits was about the m oon landings . During the s lide show the topic 
of gravity was mentione d in passing, for exampl e when asked about the lunar rover, one of 
the students suggested ?Their vehicle help th em to not float. Maybe their vehicle have 
gravit y [laugh t e r from peers] or weight?. However, the HartRAO educator did not follow 
up on this observa t i o n . 
In contr a s t to the plane t a r i u m, stude n t s at HartRAO had opportu n i t i e s to experie n c e 
the effec t s thems e l v e s . The two ma in exper i e n ce s being exper i me n t i n g with water -fi l l e d 
?rocke t ? bottle s and feelin g the simula t e d effect  of gravit y on planet s in the solar syste m . 
The extensi v e literat u r e on learnin g about  gravity is highlig hted in section 5.8.1. 
5.4.2  Stars and the Sun 
I chose stars and the Sun becau s e stude n t s have  direc t exper i enc e of them as objec t s in the 
sky and becaus e havin g knowl e d g e of their size, compo s i t i on and positi o n in space gives 
student s an underst a n d i n g of the scale of the solar syste m and unive r s e . Like the concept of 
117 
gravi t y , this is cruci a l to stude n t s ? overa l l unders t a n d i n g of astron o my , and the topic ?stars ? 
is alloca t e d its own stran d map in the Atlas of  Science Literacy (Ame rican Association for 
the Advanc e me n t of Scienc e , 2001). In the analysis of st udents? knowledge it was not 
always possible to separate what they knew ab out the Sun from their knowledge of stars, 
so they are treated together mu ch of the time in this chapt e r and elsewh e r e in the thesis . 
The RNCS refers to the following as  core knowled g e and concept s for the 
Inter me d i a t e Phase . ?The stars ? appar e n t posit i o ns in relat i o n to each other do not chang e, 
but the nightl y positi o n of the star patte r n as a whole chang e s slowly over the course of a 
year. Many culture s recogni s e a nd name star pattern s ? ? For th e Senior Phase it states: 
?The sun, an averag e star, is the centra l and largest body in the solar system? and ?The sun 
is the major source of energy for phenome n a on the Earth?s surfac e ? ? (Depar t me n t of 
Educati o n , 2002 p. 70-71) 
At the plane t a r i u m stars are a key part of the prese n t a t i o n: they are prese n t as a 
backdr o p for almost the entire durat i o n of the show. At the presenta t i o n given to Lourdes 
Girls School the planet a r i u m pres e n t e r used the word star or stars 116 times compa r e d with 
the words Mars (80 times ) , Earth (57 times ) a nd Sun (51 times). In additio n , the followi n g 
aspect s of stars were given specia l emphas i s : 
? T h e name s of stars and constel l a t i o n s e.g. Si rius and Orion; how to use a star chart 
to identi fy them. 
? A n explana t i o n of the star signs (zodiac ) 
? W o r k i n g out star composi t i o n using a spectrome t e r . 
? T h e Sun as the close st (not the bigge st ) star. 
? T h e dista nc e to the next cl oses t star: Alpha Centau r i 
? N e b u l a e and star format i o n 
At HartRAO stars are also given considerable prominen c e by the educator s in the 
follow i n g main ways: 
? A n explana t i o n of why stars appear  small and the Sun appears larger. 
? A de mons t r a t i o n of the differe n c e betw een ?living ? and ?dead? stars using a 
turnta b l e . 
? A de mons t r a t i o n of the Sun?s image, showin g sunspo t s . 
? A de monstr a t i o n of a large sundial in the car park. 
Stars and the Sun featu r e littl e in the resea r c h litera t u r e , and this was a further reason for 
includi n g them in my study. Comi ns (2001)  lists nume rou s mi sconc e p t i o n s that 
underg r a d u a t e studen t s hold about the Sun and star s, such as the Sun is not a star, and there 
are many stars in the solar system. Sharp (1996 ) includ e d childr e n ? s views on the nature of 
118 
the Sun and stars in his study of a variety of  astron o mi c a l concep t s . Roald and Mikals e n 
(2000) asked deaf and hearing children in No rway about the Earth and heaven l y bodies , 
includi n g stars, but found that interpr e t a t i o n of the data on st ars was difficult, due to the 
childre n ? s confus i o n betwee n as tro n o mi c a l stars and other bodi es which appear like stars. 
Other than occas i o na l quest i o n s in multi pl e choice  tests given as part of large r studi e s , the 
only substan t i a l report e d resear c h on stars is by Agan (2004) . Comp arin g high school and 
underg r a d u a t e studen t s , Agan used semi -s t r u c t u red interviews to find out students? ideas 
about what stars are, how far they are apart a nd whethe r the Sun is a star. Her result s claim 
that high school instru c t i o n can improv e stude nts ? knowledg e about stars, particul a r l y 
when related to nuclear fusion and energy product i o n in stars. 
5.4.3  Solar System 
I chose the solar system as a Big Idea as it is fundame nt al to students? scientific 
underst a n d i n g of the Earth in relatio n to the S un, the Moon and other planets. Like the first 
two Big Ideas describe d, the solar system is alloca t e d its own strand map in the Atlas of 
Science Litera c y (Ame ri c a n Associ a t i o n for the Advance me nt of Science , 2001). 
The RNCS Senior Phase Core Knowledge and Conce p t s refer s to the follo w i n g : 
?The earth is the third plane t from the sun in a system that includ e s the moon, the sun, 
eight other plane t s and their moons , and smalle r object s such as astero i d s and comets ? . It 
also contain s further referen c e s to the so lar system when describ i n g motion and gravity 
(Depar t me n t of Educat i o n , 2002 p. 70-71) . 
The planet a r i u m provi de s an animat e d vide o of the formation of a solar syste m 
with grav i t y pull i n g toge t h e r matt e r to form  a star, surroun d e d by orbitin g matter which 
develo p s into planet s . The plate- s h a p e d stru ctu r e of the system is de mo nst r a t e d . The bulk 
of the rest of the show is then a ?tour ? of all the plane t s of th e solar system, as well as so me 
suggestions that other stars might have their own solar systems. At HartRAO the educator 
questions students about the planets in the sola r system, descr i b e s thei r chara ct e ri s t i cs , and 
goes on to demons t r a t e , using a scale model , how far each planet is from the Sun. This 
latter demons t r a t i o n overla p s with th e fourth Big Idea: size and scale. 
Like ?Stars and the Sun?, researc h st udie s on people? s knowle d g e of the solar 
system are few, and this was one of the reason s  for inclu di n g the topic as a Big Idea on my 
study . Treag u s t and Smith (1989 ) inter v i e w e d grade 10 student s about  their understanding 
of the motion of the planet s and gravit y in  the solar syste m. Their study uncov e r e d many 
119 
misconceptions students possessed in relation to  gravity and the motion of the planets . 
Summer s and Mant (1995) in their study of primary teache r s used a true/fa l s e 
questi o n n a i r e to determi n e knowle d g e about a variety of astronomi c a l  concepts including 
the solar system. They found that the teache r s did not have a cl ear picture of what 
consti t u t e s the solar system, and the majori t y includ e d stars near to and among the planets. 
Sharp?s (1996) study in which he intervi e w e d 42 10- to 11-year - o l d childre n found that 
they held a variety of ideas about the solar system, but that  over half could describe a 
?scie nt i fi c? mode l of its struct u r e . 
5.4.4  Size and Scale 
Like the concept of gravity and the nature of  stars, most astrono me r s regard size (e.g. of 
heaven l y bodies , the solar system and the uni vers e ) and scale (e.g. distanc e s across the 
solar syste m and betwee n stars ) as being cr ucia l to an underst a n d i n g of astrono my . In the 
school classr o o m they are some of the most diff icu l t concep t s to get across to learner s due 
to the enormo u s distan c e s and scale of the univers e (Sadle r , 1998) and visitin g a 
planeta r i u m or telesco p e might  be expecte d to provide oppo rt u n i t i e s for demonst r a t i o n of 
scale not avail a bl e to the class r o o m teache r . The Atlas of Science Litera c y incorp or a t e s 
aspec t s of size and scale into the ?Stars ? and ?Galax i e s and Un ivers e ? strand maps. 
The RNCS does not make specif i c refere n c e to  size and scale , but they appea r in its 
?unify i n g state me n t ? for the knowle d g e stran d Planet Earth and Beyond which states ?Our 
plane t is a small part of a vast solar sy stem in an immens e galaxy? (Depar t m e n t of 
Educati o n , 2002 p. 69). Sharp (1996) is relativ e l y optimi s t i c that childr e n of primar y 
school age are capabl e of graspi n g ?compl e x and abstrac t informa t i o n ? about basic 
astron o my , and that ?compar i s o n s involv i n g rela ti v e size, distan c e , age and time were ? 
useful and familia r to childre n ? (p. 707 and 709). Conver s e l y , Sadler (1998) suggest s that 
?compr e h e n s i o n of vast astron o mi c a l scales a ppear s to remain beyond the reach of student s 
even after taking an Earth scie nc e course [or] astrono my cour se in high school? (p. 283). A 
simil a r conce p t to size and scale that I inve sti g a t e is student unders t a n d i n g of geologi c 
time, where the scale invol v e d is massi v e a nd difficul t to comprehe n d . Dodick and Orion 
(2003) in their study of grad e 7-12 students and ge ologic time suggest that around Grades 
7 and 8 students can begin to a pprecia t e it. This grade range is  simil a r to that of my own 
study (age 12 to 14), and I sugges t in sectio n 5.8.4 that student s are capable of a greater 
120 
understanding of size and scal e within the solar system a nd univer s e as a result of 
interac t i o n s during thei r scien c e centr e visit . 
At the planet a r i u m, the presen t e r explai n s why the Sun appear s so much bigger 
than other stars even th ough it is an ordina r y st ar, as a result of the immense distance the 
stars are away. She then demon s t r a t e s just how  far it is from the Sun to the next neare st 
star (Alph a Centa u r i ) by compa r i n g the dista n c e acros s the solar syst e m with the dista n c e 
betwe e n the 2 stars. By calcul a t i ng a rela tiv e l y simple divisi o n sum (as describ e d in 
Kaelo?s narrat i v e in Chapter 4)  she shows that it would ta ke about 4000 years to reach 
Alpha Centaur i , travell i n g at speeds twenty- t i m e s faster than our current spacec r a f t . At 
HartRAO, the educator uses an analogy of cl ose and distan t trees to explai n why the Sun 
looks so much large r than the stars . The exte nsi v e activit y of ?taking the solar system for a 
walk? uses a scale model of the solar syste m reduc e d 4 billion times , to pace out dist an c e s 
between the Sun and planets. In  additio n to these more substa ntial presentations, both sites 
referr e d to size and scale in other discus s i o n s , such  as the size of the Earth in relat i o n to the 
Sun and Moon at the planet a r i u m and when viewing sunspots at HartRAO. 
Relati v e l y few studies have incorpo r a t e d si ze and scale into their invest i g a t i o n . In 
their 1992 study of British primary school  teachers? knowledge of astro n o mi c a l 
pheno me n a , Summe r s and Mant (1995 ) concl u de d that few had an accurate knowledge of 
scale of the Earth- S u n system, wher ea s 85% kne w that the Moon is sm aller than the Earth. 
Sadler?s quantitative study of 1250 grade 8- 12 students in the USA (Sadler 1998) had one 
questio n on the distanc e between the Sun and the closest star, which the majorit y of 
student s were not able to answ er accurate l y . Trumper used Sa dler?s question and 2 others 
of the scale of the Earth and the Sun in  his studie s (Trumpe r , 2001a; 2001b) . Trumpe r 
conclu d e d that this topic was one of the w eakest areas of high school students? knowledge, 
with only 20-25 % answe r i n g these quest i o n s corre c t l y . More recent l y Agan (2004 ) has 
shown that high school students we re able to speak of ?great distances between stars?, but 
only the underg r a d u a t e studen t s could rela t e the dista nc e s to a scale mode l . 
5.5  Significant Ideas 
5.5.1  Day and Night 
I n contra s t to ?stars? , an underst a n d i n g of  the day/n i g h t cycle ha s been investigated 
exten si v e l y in the resear c h litera t ure , and I consid e r e d that it would be a useful idea to 
exami n e . The Atlas of Scien c e Litera c y incorp ora t e s the day/ n i g h t cycl e into the ?Grav i t y ? 
121 
and ?Solar Syste m? stran d maps. In the RNCS there is speci fi c refer e nc e to the day/ni g h t 
cycle in the Intermed i a t e Phase: ?Day and night may be explained by the rotation of the 
earth on its own axis as it circl es the sun?. Simil a r l y , at the Senio r Phase there is refer e n c e 
to the motion of the Earth explai n i n g th e day (Depar t me n t of Educat i o n , 2002 p. 69). 
The concep t of the Earth spinni n g , howeve r , is referred to only briefly at both the 
study sites . At the plane t a r i u m there are sever a l demons t r a t i o n s of the stars, Moon and Sun 
moving fast across the dome, and the presen t e r ma ke s refe r e n c e to the fact that this is the 
Earth spinnin g or rotat i ng rathe r than a real mo veme n t of the stars . At HartR A O there are 
two opport u n i t i e s for the educat o r s to refer to the Earth spinnin g . First, during the 
demons t r a t i o n of the Sun?s image projec t e d on to a card, where the image moves over the 
course of a few minutes , and secondl y when th e sundia l is demons t r a t e d to show how the 
Sun can be used to tell the time. In both these cases , the educa t o r s expla i ne d that the 
move me n t was due to the Earth rotati n g . 
There are more than 16 peer-r e v i e w e d articl e s examin i n g studen t s? and teache r s? 
ideas about day and night. A st udy by Baxter (1989) showed that  from age 9 to 16 a larger 
proport i o n of the older childre n in the sampl e used  a scientific explanation for day and 
night, but that miscon c e p t i o n s persisted throughout the sample . A very infl u e n t i a l paper by 
Vosniadou and Brewer (1994) explained 6- to 11-year- o l d children ? s understanding of day 
and night in terms of menta l model s : from in itia l (na?ve ) throug h synt het i c to scienti f i c , 
and again found a progress i o n towa rds the scien tific notion in the ol dest children. These 
results have been confirme d by subsequ e n t researc h e r s such as Sharp (1996), Sadler 
(1998), Roald and Mikalsen (2001) and Trumpe r (2001a, 2001b) in children , and Summe rs 
and Mant (1995) and Atwood and Atwood (1995) in primary teache r s . Kikas (1998) 
conduc t e d a longitu d i n a l study in which she found that a group of 20 students were able to 
reme mbe r the (textb o o k ) scient i f ic explanation for day and ni ght 2 months after teaching. 
Howeve r she found 4 years later their scientific know le d g e was not remembe r e d and they 
relied heavily on everyda y experie n c e to explain the phenome n o n . Albanes e et al . (1997) 
strong l y critic i s e the work of Baxter ( 1989) and Vosniadou and Brewer (1994), and by 
impli c a t i o n the subse q u e n t paper s based on th eir mental mo del theori e s , and suggest that 
further research on this topic is needed. 
122 
5.5.2  Phases of the Moon 
L i k e the day/ni g h t cycle, th e phases of the Moon are a concep t which has been studie d 
exten si v e l y . The Atlas of Scien c e Litera c y incor p o r a t e s the lunar cycle into the Solar 
System strand map. At the Interme d i a t e Phas e of the RNCS core knowledge about the 
Moon includes ?The moon?s apparent shape changes in a predictable way and these 
change s ma y be explai n e d by its motion relati v e to the earth and sun? (Depar t me n t of 
Education, 2002 p. 69). There is further referenc e to an explanation of the Moon phases at 
the Senior Phase . 
At the study sites the Moon is discussed in several contexts , for example at the 
planeta r i u m the path of the Moon through the sk y is demonst r a t e d , and photogr a p h s of its 
surface are shown. At HartRAO there is a gian t (3m diamet e r ) model of the Moon donate d 
by NASA (and therefor e ?upsid e down? from our point of view in the southern 
hemi s p h e r e ! ) which stude n t s are referr e d to a nd the Moon is discuss e d in relatio n to the 
Earth when the scale of the solar syste m is expla i n e d . Howev e r , at neith e r site are the 
phases of the Moon specifically discussed, mo de l l e d or explain e d in terms of the Moon 
orbitin g the Earth with sunligh t fallin g on diffe r e n t amoun t s of the Moon? s surfa c e as 
viewed from Earth over 28 days. It would ther e f o r e not be expec t e d that stude nt s in the 
study improve their underst a n d i n g of the Moon phases after the visit. 
I identi f i e d more than 10 peer-r e v i e w e d articl e s discus s i n g pe ople?s understanding 
of the Moon phases . In contra s t to the majori t y  of older children being able to explain day 
and night, knowle d g e of the cause of the Moon phases is genera l l y poor in school - a g e 
children , tertiary students and teachers (e.g. Barnet t & Morran, 2002; Baxter , 1989; 
Summer s & Mant, 1995). Howeve r , a number of  resea r c he r s have been able to show 
substan t i a l increas e in student unders t a n d i n g as a direct result of instructional activity (e.g. 
Stahly, Krockov e r & Shepards o n , 1999). 
5.5.3  Dominant Artefact: Parabolic/Satellite Dish 
I chose this as the domi n a n t  artefa c t of the study becaus e  of the radio telescope at 
HartRAO which dominate s the entire visit to the site. Just as the back drop of stars strongly 
infl u e n c e s a visit to the plane t a r i u m, the para b o l i c dish of the teles c o pe is the main centr e 
of attraction at HartRAO. Since the advent a nd increas i n g popular i t y of  satellite television 
in the 1990s, dishes are common sights on buildin g s in most ur ban settings, and students 
could be expec t e d to be fa mil i a r with their shape .  
123 
Not surpri s i n g l y , the RNCS does not make sp ecif i c refere nc e to satell i t e dishes , but 
does refer to satell i t e s as object which may be observed in the sky (Foundat i o n Phase), and 
both Earth-b a s e d and orbitin g teles c ope s (Seni or Phase ) . 
The planet a r i u m prese n t a t i o n s made no refe r e n c e to satel l i t e s or dishe s, altho u gh 
they did show dishes fixe d to spacec r a f t as pa rt of the slide show. At HartRAO the 2 main 
ways in which parabo l i c dishes were de monst r a t e d were the ?whisp e r  dish? activi t y and the 
explan a t i o n of how the main telesc o p e func ti o n s . During the activit y , pairs of student s 
whispe r e d into parabol i c dishes placed 20m apart , and the educa t o r expla i n e d how sound 
waves are focus e d in the dish and refle c t e d from  one to the other . She furt h e r compa r e d the 
parab o l i c shape with torch refl e ct o r s, satel l i t e  dishes and the radio telescope. One of the 
highlights of the visit for the st udents was standing close to th e radio telesco p e dish (Figure 
4.11) and seeing it move to point to a particul ar part of the sky. Th e explanation of the 
telesc o p e ? s shape and functi o n , togeth e r with the playin g of sounds r ecord e d from stars, 
allowed students to question the educator s about various aspects of the site.  
The only study invol v i n g whisp e r dishe s repor t e d in the liter a t ur e is one by 
McClaf f e r t y (1995) in which he found that some  visit o r s were able to learn from the 
exhibi t . Others howeve r , were unable to tran sfer their prior knowle dge of reflect i o n and 
focussi n g to the new context of the whisper dish, sugges t i n g the situat e d nature of 
knowledge that others have described (Lave & Wenger, 1991).  
5.6  Coding for Big Ideas 
T h r o u g h o u t the rest of this thesis , the eight c oncep t s in astron o my I have identi f i e d as Big 
and Signif i c a n t Ideas as well as the Domi na n t Artefa c t will be refer r e d to colle ct i ve l y as 
Big Ideas. For each Big Idea, I develop e d hier arc h i c a l categor i e s of student knowled ge (1 
highest and 3 lowest) based on how the stude nts responde d to the intervie w question s 
during their pre and post-int e r v i e ws . The Bi g Idea categori e s and cr iteria for assigning 
studen t s ? views to a catego r y are self-e x p l a n a t o r y and are shown in Table 5.3. Each student 
was placed into a category for each of the Big Ideas they demons t r a t e d during their 
struct u r e d inter vi e w , both prior to and after the visi t to the study site. As exemplar s , I 
examine each Big Idea and show how stude nt s were categor i s e d in section 5.6.1. 
 
124 
Table 5.3 Criteria for assigning students to  knowledge categories for Big and Signif icant Ideas and the Dominant Artefact 
Big or Significant Idea Knowledge Leve l 1* Knowledge Level 2 Knowledge Level 3 
Grav ity con cept ? the exten t of 
know ledg e and und er stand ing 
of grav ity 
Grav ity as a pullin g (or push in g) 
force only. No add ition al knowledg e 
or und e r s tan d in g 
1.5 Ver y limi te d und er s tand ing of 
grav ity, one correct idea, but no 
con cep tu al under stand ing . 
Limited unde r stan d ing of how grav ity pulls 
towar d s a body. Misc on c e p tion s appa r en t (e.g . 
no grav ity in space, on Moon etc.) 
2.5 : Basic scien tific und erstan d in g, but still with 
at least 1 misco n c ep tion . 
Sub stan tial scien tific und erstan d in g reg ard in g 
mass and grav ity. No maj o r misco n cep tion s 
appar en t. 
Star conc ep t ? the exten t of 
know ledg e and und er stand ing 
of stars. 
Min i mal idea: stars as ligh ts at nigh t, 
no correct referen ce to comp o sitio n, 
no scientifically correct idea of size, 
litt le or no idea of position in space 
2: Star s as suns. Burn in g ball of gas. 
Misco n cep tio ns in terms of e.g . size, 
composition, and position in space. 
Fully scien tific exp lan ation . Includ ing : Ball of 
burning gas, referenc e to H/He. Correct idea 
of size & positio n in space. May inclu d e 
misc o n c e p tion( s ) or an omiss io n rega rd ing 
composition, distance or size 
Sun conc ep t ? the exten t of 
know ledg e or under s tand ing of 
the Sun 
The most naive lev el of con cep tio ns: 
e.g. sun bigg er & brigh te r than stars , 
sun-b y- da y & stars -b y-n igh t, ball of 
fire , sun hot, sun ligh t, sun huge, uses 
for drying , for plan t s 
Sun as star, sun as bigge s t star . Some indic a tio n 
of more deve lop e d than lower conc e p t. No 
refer e n c e to diff e r en c e as being related to 
dista n c e or wrong idea of dista n c e . Other sun 
facts may include: energy, gas, sunspots etc. 
Sun as closest, not biggest star, i.e. idea of 
scale . Indic a tion of more develo p ed 
knowledg e than lower con cepts. Prop erties 
may name gases , refe r to nucle a r ener g y, UV 
lig h t, age, future 
Solar System ? the exten t of 
know ledg e abou t the 
comp o s itio n and shap e of the 
solar syste m. 
No scien tific knowledg e of what 
solar syste m is. 
Knowledg e of what con stitu tes solar system, but 
misconceptions prevalen t (e.g. shape, stars in 
solar syste m) 
Sub stan tial scien tific kno wled g e of solar 
syste m, including shape. No major 
misconceptions. 
Size and scale ? the extent of 
know ledg e and und er stand ing 
of size, mass and distan ce 
(exc lu d in g grav ity) 
Conf u sed and conf lictin g know ledg e 
regardin g size, mass and distan ces. 
2: Some correct ideas regard in g size, mass and 
distance. Some incorrect ideas and 
misconceptions. 
2.5: Basic scien tif ic und e r s tan d in g, but with at 
least 1 misconception. 
Scien tif ic und er s ta nd ing of size, mass and 
distance in the solar syste m and beyo nd. 
125 
Big or Significant Idea Knowledge Leve l 1* Knowledge Level 2 Knowledge Level 3 
D a y and nigh t ? an explan a tion 
for why the Sun appe ar s to 
move across the sky 
Stud en t show s confu s ion regar d in g 
the conc ep t or stud en t may show 
misco n cep tions such as day and nig h t 
rev er sed , revo lu tio n arou nd sun as 
reas on for sun move men t. 
Scien tific con cep tio n dev elo ps as a resu lt of 
using mode l. May have refe rr e d to Earth 
revo lv in g /o rb itin g aro und sun as initial 
explan a tion of day and nigh t, but by 
manip u la ting mode l, deve lop s corr e c t 
conc ep tion . 
Full scien tific con cep tion from start of 
explan a tion : Earth spinn ing on axis. 
Phases of the Moon Little, confu sed or no idea of cau se 
of Moon phas es.  
 
2: Exp lan atio n invo lv es a mis co n cep tion e.g. 
Earth 's shado w. Or limi ted exp lan ation but not 
wro ng . 
2.5 : Partial scien tific: exp lanatio n has some 
refer e n c e to sun shin in g or 'ref le c tin g ' on moon, 
but no ref to Moon orb iting Earth . No obv iou s 
misc o n c e p tion . 
Full scien tific exp lan ation referring to the 
Moon orb itin g the Earth and sun ligh t shin in g 
on part of the Moon . 
Domin an t Artefact: 
Parab o lic/satellite dish 
Little or no idea of idea of 'collectio n 
of sign als' or similar, but not able to 
expr ess prop er ly. Direction not 
correct. 
2: eith e r corr e ct explan a tion of waves boun c in g 
or corr e c t pointin g dire c tion (but not both) 
2.5: correct explanation of wav es boun cing and 
correct pointing di re c tion but need ed 
consid e r ab le prob in g. 
Corr e c t expla na tio n of waves boun c ing and 
corr e c t poin ting dire c tion with no prob in g. 
* one student (Nonkululeko) had no idea what gravity was, and was classif i e d as knowled g e level 0.  For all other Big Ideas, stu dents had at least 
a mini mu m level of knowled g e , and so the lowest knowled g e level was 1. 
126 
5.6.1  Helen and gravity 
The students in Lourdes Girls School, who visited the Johann e s b u r g Planet a r i u m, were not 
asked about gravit y in their stru c t u r e d inter v i e w , but Helen re fer r e d to it in her PMM, and I 
questi o ne d her on the basis of what she had writte n . In her PMM Helen had written that 
there is ?No gravity in  space so you float around? (scf 11pre 22). The tran script of the 
inter v i e w is as follo w s : 
Inter v i e w e r : What is gravi t y ? 
Hele n : It?s a grav?  It?s a pull that hold us on Earth . 
Interv i e w e r : Uh-hmm . 
Hele n : Umm.  Ther e is no gravi ty in space so we just float aroun d if we 
went there. 
Interv i e w e r : Uh-hmm . 
Helen:   Like the astron a u t s and the spaces h i p s . 
Inter v i e w e r :   Uh-hmm .  Okay .  What about if you would go to the moon?  Is 
there gravi ty on the moon? 
Helen :   No.  You would just float aroun d . 
Interv i e w e r : Uh-hmm .  Is there gravit y on the sun? 
Helen :   Umm.  Nnnn? . N o . 
(scf1 1 p r e p m m 45-65 ) 
In this excha n g e , Helen gives a brief defi n i t i o n of gravi t y as a pull that holds us on the 
Earth, and indica t e s that there is no gravity in space. When probed, she also suggest s that 
there is no gravity on the Moon or on the Sun. According to my gravity criteria Helen has 
basic knowledge of gravity as a ?pull? and ha s misconceptions that there is no gravity in 
space, on the Moon or the Sun. She is theref o r e placed in categor y 2 for gravity prior to her 
visit .  
After the visit she added to her PMM, wr iting under her previous statemen t that 
there is no gravity in space the additional not e ?But the planets do have gravity? (scf11pos 
13). She further wrote that ?Each planet ?h olds the sun in place by means of their 
gravit y ? (scf11 p o s 15). When questi o n e d on these new stateme n t s she had writte n , the 
inter v i e w went as follow s : 
Inter v i e w e r : Tell me about this, you say plane t s in our solar sy ste m hold the 
sun in place by means of their gravi ty .  Tell me a littl e bit about 
that. 
Helen : Okay . The sun doesn ? t stand s til l .  It moves arou n d .  If you look 
throug h a telesc o p e you can actual l y see it wobble a bit from ti me 
to time. 
127 
Intervi e w e r : Okay . 
Helen :   And becaus e of our gravit y .  The sun ?  Hold.  The sun? s 
gravi t a t i o n holds us in place, but we also hold the sun in place by 
our, by the means of our gravi t a t i o n a l pull.  And the plane t s . 
Intervi e w e r : Uh-hmm.  Okay .  Umm.  Okay .  You? re say ing that there? s no 
gravi ty in space .  You?v e said befor e ?  And the plane t s do have 
gravi ty .  What ??  Umm, what is gravi t y relat e d to?  Do you 
know ?  Why do plan e t s have grav i ty ? 
Helen :   To keep things onto them, like their ?  Like ?  We would n? t 
float away . 
Interv i e w e r : Yeah. 
Helen :   So, we could be ? Stuck to the groun d. 
Inter v i e w e r : And ?  Umm, so, which ones ??  Do they have the same 
amou n t of grav i t y or is it diff e r e n t on diffe r e n t plan e t s ? 
Helen :   They?r e differ e n t . 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Why is that?  What?s it relat e d to? 
Helen :   I?m not sure. 
Inte r v i e w e r : Okay .  Like the moon or ? you don? t know ? 
Helen :   The moon is ?  If we wen t like, right here ?  Here ? Umm, we 
?  If, let?s say, I weigh 40kg here [on Earth ], on the moon I?ll 
weig h abou t 10. 
Helen:   Becaus e the gravit a t i o n a l pull is less. 
Helen :   And it pulls you more, yeah, it pulls you in. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  And suppo s i ng you are on Jupit e r ?  Would it be great e r or 
lesser? 
Helen : Great e r , I think.  The bigger the planet is I think, the mor e gravit y 
you? v e got. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  (Pause) Where did you learn that? 
Helen :   I don?t reme mb e r . 
Inter v i e w e r : You just knew it? 
Helen :   Yes, I think. 
(scf 1 1 p o s p m m 01-0 7 & 37-75 ) 
Helen? s knowle d g e was quite differ e n t from that  prior to the visit . Not only does she have 
a sophis t i c a t e d under s t a ndi n g of how the Sun holds the plane t s in space (and vice versa ) by 
means of gr avi t y , but also that the Moon and pl anets have gravity and the cause of gravity 
is related to mass of the body. In Braund and Reiss?s terms (2004), she has deepene d her 
knowledge and understanding of grav ity . While it is not certai n  whether she still holds a 
miscon c e p t i o n that there is ?no gravit y in space? or on the Sun, her understanding of the 
concep t of gravit y appea r s to be modera t e l y well develo p e d . I theref o r e placed her in 
gravit y catego r y 3 as a result of  this post- v i s i t inter v i e w . 
128 
5.6.2  Helen and stars 
A l l the studen t s who visite d the planet a ri u m we re asked about stars in the follow i n g way 
?Stars at night look like pinpr icks of light. Why? What are stars? ? Helen?s response before 
the visit was as follo w s : 
Helen :   Umm.  They ? r e masse s that give off their own light and heat and 
that? s how and that? s why they twink l e and the light takes a long 
time to trave l to us. 
Intervie w e r :   So why are they so tiny? 
Helen:   Becaus e they are far away from us. 
Intervi e w e r : Uh-hmm.  What size are th ey  appro x i ma t e l y ?  I mean, in terms of 
a moon, Earth , sun, what sort of size are the stars ? 
Helen:   Oh, they can be differ e n t sizes. 
Interv i e w e r :  Uh-hmm.  Give me so me range. 
Helen:   They can go ? well, there is anothe r ?  Umm, well, a star that is 
as big as our sun?s o me kilom e t e r s or light -y e a r s away from us 
and then there are sta rs as big as the mo on and the Earth. 
Intervi e w e r :  Okay .  What are they mad e of? 
Helen :   Umm.  Mass. 
Interv i e w e r : Mass being what? 
Helen:   Rock? More or less ? 
(scf1 1 p r e i n t 146- 1 70 ) 
Helen clear l y has some scient i f i c knowled g e of stars: she kn ow s that they emit thei r own 
light and heat as well as having some idea of their si ze, although her knowledge here 
appear e d insecu r e . She also knows that they  are ?far away? which results in their 
appea r a nc e being so tiny. One misco n c e p t i o n she has regar ds their compo s i t i o n: she think s 
that they are made of rock, but  again appears to be uncertain. I classi f i e d her at level 2 in 
my criter i a becau s e altho u g h she didn?t know the composi t i o n of stars, she had some idea 
of their positio n in space and size, as well as their product i o n of heat and light. 
After the visit, Helen? s respon s e was simila r , but showed that she now reme mbered 
that stars are made of gas, but did not el abora t e enough about their compos i t i o n size or 
positio n to be classif i e d as level 3: 
Helen:   Stars are mas ses, no, they ?re balls of burning gasses that umm, 
produ c e ligh t and heat, whic h is their own light and heat. 
Intervie w e r : Uh-hmm.  Okay .  What sort of size are they? 
Helen:   Umm, they ? re big.  They can actual l y ?  I think they can vary 
but normal l y they? r e as big as our sun. 
(scf 1 1 p o s i n t 82-8 6 ) 
129 
Helen was theref o r e classi f i e d as having a si mila r concep t i o n of stars from pre- to post-
 v i s i t . While she had not acqui r e d addit i o na l miscon c e p t i o n s or errone o u s knowle d g e , she 
had also not substa n t i a l l y changed her knowledg e about stars. 
5.6.3  Helen?s conception of the Sun 
In her pre- and post-visit interviews I aske d Helen ?Tell me anything you know about the 
Sun?. Her answers were relativ e l y brief, a nd I did not probe her very much. Before her 
visit she state d : 
Helen:   Umm.  It con sis t s of lots of gases, then it gives light to all the 
plane t s aroun d it and then it?s the centr e of our solar sy ste m. 
(Laug h s ) 
Interv i e w e r : Okay .  Umm.  What are the gases doing? 
Helen :   They form heat. 
Intervi e w e r :  Okay .  Do you know the name s of any of them? 
Hele n :   Umm.  No.  I dunn o . 
(scf1 1 p r e i n t 87-95 ) 
She also knew that the Sun is regard e d as a star, as when she was asked for the names of 
any stars she knew, she referr e d to the Sun. On these bases, I classif i e d Helen as having 
knowled g e about the Sun in categor y 2. Her pos t visit responses to the same questions 
were very similar , and she added nothing to her ideas about the Sun. 
5.6.4  Helen?s conception of  the solar system 
L i k e other student s visitin g the plane t a r i u m, I asked Helen what the solar syst e m is, and 
probed by asking what it consist s of and what shape it is. Prior to the visit , Helen alrea d y 
had a scienti f i c concept i o n of the solar system, but I classif i e d her as knowled g e level 2 on 
the basis that she thoug ht the solar syste m cont a i n e d stars (in additi o n to the Sun) and was 
oval shape d , like a plum: 
Helen:   It consis t s of planet s , the stars, umm, the sun, our ?  The centre 
of our u ? eer?.s o l a r sy ste m, yes.  And then nine planet s and 
then they all have moons , (paus e ) yeah. 
(Inte r r u p t i o n on schoo l P.A. sy ste m) 
Intervi e w e r : Umm.  What shape is ? the solar sy stem? 
Helen :   Umm.  Umm. An oval shape ? 
Inter v i e w e r : Uh-hm m .  Oval ?  Oval like a plum or oval like on a flat 
surface? 
Helen :   Oval like a plum. 
Inte r v i e w e r : Okay .  Umm.  How?d you know ? 
130 
Helen :   Cause we?ve done proje c t s on this. 
(scf1 1 p r e i n t 65-79 ) 
After the visit her knowl e d g e was very simil a r , except that she also inclu d e d the Milky 
Way in her conce p t i o n of the solar syste m, and I again class i f i e d her as being at knowl e d g e 
level 2. 
5.6.5  Helen?s conception of size and scale 
H e l e n showed a relativ e l y soph isti c a t e d concepti o n of distance s , sizes and the scale of the 
solar syste m and unive r s e prior to the visit. She was one of only 6 students who referred to 
the conce p t of the light year as a measu r e of distanc e , and her knowled g e of the relativ e 
sizes of the Earth , Moon and Sun was scient i f i c a l l y accura t e . Her knowle d g e of star size as 
being rough l y the same as the Sun was cleare r  after the visit, and she reme mb e r e d the 
length of time it would take to reach the ne arest star to our solar system (4000 years) 
whereas she had guessed at 100  years in her pre- v i s i t inter v i e w . On the basis of her 
relativ e l y advanc e d concep t i o n s of scale, I classi f i e d her as bei ng at knowle d g e level 3 
both pre- and post-visit. 
5.6.6  Helen?s understanding of day and night 
T h e extensi v e l y - r e s e a r c h e d Big Idea of the day/ ni g h t cycle has been discus s e d in Chapte r 2 
and secti o n 5.5.1 . Prior to her visit Helen appe ared to be confused about day and night. 
Like several other studen t s she used the term revol v e without clari fy i ng whether she really 
meant revolv e or rotate, and when supplie d w ith a model of the Eart h and Sun was unable 
to explai n the Sun?s appare n t moveme n t across the sky. For these reason s , Helen was 
classif i e d at knowled g e level 1 for the day/nig h t cycle: 
Helen:  Becaus e the Earth revol v e s . 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Can you, if I gi ve you a littl e model like that and can you 
and let me get my sun out, if you got the sun and you turn it on, 
have the sun on one side, show me what? s goin g on. 
Helen :   Okay .  Here you have the sun. 
Intervi e w e r : Yeah. 
Helen :   Then the Earth moves , umm, this way? 
Interv i e w e r :   Uh-hmm. 
Helen:   Wait, wait, this way . 
Intervi e w e r : Okay .  
Helen:   And then keep s like that. 
131 
Interv i e w e r : So, ?  What?s that side at the mo ment?  What? s??   When you 
have the sun movi n g acro s s the sky ever y d ay .  What? s on this 
side? 
Hele n : Noth i n g . 
Intervi e w e r : Okay .  And the other si de?  Okay .  So, so on this side? 
Helen:   Yes? 
(Blan k on tape.  About 10 secon d s . ) 
Inter v i e w e r : What? s going on with the sun moving over the sky there? 
Helen :  It moves fro m West to East.   
Interviewer:  Okay .  Right.  Okay , thanks. 
(scf1 1 p r e i n t 99-13 2 ) 
Howev e r , in her post- v i si t inter vi e w Helen was able to artic ul a t e the reaso n for the Sun?s 
appare n t mo veme n t fluent l y . Unfort u n a t e l y I di dn?t probe her about her change in idea, but 
clearl y some clarif i c a t i on ha d taken place in her mind over th e week between the first and 
second interv i e w . Some t hi n g may have increas ed her reflective capa cit y (Braun d & Reiss, 
2004). On the basis of the followi n g transcr i p t , I classif i e d her knowled g e level as 3: 
Helen:   It? s because we rotate. 
Interv i e w e r : Uh-hmm .  Okay .  So, with the model we got here.  If the sun is 
there , what? s the Earth doing ? 
Helen :   The Earth is going this way like a? 
[Demo n s t r a t e s the Eart h spin n i n g on its ax is and orbiti n g the Sun with the latte r 
statio n a r y ]. 
Intervi e w e r : Okay . So what? s actuall y caus i n g the sun to move?  Appar e n t l y 
?  Is it the movi n g roun d of the sun or is it the rotat i n g ? 
Helen :   Rotati n g . 
(scf 1 1 p o s i n t 57-6 5 ) 
5.6.7  Helen and the phases of the Moon 
Helen was one of only five stude nts in the study who, prior to he r visit, was able to explain 
why the Moon?s shape appear e d differ e n t over the course of a month. Most student s had 
some diffic ulty with expressing themselves  when asked about the cause of the Moon?s 
phases , and Helen was no except i o n . Howeve r , she was able to explai n the lunar cycle in 
terms of the Moon orbiting the Earth (although she initially said th e Sun, she corrected 
herse l f ) and the Sun?s light falli n g on di ffere n t amounts of the Moon?s surface , thus: 
Helen:   Becaus e, umm, it refl ec t s like the sun? s light and its shadow , 
so me, so me parts are differ e n t , like it revo lv e s around the sun, the 
Earth ? t h e Earth and then the sun?s light cover s so me parts of the 
moon and somet i m e s other parts like you can see only one part of 
the sun. 
(scf 1 1 p r e i n t 182- 1 86 ) 
132 
After the visit she gave a simil a r expla n a t i o n: 
Helen :   Becaus e ?  Umm, the mo on revolv e s aroun d us, the Earth .  And 
the sun?s light ?  Umm how can  I say , the sun? s light ?OK  
You can only see the parts of the moon that ?  Umm, the sun, 
that recei v e s the sunli g h t. The sun? s light.  The shado w part you 
can? t see. 
(scf 1 1 p o s i n t 094) 
On this basis I classifi e d Helen at knowledg e level 3 both prior to and after her visit to the 
plane t a r i u m.  
5.6.8  Sipho and satellite dishes  
S t u d e n t s were questio n e d about sa tell i t e dishes only if they visited Hartebeesthoek Radio 
Astronomy Observat o r y , so I here use Sipho fro m Balfour Forest Scho ol to show how I 
classi f i e d a studen t into categ or i e s of knowled g e for this do minant artefact. Although he 
was fa mil i a r with dishe s , prior to his visi t to HartRAO Sipho had only a limited idea about 
their shape or where they point to, as the follo w i ng trans cr i pt shows : 
Interv i e w e r : Like a satell i t e dish.  Okay .  What is the shape of that dish? 
Sipho :   The shape ?  It is circl e an d it?s a little bit like a circle and oval. 
Intervi e w e r : Okay .  And it? s?  Like a dish at the same ti me? 
Sipho :   Yes. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Umm ...  Why is it that shape do you think ? 
Sipho :   I think it could be, it? s easy for it to pull ?  Some messa g e fro m 
? the planet s or someth i n g what ever it?s (//) it to do. 
Inter v i e w e r  Where is it point i n g to?  You know the satel l i t e dish on the 
hous e , wher e is it point in g to? 
Sipho :  The ? at the sky ? and the moon. 
(swo 0 5 p r e i n t 154- 17 0 ) 
On this basis, I classified him at knowle dge level 1.5, where he could not explain the 
reason for the satell i t e dish shape, and was not  clear about the direc t i o n in which the dish 
might point. Howeve r , he was able to expand consid e r a b l y on his explan a t i o n in the post-
 vi s i t intervi e w , and althoug h he was not scient i f i c a l l y  correc t in all deta ils , he showed an 
improve d knowle d g e level, which I classif i e d as 2.  
Intervi e w e r : Okay .  And a satelli t e  dish, why is it that shape ? 
Sipho :   In order for let? s say that we used in Harteb e e s when we talk 
about our voice it bounc e back and then it bounc e to the other 
dish then it comes out with the wires . 
Interv i e w e r : Okay .  And a satell i t e dish that is may b e used for somet h i n g like 
DSTV where is it point in g to? 
133 
Siph o :   To the moon . 
Intervi e w e r : Uh hmmm.  Why is it point i n g to the moon? 
Sipho :   Becaus e there are so me oth er satel l i t e dishe s that are al so pointin g 
to us so becaus e when it? s there on the moon the satel l i t e s that are 
on the moon they can see every th i n g aroun d the world .  Now 
when this one point s it just get the informa t i o n . 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  So it comes fro m th e moon? 
Sipho :   Yes. 
Intervi e w e r : Okay . 
Siph o : No, with the Ameri c a n s buil d it there on the moon. 
Inte r v i e w e r : Okay .  How do you know that?  How do you know it?s on the 
moo n ? 
Sipho :   Becaus e of when I watch te levi s i o n I can see that like even there 
at Hartebe es when we asked some quest i o n s they also even told 
us that every count ry can build it?s own satel l i t e dishe s or 
whate v e r it wants to do there on the moon it does. 
(swo 0 5 p o s i n t 085- 11 1 ) 
In Braund and Reiss?s terms, Sipho actively enga ged with his experience at HartRAO, and 
learned as a result of that experie n c e (Braund & Reiss, 2004). 
5.7  The Extent of Learning 
T h e categori s a t i o n of students using their know le d g e level of Big Ideas  was then used as 
an organi s i n g scheme to determi n e the exte nt of their learning , specific a l l y how they 
develop e d their knowle d g e , unders t a n d i n g and ideas (Braun d & Reiss, 2004). The 
remai n d e r of this chapt e r exami n e s the stude n t s as a group and de mons t r a t e s the extent to 
which they acquire d knowled g e duri n g the visit to the stu dy site. Miles and Huberma n 
(1994) recomme n d three ?flows? of  analysis activity , name ly data reductio n , data display 
and the drawing and verifica tion of conclusions. I have reduced the data from the 
interviews conducted with students by categor ising their knowledge of  Big Ideas as shown 
in Table 5.3. The result s from the respon s e s to the pre- and post-v i s i t inter v i e w s were 
coded and converted into tabular for m us ing the softwa r e progra m ATLAS. t i . The 
follow i n g sectio n displa y s the reduce d data in the for m of ta bles and bar charts, which are 
subse q u e n t l y disc u s s e d and concl us i o n s draw n from them. For each secti o n I provi d e a 
short comment to clarify the da ta presente d . In all of the respon s e s lis ted, the numbers of 
stude n t s some t i me s vary from the total numbe r intervie w e d and analysed (34: 26 who 
visited HartRAO and 8 who vis it e d the plane t a r i u m) . Where va rianc e occurs, I provide an 
explan a t i o n in the releva n t sectio n . 
134 
5.7.1  Preparation for, anticipation of and knowledge regarding the visit 
T h e studen t s were going to visit either the planetar i u m or HartRAO, and the visit was 
relat e d either to the learn i n g area of natur a l scienc e (NS) or human and social scienc e 
(HSS) depend i n g on how the school had impleme n t e d the new curri c ul u m. It was pleasi n g 
to see that, prior to their visit over 80% of  student s knew both where they were going and 
that it related to their school curric u l u m. After the visit, th is figure increa s e d to over 90%. 
Nearly 90% of studen t s ? views regardi n g the purpose of the visit related to 
educatio n or learning . Prior to the visit, wi tho u t being promp t e d , onl y one student referred 
to ?havin g fun? as being part of the visit? s purpose. After the visit, the number of students 
who considered the visit was related to learning dropped s light l y , while the number who 
relat e d it to ?havi n g fun? increa s e d mark e d l y . This is likel y to have been due to the fact that 
I asked whether student s thought ?having fun? was part of the visit?s purpose after they had 
return e d , but was not asked prior to  the visit (Table 5.4 and Table 5.5). 
Table 5.4 Responses to Question B1 and B2: Where are you going/did you go on the 
forthcoming visit? What subject and to pic area is/was the visit related to? 
 Pre-visit Post-visit 
Visit location and subject/topic Number 
of 
students 
(n=34) 
Percent
 age 
Number 
of 
students 
(n=34) 
Percent
 age 
Students who knew where they were 
going/had gone and what the subject 
and topic was 
2 7 79 31 91 
Students who knew where they were 
going/had gone and had an 
approximate idea* of the topic 
4 12 0 0 
Students who didn?t know where 
they were going/had gone, but knew 
what the subject and topic was 
3 9 3 9 
Total 3 4 100 34 100 
* Answ er s inclu d ed astr o lo g y, exper imen ts and Mars 
135 
Table 5.5 Purpose of visit: Responses to Qu estion B3: What do you think is/was the 
purpose of the visit? 
 Pre-visit Post-visit 
Purpose of visit Number of 
students 
(n=34) 
Percent
 age 
Number of 
students 
(n=34) 
Percent
 age 
Related to education or learning 3 0 88 31 91 
Related to interest in or 
visualisation of space 
4 12 3 9 
Related to having fun 1 3 16 47 
Not related to having fun 0 0 1 3 
Related to Mars 1 3 1 3 
Purpose unknown or not answered 1 3 1 3 
Sever a l stud e n ts gave more than one purpo s e, so the totals do not add to 34 or 100% 
 
O n l y a fifth of the studen t s report e d having done any prepar a t i o n in class, most of who 
refer r e d to cover i n g the topic s of space, stars and planet s earl ie r in the year (Figur e 5.1). 
However, other than covering the topic in  class, no student s described any specific 
activi t i e s relat i n g to the visit . There were however six student s who did some prepa r a ti on 
for the visi t outside class. Three of these were me mb e r s of a scien c e club at Bokamos o 
School who prepared together with a teacher and made a s undial. Two other students did 
some privat e readin g , and at l east one student (Banyana ) appear ed to have made the effort 
to prepa r e for the visit as a direc t resul t of my  initi a l visit to the schoo l , as can be seen from 
the followi ng exchang e : 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Have you been doing any prepa r a t i o n for the visit at 
schoo l , rece n t l y ? 
Bany an a :   Of course .  I did a little bit of resear c h . 
Inter v i e w e r : You have.  Okay .  Did you do that your sel f ?  Or was it done with 
every body in the class? 
Bany a n a :   I did it my self . 
Inte r v i e w e r : Okay .  What did you do? 
Bany a n a :   No, umm? We have books at home. 
Interv i e w e r : Uh-hmm . 
Bany a n a :   About stars, the univer s e and every t h in g .  Becaus e we have sort 
of like a librar y so, I just wanted to use it? 
Inte r v i e w e r : Okay .  Right .  When did you do that ? 
Bany a n a : Umm ?  I did that after you came to visit . 
(swo 2 0 p r e i n t 17-3 5 ) 
136 
This is an example of how the researc h pro cess, although not planned as an intervention, 
had direct bearing on the pre-knowledge of a st udent . Accordi n g to he r pre-vis i t intervi e w 
Banya n a had a strong inter e s t in space, whic h would also likely have influenc e d her 
decision to prepare on her own. 
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 7 0
 No preparation Preparation done in class
 recently
 Preparation done in class
 earlier in year
 Preparation done alone or
 out of class
 %
  
Figure 5.1 Student preparation for the visit (n=34) 
A l l stude n t s expre s s e d that they  were looki n g forwa r d to the vi sit , six of them empha t i c a l l y 
so. 16 studen t s (47%) indica t e d that learnin g or educati o n was their main reason for 
looking forward to the visit. By expressing this, they may have been linking the visit to the 
academi c goals of schooli n g . A similar number ( 41%) expre s s e d their antic i p a t i o n in terms 
of intere s t , exper i e n c e or excite me n t , but di d not directl y relate it to learnin g . Only 5 
stude n t s (15%) refer r e d to the fun or enjoyme nt of such a visit, and three of these had 
heard about this aspect from others (Figure 5. 2). However, it is likely that the students 
percei v e d me as being ?educa tional?, and answered me in  a way that they thought I 
expect e d . 
137 
0
 5
 1 0
 1 5
 2 0
 2 5
 3 0
 3 5
 4 0
 4 5
 5 0
 Learning: wants to know
 more, find out more*
 Interest, wants the
 experience or is excited*
 Fun, enjoyment of visit Unspecified
 %
  
Figure 5.2 Student anticipation of visit.  
Some students gave more than one purpose, so the totals do not add to 100% 
I aske d these init i a l quest i o n s to deter mi n e if stude n t s knew where th ey were going on their 
visit , what subje ct ( s ) in thei r curri c u l u m it was relat e d to, thei r understanding of the 
purpo s e of the visit , the exten t to which they ha d prepa r e d for the visit , and what they were 
expect i n g from the visit. The majori t y of st udents in my study knew where they were going 
and how it was relate d to their schoo l curri c u l um, indic a t i ng that their teach e r s had given 
them this basic infor ma t i o n . Nearl y 90% of studen t s were aware that the purpos e of the 
visit was related to education or learning, which is not really surpris i n g given that the visit 
is organis e d by their teacher s a nd is relat e d to their curri cul u m. It is also possi b l e that 
stude n t s relat e d to me, their inter v i e w e r , as  a teacher , and gave me that answer they 
though t I wanted to hear. Ve ry few students (23%, n=34) reported having done any 
prepara t i o n for the visit in class, and none pr ovided any details of what they had been 
doing. The museum resear c h litera t u r e on school vi sits suggests that very  little is done to 
prepar e studen t s for trips to museums and scienc e centre s . Griffi n and Symi ng t o n (1997) 
reported from Australia that out of 12 sc hool s going on excurs i o n s to museums , the 
studen t s invol v e d from 7 school s repor t e d no prepara t i o n had taken place (beyon d 
logis t i c a l arran g e me n t s ) , while the studen t s from a furthe r 4 school s report e d some task-
138 
specific preparation such as handing out of workshe e t s . Similar l y , Storksd i e c k (2004) in 
Germa n y found that in self- r e p or t inter vi e w s  over half the teache r s taking studen t s on a 
visit to a plane t ar i u m admi t t e d they did not prepa re studen t s for the visit . Yet there is 
conside r a b l e resear c h eviden c e which shows th at prepar a t i on for a vi sit can have a highly 
positi ve effect on the studen t s? learni n g expe riences. Kubota and Olstad (1991) showed 
that prepar a t i o n by the teache r in reduci n g the novelty of the visit was effecti v e in 
impro v i n g cogni t i ve learn i n g for the boys (but not the girls) in their 6 th  grade sampl e , while 
Anders o n and Lucas (1997) confir me d this for both genders in year 8. Lucas (1999) 
demon s t r a t e d that Mr. Jones ? exemp l a r y prepa r a t ion for his class of 10- ye a r - ol d s ? visi t to a 
scienc e centre was highly benefi c i a l to his studen t s ? visit. Althou g h a limite d sa mple , my 
study suppor t s the resear c h elsewh e r e that pre- vi s i t prepar a t i o n for school visits is not 
regarde d as importa n t by most teacher s .  
5.8  Knowledge about Big Ideas in astronomy prior to the visit 
T h e followi n g questio n s (from Sec tion C of the Interv i e w Schedu l e , Append i x B) were 
intende d to ascerta i n student s ? prior knowledg e about Big Idea s in astron o my as described 
earlie r in this chapte r , and to determi n e wh ethe r that knowle d g e change d as a result of the 
visit to eithe r HartR A O or the plane t a r i u m. The knowle d g e levels referr e d to in the 
discussion are those defined in Table 5.3. Where I have appropr i a t e data  I use percentages, 
while if number s are low I give the actual number s . 
5.8.1  Gravity 
S t u d i e s of students ? understa n d i n g of gravit y are promin e n t in the resear ch liter at u re . Much 
of this resea r c h has been within the subjec t of physic s ,  and related to student s ? 
underst a n d i n g of gravita t i o n a l acceleration, often with respect  to applied problems (e.g. 
Bar, Zinn, Goldmu n t z & Sneide r , 1994; Guns tone & White, 1981; Palmer, 2001). Another 
strand of research ha s been with respect to how gravity  is understood in  relation to the 
Earth (Nussb a u m & Novak, 1976; Schoul t z et al ., 2001; Vosniadou & Brewer, 1992). A 
few studie s have examin e d gravit y with respec t to space and spaces h i ps (e.g. Bar et al ., 
1997; Treagust & Smith, 1989). There is a degree of overlap in many of  these studies, and 
they identif y a number of misc onc e p t i o n s that student s and ot hers have about gravity. One 
promi n e n t misco n c e p t i o n is that the cause of gr avit y is not relate d to the mass of an object 
but is due to someth i n g else, such as air or the Earth?s rotati o n (Borun et al . , 1993 ; Palme r , 
2001). A relate d miscon c e p t i o n that there is no gr avit y in space has been report e d as being 
139 
very preval e n t among both studen t s and adults . This miscon c e p t i o n seems to be associ a t e d 
with the idea that gravi t y requi re s a mediu m in which to operat e (usual l y air), and that 
since ther e is no air in space , there will also  be no gravi t y . Sever a l resea r c h er s (e.g. Bar et 
al . , 1997; Borun et al . , 1993; Moolla, 2003; Watts, 1982) have conduct e d studies which 
demonstrate the predominance of the idea. Although the misconce p t i o n may well be linked 
to the absenc e of air, it is likel y to be reinfor ce d by image s of weigh t l e s s astro naut s in 
space and space s h i p s .  
In my study, how the studen t s unders t o o d gr avit y was invest i g a t e d in two main 
ways. First, studen t s might refer to the word ?gravi t y ? in their PMMs, which would 
norma l l y resul t in some discus s i o n of it duri ng the intervi e w, by pr obing their knowle d g e . 
Secondly, students visiting HartRAO were spec ifi c a l l y questi o n e d on  their knowledge of 
gravity during the structur e d part of the inter v i ew . The in iti al quest i o n they were asked was 
?What is gravity ? ? or ?Do you know what grav i t y is?? This was somet i me s rephr a s e d , if 
studen t s initia l l y did not answ er , to ?What does [gravi t y ] do? ? Further questions asked 
were ?Do you know what causes gravity? ? ?What would gravity be like on the moon? ? 
?What about on some wh e r e like Jupite r ? ? ?Does the Earth?s gravity have any effect on the 
moon?? and ?Do you think the moon?s gravity ha s any effect on the Ea rth? ? For each of 
these questi o n s , depend i n g on the studen t ? s re sponse , further probing of their answer 
ensued . Studen t s visi ti n g the planet a r i u m we r e not specifically questioned on gravity 
during the structu r e d part of the intervi e w becau s e when the inter v i ew quest i o n s were 
planne d , I did not antici p a t e th at the planetar i um show was going to cover gravity in the 
detail that it actua ll y did. In retrospe c t , these students should  have been asked the same 
questions as those visiting HartRAO. Howeve r, two students visiti ng the planetarium who 
refer r e d to gravi t y in thei r PMM or the inte rvi e w based on their PMM are include d in the 
result s which follow . 
Student s showed a substan t i a l ch ange in their overal l knowledg e of gravity after their visit. 
Where a s initi a l l y over half the stude nt s were cl assi f i e d at knowled g e level 1, whereby they 
knew that it is some sort of pulling (or in some cases pushing) force but knew very little 
else, after the visit this num ber had dropped to 3. The number  of students at knowledge 
level 2 showed a slight increase (by 2 student s) after the visit. However, the number of 
studen t s at level 2.5 and 3 increa s e d from 2 to 9 studen t s , indica t i n g a considerable shift in 
knowledg e across the group. Only one student did not know what gravity was either before 
140 
or after the visit. Fi gure 5.3 shows students? overall conc eption of gravity both prior to and 
afte r thei r visi t . 
0
 2
 4
 6
 8
 1 0
 1 2
 1 4
 1 6
 Level 0 Level 1 Level 1.5 Level 2: Level 2.5: Level 3
 Number
  o
 f s
 tude
 nt
 s Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.3 Students? conceptions of gravity (Pre-visit: n=28; post-visit: n=27) 
P r i o r to the visit, 23 out of 28 students who were asked specific a l l y gave some sort of 
defin i t i o n of gravi t y durin g their inter v i e w . Of these, 20 gave an accepta b l e , partial 
scient i fi c answe r (rela t i v e to school grade 7-8) to the quest i o n ?What is gravi t y? ? by 
stati n g that it is some type of pulli n g forc e . The major i t y of these stude n t s refer r e d to a pull 
towar d s the Earth . These ideas held by stude n t s are consi s t en t with much of the liter a t u re 
(from Bar et al ., 1997; to Nussbaum & Novak, 1976) wh ereb y studen t s identi f y object s as 
falli n g down toward s the Earth . The 3 student s who didn? t ident i fy gravi t y as a pullin g 
force downwards referred to it ?pulling you up?  to ?holding us up?. These notions are not 
consis t e n t with most descri pt i o ns in the l itera t u r e but may refer to gravity stopping us 
?falli n g off the Earth? , a common idea in the re searc h litera t ur e (e.g. Vosnia d o u & Brewer, 
1992). After the visit the definit i o n s of gr avit y elici t e d from th e students were not 
substan t i a l l y differ e n t from those expres s e d befo r e the visit . I consi d er it unsurprising that 
there was little change in students? definiti o n s  of gravity, as neith er of the study sites 
discu s s e d the meaning  of the word gravity , or defined it as a concept . 
141 
However, a fuller understa n d i n g of  the concep t of grav it y is likely to in volve not just the 
idea that it involv e s a pullin g fo rce , but that a stude n t can expan d their defin i t i o n to invol v e 
what cause s gravi t a t i o na l force , or how it occu r s withi n the solar system. Stude n t s in the 
study were asked about the cause of gravity duri ng their pre-vis i t interv iew only if they had 
already expres s e d some ideas about gravit y . De spi t e the preva l e n c e in the liter a t ur e of the 
idea that gravi t y is re late d to the presen c e of air (B erg & Brewer, 1991; Palmer , 2001)) , 
only 2 studen t s during their inte rvie w specific a l l y spoke about gravity being related to an 
atmos p h e r e . Howev e r , other stude nt s state d in their PMMs that in space there is no air and 
no gravity , indica t i n g a possibl e relat i o n s h i p betwe e n the tw o. One student stated that 
gravity is somehow related to rocky plane t s , and this idea was alluded to by a number of 
other studen t s who sugges t e d that  a gas planet is unlikel y to have gravity. Four students 
stated that they did not know what caused gr avity and only three students were able to 
expla i n that gravi t y is relat e d to mas s or si ze. After the visit, only one student thought that 
gravity might be caused by the atmosph e r e , while 10 student s now were able to explain 
that gravit y is relate d to the mass (or size) of an object .  
Most students who visited HartRAO were asked about their know ledge of gravity 
on Jupite r and the Moon as shown in Figure 5.4 and Figure 5.5. The number of studen t s in 
these chart s is lower than the total who vis ited HartRAO as some st uden t s did not discus s 
these issues if they lacked knowledg e about gravity generall y . Figure 5.4 shows that prior 
to their visit, the majorit y of student s (12) believed there would be  no gravity on Jupiter. 
Most students did not give reasons  for their belief, but those that  did ascr i b e d it to the lack 
of an at mo sp h e r e , it ?being in space? , or being gaseou s . After the visit, there was a 
substa n t i a l increa s e in the nu mber of studen t s (from 4 to 12) who underst o o d that the 
gravity on Jupiter is high. The number who sa id there is no or lo w gravity on Jupiter 
decrease d from 12 to 8, and the number of ?don?t knows? decrea s e d to 3. These figure s 
suggest that the students ? vis it changed their know ledge about Jupiter?s gravity towards a 
more accept e d scient i f i c view.  
142 
0
 2
 4
 6
 8
 1 0
 1 2
 1 4
 Stated that there is gravity and
 that it is high
 Stated there is no or low gravity Don?t know
 Number
  o
 f s
 tude
 nt
 s Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.4 Students? ideas of gravity on Jupiter (n=24) 
B e f o r e the visit, 9 student s knew that gravity on the Moon is less than that of Earth, while 
12 believe d that there is no gr avity on the moon, and several of  these students referred to 
being able to ?float ? or ?fly ? above the Moon?s surface . Ther e appears to be confusio n in 
some student s ? minds betwee n being ?in space? (where many student s believe there is no 
gravity) and being ?on the Moon? . As the Moon is ?in space?, there is a conflation between 
the two, and student s may believe  that being distant from th e Earth, in an envi ron me n t 
where there is no air (?space ? ) result s in th ere being no gravity . This misconc e p t i o n is 
consi s t e n t with the liter a t u r e (e.g. Palmer 2001) and was not  necess a r i l y correc t e d during 
the visit . After the visit , the numbe r of st udents who knew that there is gravity on the 
Moon increa s e d to 19. Simila r l y , the number of studen t s who believ e d there is no gravity 
on the Moon dropped to 5, and the number of students who were uncertai n dropped to one 
(Figure 5.5). These figures indica te that the visit assisted students to change their mind 
about the absence or presenc e of gravity on the Moon, and to adopt a more scienti f i c 
concept i o n . The majorit y of student s who we re question e d about the Moon and gravity 
visited HartRAO. The features of the visit may have account e d for their chang e in view 
were likely to have been the Coke can exhibit ,  the ?gravi t y scales? and a slide show about 
143 
the Moon landings. Six student s changed their ideas regard ing gravity on both Jupiter and 
the Moon to a scienti f i c a l l y accept e d concep t i o n .  
0
 2
 4
 6
 8
 1 0
 1 2
 1 4
 1 6
 1 8
 2 0
 Stated that there is gravity and
 that it is low
 Stated there is no gravity Don?t know
 Number
  o
 f s
 tude
 n
 ts
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.5 Students? ideas of gravity on the Moon (n=25) 
5.8.2  Stars and the Sun 
T h e word ?stars? was provided to students on their blank Person a l M eaning Map, while 25 
stude n t s (74%) refer r e d to the Sun of their own volition in th eir PMM. Prior to the visit , 
nearly half of the studen t s (44%) had a very mi nima l knowl e d g e of what a star is (leve l 1): 
they typica l l y referr e d to stars as lights in th e night sky, and they had no idea of stars? size, 
compo s i t i o n or positi o n in space. These stude nt s gave no indi ca t i o n that stars are actual l y 
suns or that our Sun is a st ar. Slight l y more student s did understand that stars are suns 
(level 2), and they typically gave the defini t i on of a star as a ? burning ball of gas?. 
Howeve r , this catego r y of studen t s posses s e d vario u s misco n c e p t i o n s about stars in terms 
of their size, compo s i t i on or posit i on in space , and examp l e s of these misco n c e p t i o n s can 
be found in the student portra its in Chapters 7 and 8. Two studen t s (6%) from the same 
school could give a fully scien tific explanation of stars (lev el 3) with no misconce p t i o n s 
and furthe r elabor a t e d on aspect s of star co mpos i t i o n . After the visit there was a 20% shift 
from a minima l knowl e d g e of a star (leve l  1) towards level 2, while only 1 student 
144 
increa s e d their knowle d g e of stars to catego r y 3. While I cannot deter mi n e wheth e r this 
shift is signi fi c a n t , there is a trend towar d s an improved scientific understanding of stars, 
shown in Figure 5.6. 
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 7 0
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.6 Students? conceptions of stars (n=34) 
A n a l y s i s of students ? concepti o n s of the S un are based on their answers to questio n s 
HartRA O C1 (?how is the sun simi l a r or differ e n t to stars? ? ) and C2 (stude n t s were asked 
about the day?s temper a t u r e , boilin g water temp e r a t u r e and the Sun?s tempera t u r e , before 
being further asked ?What else can you tell me about the Sun? ?). Students visiting the 
plane t a r i u m were asked C3 (?Tel l me anything you know about the Sun?).  
Prior to the visit, one quarte r of the studen t s had a relative l y na?ve concept of the Sun 
(level 1) as bigger and brighte r  than stars , as oc curring during the day and with proper t i e s 
such as being fiery and hot. None of these stude n t s referr e d to the Sun being a star. Half of 
the studen t s had a more sophis t i c a t e d scien t i f i c id ea of the Sun (level 2) : as a star, often the 
bigges t star, but no explan a t i o n of size as be ing appar e n t and relate d to distance. These 
stude n t s refer r e d to simil a r prope r t i e s as th e first group, but tended to also include 
additi o n a l scient i f i c facts such  as gas, and energy. The rema ining quarter of students had 
substan t i a l knowle d g e of the Sun (level 3) in relation to scal e, as the closest but not the 
145 
biggest star. Some of this gr oup also referre d to facts ab out the Sun indicating a more 
advanced understa n d i n g , such as ultra-vi o l e t light, nuclear react i o ns and the Sun?s age and 
future. 
After the visit consi de r abl y fewer stude n t s (9%) held the mo st na?ve conce p t i o n of the Sun. 
Compa r e d with the pre- v i s i t resul t s, fewer stude n t s (41% instea d of 50%) held level 2, 
while half the student s now had the most subs tantial knowledge of the Sun (level 3). These 
findings (Figure 5.7) indicate that students show e d a more devel o p e d understanding of the 
Sun during the post-visit interviews than  they did prior to their visit.  
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.7 Students? conceptions of the Sun (n=34) 
F e w studie s have been report e d in the literat u r e about student s ?  conceptions of stars or the 
Sun (Bailey & Slater, 2003). Sh arp (1996) explored the ideas of 42 10- and 11-year-olds in 
Englan d and showed that most had a basic knowle d g e of the Sun?s shape, compos i t i o n and 
posit i o n . Howev e r , only 57% were sure that it  was a star. The same stude n t s ? knowl e d g e of 
stars was much less clear , and consi s t e d mainl y  of obser v a t i o n a l and st yli s e d descr i pt i o n s. 
Comi ns (2001) indicated that undergraduate students do not understand that the Sun and 
stars are essen t i a l l y the same, as well as holdin g severa l other miscon c e p t i o n s . 
146 
Agan (2004) worked with small number s of high school and univers i t y studen t s in 
a study of their understanding of  stars, and one of her inte rview questions was almost 
ident i c a l to my C1. My findi n g s for compa r a b l e  age ranges (12 to 14 years in my study and 
14 to 15 years in Agan?s resea r c h ) are simila r . Pr ior to their visit , over three quart e r s of the 
students in my study had knowledg e of the Sun as a star. Agan ?s 8 students did recognise 
the Sun as a star, but believ e d that it is the bigge st star, as did 6 of th e stude n t s in my study . 
My student s ? knowled g e of the Sun appeare d to  be mor e sophisticat e d after th e visit, 
showin g a greate r abilit y to name sun co mposit i o n , properti e s and evolutio n . More 
import a n t l y , severa l studen t s seemed to have gained a greate r unders t a n d i n g of scale, as 
they now relat e d the Sun as being close r than the other stars as the reaso n that it appea r s 
bigger. Agan?s senior high school and university  undergraduates were ab le to explain this, 
but not the studen t s close to the age range of those in my study.  
5.8.3  The Solar system 
A l t h o u g h both study sites provide d student s with informa t i o n about the solar system, only 
the 8 student s visiti n g the planeta r i u m were sp ecifi c a l l y questio n e d about it, but the results 
from this questi o n are inconc l u s i v e probab l y due to the small numbe r s invol v e d . All I can 
deduce from Figure 5.8 is that after the visit there were no studen t s who had no scien t i fi c 
concep t i o n of what the solar syst em is, wherea s  prior to the visit there was one studen t in 
this category.  
147 
0
 1
 2
 3
 4
 5
 6
 7
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 Number
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.8 Students? conceptions of the Solar System (n=8) 
A cleare r assess me n t of what student s learnt fr om the visit is gaine d from an exami n a t i o n 
of their PMMs which demon s t r a t e addit i o na l fact s about the planet s , partic u l a r l y ones such 
as Mercur y (clos es t to the Sun and hotte st ) ,  Pluto (furthes t from the Sun and coldest) , 
Jupit er (the bigge s t ) , Mars (vari o u s facts as it had been in the news recen t l y ) and the Earth 
(as the most fa mili a r planet ) . Both study sites provid e d this sort of informa t i o n about the 
Solar System, so an increase in factual knowledge about indi vidual planets is the most 
likely outcome . Only 4 students (a nd one student from pilot interviews) referred to there 
being other solar syste ms aroun d other stars after the visit. 
5.8.4  Size and scale 
S i z e and scale as a Big Idea is a compos i t e esti mation of a student?s kno wledge of the size 
of the Sun, stars, the Moon a nd other heavenl y bodies as we ll as his or her understa n d i n g 
of distan c e withi n the solar syste m and be yond. Studen t s ? knowle d g e and appreci a t i o n of 
size and scale in the solar syst e m (and for some students the universe as well) improved 
consid e r a b l y over the period betw een the pre-visit and post-vis it interviews. Students at the 
lowest level, with confuse d and conflic t i n g ideas about size and scale decrea s e d from 15% 
to 6%; only 2 studen t s rema in e d at knowle d g e  level 1, and they both showed the least 
148 
chang e in all the Big Idea categ o r i e s acros s the study (see Chapter 7). Student s with an 
apparen t scienti f i c knowled g e (levels 2 and 2.5)  decrea s e d while level 3 studen t s increa s e d 
from 20% to 48% (Figur e 5.9). Since both stu dy sites dealt with variou s aspec t s of both 
size and scale, it is likely th at the visit itself was mainly responsi b l e for the change in 
knowle d g e , and was succes s f u l in doing so. Given that Trumpe r ? s work (Trumpe r , 2001a , 
2001b) suggests that this is on e of the weakest areas of high  school knowledg e in Israel, 
my study sugges t s that astron o my - r e l a t e d scienc e cen tres are effective sites to get this type 
of knowledge and understanding across to students.  
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 7 0
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 2.5 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.9 Students? conceptions of size and scale 
I n relat i o n to size and scale, a specif i c quest i o n relat i n g to the relat i ve size of the Sun and 
Moon and the explana t i o n for this was asked.  
Knowled g e levels for this questi o n were define d as follow s : 
Level 1: D o e s not know relati v e 
sizes of sun & moon, thinks 
moon is larger than sun, or 
think s they are the same size. 
Distan c e may not have been 
asked.  
Level 2: R e l a t i v e size of sun 
and moon is correc t , but studen t 
does not underst a n d relati v e 
distan c e of sun and moon 
correct l y , or cannot explain the 
diffe r e n c e in size . 
Level 3: S t u d e n t has 
correct concept i o n s of the 
relat i v e size and dista n c e 
of the sun and moon  
149 
Like a number of other question s (e.g. question s  on stars and gravity)  I asked this question 
to determi n e whethe r studen t s could not only relate scale a nd size in the sky, but give 
reasons for why objects look si milar or different in size . Many students do know the 
diffe r e n c e in size of the moon and the sun, but can they expla i n why they look  the same 
size in the sky? If  they can, then they understa n d more about scale in the solar system. The 
questi o n only determi n e d the relative size  of the Sun and the Moon, not the extent of the 
Sun?s magni tu d e in relatio n to the Moon.  
Prior to the visit , just over half the st udent s (19 student s , 56%) had a scienti f i c a l l y 
correc t concep t of the relativ e  size of the Sun in relatio n to  the Moon (level 3), and could 
also explai n that their appare n t si mi l a r i t y in size is due to the fact that the Moon is closer to 
the Earth than the Sun. Before going on the vi sit, 12 students (35%) did know that the Sun 
is larger than the Moon, but could not explai n why they looked the sa me size in the sky in 
terms of thei r relat i v e dist a nc e s from the Earth (level 2). Taking these two categori e s 
togeth e r , the vast majori t y of studen t s (91% ) did know that the Sun is larger than the 
Moon, and only 3 studen t s though t that the Moon is larger than the Sun or that the 2 
object s are the same size. After the visit the pe rcen t a g e of studen t s in  category 3 (scientific 
concep t i o n ) increa s e d to 69%, and all students could describe the Sun as being larger than 
the Moon. The percent a g e of student s in categor y  2 decreas e d slight l y after the visit, and 
no student s rema in e d at knowled g e level 1 (F igure 5.10). These result s are in line with 
Summers and Mant?s study of British primar y school teache r s , most of whom kne w the 
relati v e sizes of the Moon and th e Earth (Summe r s & Mant, 1995). 
150 
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 7 0
 8 0
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.10 Students? knowledge of the size of the Sun and Moon (n=34 pre-visit, n=29 post-
 visit) 
5.8.5  Day and Night 
U n d e r s t a n d i n g the cause of day and night is an importan t st ep in understanding how the 
Earth-Su n - M o o n system works. Instead of asking why day and night occur I asked ?Why 
does the Sun move across the sky each day? ? to  determi n e whethe r studen t s could explai n 
the apparent motion of the Sun across the sky as being due to the Ea rth spinning. However, 
in discus si on with collea g u e s while analy s i n g my data, I have consi d e r e d that the 
question ? s phrasing might have prompted a par tic ul a r type of answer , which I initia l l y 
classi f i e d as teleol o g i c a l 1 2 , and the implica t i o n s of this are discussed below. After the 
initia l quest i o n was asked, I handed each studen t a model Earth globe and a torch to 
repres e n t the Sun, and asked them to use these to elabor a t e on their ex planation (?show me 
what?s happenin g ? ) with relevant promptin g .  
Prior to the visit, 25 of the student s ( 74%) could explai n the cause of the Sun?s 
motion (level s 2 and 3), but of these, 10 (30%) neede d the ?prompt ? of the model in order 
to expla i n the conce p t in an accept a bl e scien t i f i c manne r . In knowle d g e categ o r i e s 2 and 3 
                                                 
 
12  Teleo log y is a bran ch of philo soph y which ascr ib es desig n or purpo se to natur al phen omen a. 
151 
four studen t s also used a ?teleo l o g i c a l ? answ e r as their first expl an a t i o n of why the Sun 
moves. Nine studen t s (26%) showed confus i o n in the explan a t i o n of day and night (level 
1) even when they had the assi st a nc e of the model. This catego r y include d student s who 
confu s e d day and night on the mode l (or maint a i n e d miscon c e p t i o n s su ch as revolution of 
the Earth around the sun as the cause of day and night) and ?teleo l o gical? explanations 
(Figur e 5.11). 
After the visit, ther e was little chang e ove ral l in studen t s ? concep t i o n s of why the 
sun moves acros s the sky. While there was a sligh t incre a s e of students who could explain 
the Sun?s motion (now 79%), a similar number (32%) still relied on the model to assist 
their explana t i o n . There was a slight decrea s e in the number of student s who showed 
confus i o n in their explan a t i o n of day and ni ght , but it is unlik e l y that this number is 
signif i c a n t . As the sa mp le s were not randoml y select e d , it would not be valid to carry out 
signi f i c a nc e tests . The impor t a n c e of  my findin g s in this knowle d g e  area is that the use of a 
model as a prompt in the interv i e w proces s ch anges student response s , as discuss e d later in 
this sectio n . 
0
 5
 1 0
 1 5
 2 0
 2 5
 3 0
 3 5
 4 0
 4 5
 5 0
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.11 Students? conceptions of the cause of day and night (n=34) 
152 
Severa l studie s (e.g. Baxter , 1989; Dove, 2002; Kikas, 1998a; Sadler , 1998; Sharp, 
1996) have asked studen t s to explain the cause of  day and night, usually as part of a larger 
study . The most influe n t i a l piece of resear c h was conduct e d by Vosniad o u and Brewer 
(1994) in which they postula t e d  a series of me nta l model s held by children ranging from 
age 6 to 11 years. All the 16 peer-re v i e w e d st udies used test questi ons, questionnaires or 
inter v i e w s to elici t infor ma t i o n from thei r subjects, often encour agi n g them to draw 
diagrams . Only three used models as part of  the inter vi e w proce s s . The inten t i o n of my 
asking the questi o n in the way I did was to try and determi n e whether the student s could 
relate direct observation of the Sun ?moving? acros s the sky to the conce p t of the Earth 
spinnin g . During each intervi e w , after asking the in itial questions, I showed the student a 
model of the Earth (a small globe) and hand ed her or him a small torch, which I said 
represe n t e d the Sun. I then invited the student  to expand on what he or she had already 
said, but now using the model. I also probed as necess a r y , to determi n e the studen t ? s own 
underst a n d i n g of what she or he was explai n i n g . In asking the questio n , I deliber a t e l y 
avoide d referr i n g to the apparent moveme n t of the Sun across the sky, as I consid e r e d that 
this would cue the students towards a scientific  rather than their ow n explanation. In my 
initia l coding , I identi fie d a type of explanation which I cl assi f i e d as ?teleo l o gi c a l ? , 
whereby the student explained the reason why the Sun move d across the sky as being ?in 
order to do somet h i n g ? , such as to give light or heat to the people on the other side of the 
Earth. Original l y propound e d by Plato and Aristo tl e , current adherent s of teleolog y are 
mainl y religi o u s belie v er s who argue that the purpo s e in natur e can be attri b u t e d to the 
presenc e of a Supreme Being or God. 
Althou g h I consid e r that severa l stateme n t s made by studen t s can be consid e r e d 
teleol o g i c a l , it is possibl e that the questio n ?Why does th e Sun?.? was interpre t e d by 
some studen t s to requir e an answer in which the Sun?s mo veme n t does have a purpose . 
However, when presente d with a globe a nd asked for a furthe r explan a t i o n , studen t s 
underst o o d this cue to mean that they now had to give an explana t o r y answer, which they 
accordi n g l y did. As shown in Figure 5.11, prior to  the visit nearl y half the stude nt s were 
able to expla i n the appar e n t cause of the sun?s move ment as being due to the Earth?s 
rotation without the use of th e model as a suppor t for this explan a t i o n . I regard it as 
impor t a n t that such stude n t s were able to expl ain in scienti f i c terms withou t the use of the 
model (and yet could also  use it corr e c t l y to assi s t thei r expla n a t i o n) . The most intere s t i n g 
group of students were the ones (10 before the visit and 11 after the vi sit) who started their 
153 
expla n a t i o n in a somewh a t confu s e d ma nne r , and it was only when the model was 
avail a b l e that they coul d provi d e the scient i f i c a l l y acceptab l e explanat i o n of day and night.  
For exampl e Lara, in her pre-vi s i t interv i e w ga ve an explanation for the sun?s move me nt as 
the Earth?s revoluti o n . As she is  not presented as a portrait in Chapter 7 or 8 I show the 
transcr i p t of her inter vi ew s here. 
Lara:   It?s the Eart h that ? s revo lv in g arou nd the sun. 
Interv i e w e r : Okay .   
Lara:   Which cause s it to rise and [inau d i b l e ] 
I then introd u c e d the model, and asked her to explai n the ph eno me n o n using it:  
Interv i e w e r : Okay .  So, if you have the s un here and that? s the sun [the torch ], 
you hold that .  What? s going on for the ever y day risi ng and 
setti n g ? 
Lara:   The Earth is movin g like this.  No, yes, I?m right .  And then the 
Earth rotat e s and then every year the Earth revol v e s 
(scf1 0 p r e i n t 61-69 ) 
At this point the student, by manipulating the model is realis i n g that while the Earth does 
revolv e aroun d the Sun, it also ro tate s . Furthe r probin g result s in  the student realisin g that 
the Earth?s rotation is the cause of the Sun?s apparent move ment (day and night), and not 
its orbit . 
Less than a quarte r of the signif i c a n t studi e s on stude n t s ? and teache r s? 
understanding of day and night repo rt e d in the litera t u r e used m odels as part of their data 
collection process. A recent paper by Schoultz et al. (2001) criticised Vosniadou and her 
associ a t e s for failin g to use a globe when que stion i n g children about the Earth and gravity 
(Vosnia d o u and Brewer 1992). Using a situat ed and discursive framework, Schoultz et al. 
claim that the introd u c t i o n of a globe to an intervi e w results in su bstantially different 
respon s e s from childr e n (ages 7 ? 11) than in the Vosnia d o u study in which the childr e n 
had to think abstra c t l y . Schoul t z et al. show that the childre n ? s concep t i o n of the Earth as a 
globe and gravity as an ?e xplanatory resource? (p. 114) are more prevalen t in young 
childre n than Vosniad o u had found. Schoult z ? s findin g s have been critic i s e d by Candel a 
(2001) for exclud i n g a cognit i ve expl an a t i o n , an d very recentl y Vosniado u has rebutte d the 
claims made by Schoult z and colleag u e s . Vosniad o u et al . (2005) conducted a study of 
grade s 1 to 3 using a globe to repre s e n t the Ea rt h . Whil e they admi t that the prese n c e of the 
globe can assist with childre n ? s thinki n g , they consi d e r that it can bias the resul t s of the 
study, by provid i n g a ready- ma d e concep t i o n of the Earth.  
154 
In my study of childre n several years ol der than those studie d by Vosnia d o u or 
Schoul t z , I found that the presen ce of the Earth-Sun mode l wa s a valuable tool in the 
inter v i e w proce s s . By the time childr e n reac h the age of 12 years, a globe would be the 
accep t e d ?cult u r a l artef a c t ? to repr e s e n t the Ea rth. Therefo r e , the finding s for my study are 
for children significantly olde r than those in the Vosniado u  and Schoultz studies and can 
neithe r confir m nor dispro v e the claims of e ithe r side. What my re sults show however , is 
that the use of a model as a ?p rop ? to disc u s s the relat i v e moti on of the Earth and the Sun is 
very valuable , as it directs children ? s thinking to respond to the questi on asked, rather than 
abstra c t l y discus s i n g the notion of the Earth? s orbit round the Sun, whi ch seems to be the 
first thing responde n t s refer to wh en asked about day and night. 
Parker and Heywood (1998) and Cameron (2003)  have referr e d to the confusion of 
orbit and spin and the terms revolve and rotate by the parti c i p a n t s in their studi e s . Parker 
and Heywoo d were workin g with pre- se r vi c e pr ima r y teach e r s , many of whom admi t t e d to 
being concep t u a l l y confus e d betwee n orbit and spin, and used a variety of words such as 
move round, goes round  and turns . 20 of Ca meron?s 54 univers i t y stud e nts used the terms 
rotation and revolution incorr ec t l y , these terms appea ri n g to be more widel y used in the 
South African context than orbit and spin. Si mila r confus i o n was prevale n t in my study. In 
answer to ?Why does the Sun mo ve across the sky every day? ? Sarah responded as 
follo w s : 
Sarah :  Becaus e our Earth revol v e s . 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Can you show me w ith this ?  [hand s stud e n t mode l ] This 
is the ?  There is the sun.  Can you hold the sun up?  What? s 
goi n g on?   
Sarah:   The Earth is turni n g . 
Here Sarah uses the term ?revo l v e ? but actua l l y means rotat e . Havin g initi al l y used the 
incorr e c t term, she then co rrec t l y uses the model to demons t r a t e night and day: 
Interviewer: Okay.  So if Africa is faci n g the sun like that [stud e n t is point in g 
torc h at Afri c a on the globe ].  What? s on ??   What? s goin g on 
now in Afric a ?  At this poin t . 
Sara h :   It?s day lig h t . 
Inter v i e w e r : It?s day lig h t .  Right .  And the other side of the Earth ? 
Sara h :   It?s nigh t . 
(scf 1 2 p r e i n t 117- 1 33 ) 
The import a n c e of this confus i o n , whethe r it is conceptual confusion between orbit and 
spin or confusion of terms (rotation and revolu t i o n look and sound simila r , which may 
155 
present difficulties for second-language English speake r s ) is that it has implic a t i o n s for 
researc h e r s who use questio n n a i res (or interviews without a model prese n t ) to deter mi n e 
subjec t s ? unders t a n d i n g of the day/ni g h t conce p t . There is  consi d e r a bl e furth e r poten t i al 
room for confusi o n . Eight studen t s (24% of  the sample) used the phrase ?rotat i n g around 
the sun?, which could mean that the Earth rota tes (on its axis), while  it is orbitin g the Sun. 
It doesn?t necessarily  mean revolving around the Sun. One student al so used the phrase 
?surro u n d i n g the sun?, meanin g the Earth goes round (orbit s ) the Sun. These exampl e s 
demonstr a t e that educator s and research e r s need  to be very carefu l in both their use and 
interpretation of what students me an when  they expre ss thems e l v e s . An examp l e is 
Summe r s and Mant (1995 ) , who asked a serie s of true/fa l s e questi o n s which includ e d The 
Sun goes round the Earth once in 24 hours  and The Earth moves around the Sun once in 
24 hours . While only 7% of the teach e r s agree d with the first stat e me n t , 32% agree d with 
the second. Althoug h this appears to indicat e th at the teache r s beli eve the Earth revolves 
around the Sun once a day, causing day and night  their answer may merely represent 
confus i o n of the terms and not a miscon ce p t i o n of how day and night occur.  
5.8.6  The Moon Phases 
R e s e a r c h into people ? s knowle d g e  of the Moon? s phase s is exten s ive in the literature which 
shows that while subject s can describ e the pha ses they do not necessar ily know the order in 
which they appear or why they  occur. Although I asked all students about Moon phases in 
the pre- v i si t inter v i e w , I only quest i o n e d 19 stude n t s about it after the visit , so the resul t s 
are limit e d . Prio r to their visi t , only five st ude nt s (15%) were able to give a satisfactory 
scienti f i c explana t i o n for the Moon phases (level 3), in wh ich the sun?s rays shining on the 
Moon as it revolve s around the Earth was explained. A furthe r four (12%) could give a 
parti a l scient i fi c expl a n at i o n (level 2.5), but  these stude nt s also inclu de d a misco n c e p t i o n 
in their explan a t i o n , such as the idea that the views of the phases are due to the Earth?s 
rotation. The 7 students (20 %)  at knowledg e level 2 had eith er a limited explanation (2 
studen t s) or referr e d to miscon c e p t i o n s about the phases : either the Earth?s shadow falling 
on the Moon (2 studen t s ) or someth i n g comi ng betwee n the Moon and the Earth result i ng 
in the Moon being ?eclip s e d ? (3 studen t s ) . Over half the students (53%) did not know the 
cause of the Moon phases (level 1) and we re prepare d to state this (Figure 5.12). 
Of the 19 studen t s asked about the Moon pha ses in the post visit interviews, there 
was little differen c e in students ? ability to explain the phases of the Moon. Neither of the 
156 
study sites discussed the phenomenon in any deta il, so the lack of change in knowledge is 
to be expected. 
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 2.5 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.12 Students? explanation of Moon phases (Pre-visit n=34; post-visit n=19) 
5.8.7  Parabolic/Satellite Dishes 
D u e to the physica l domi na n c e of satelli t e and paraboli c dishes and the activity on 
?whisp e r dishes ? at HartRA O , I though t it appr opri a t e to ask students about them. Pr ior to 
their visit, nearly  half the student s had a limited knowle d g e  of satellit e dish es (levels 1 and 
1.5): they did not know where a dish might point  towar d s , and had little or no idea of the 
reason for its shape. 38% could either explai n the reaso n for the shape of the dish or where 
it might point to but not both (level 2), wherea s only 4 studen t s (15%) had a more 
sophist i c a t e d knowled g e (level 2.5 or 3) of dishes (Figure 5.13). After the visit students 
had develope d in several knowle dge areas, with fewer studen t s at the lower levels and 
nearly half who could explain where the dish point s towar d s and the reason for its shape 
(thoug h 19% needed probin g to detect this knowle d g e ) . Most of studen t s ? prior knowle d g e 
of satel l i t e dish e s come s from thei r expe r i e n ce of seeing such dishes on buildings used for 
satelli t e televis i o n . Student s who appear e d to extend their knowle d g e as a result of the visit 
157 
are likely to have done so due  to the activi t y using the whis pe r dish as well as observ i n g 
the radi o telesc o p e pointi n g to stars in space. 
0
 5
 1 0
 1 5
 2 0
 2 5
 3 0
 3 5
 4 0
 4 5
 Level 1 Level 1.5 Level 2 Level 2.5 Level 3
 %
 Pre-visit
 Post-visit
  
Figure 5.13 Students? conceptions of parabolic/satellite dishes (n=26; only HartRAO 
students) 
5.9  Summary of collective learning 
F o r the 5 main Big Ideas (gravi t y , stars, Sun, Solar System, size and scale) there is a shift 
from level 1 towards levels 2 and 3 between the pre- and post- vi si t inter vi e w s (Figu r e 
5.14). Broadly, this suggests that students ch anged their knowledge of Big Ideas from a 
less to a more scienti f i c notion . In the cases of gravity , stars and the Solar System the shift 
was most marke d from level 1 to level 2 (more than 15%). In the case s of the Sun and size 
and scale the shift was greater toward s level 3 (more than 15%). Howeve r , the Solar 
Syste m was based on relati v e l y small numbe r s of  students and interpre tation of changes in 
level shoul d be appro ached with caution.  
158 
0
 1 0
 2 0
 3 0
 4 0
 5 0
 6 0
 7 0
 8 0
 Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 Per
 cen
 ta
 g
 e
 Gravity 
Stars
 Sun
 Solar System
 Size and Scale
  
Figure 5.14 Summary of percentage changes in levels for 5 Big Ideas  
T h e changes in knowled g e level for the two signif i c a n t ideas of ?day and night? and the 
Moon phases , as well as the domina n t artefa c t the satelli t e dish are shown in Figure 5.15 
which shows a similar trend to that of Figur e 5.14, although the shifts involved for day and 
night are less marked than those for the 5 main Big Ideas. The Moon phases show a shift 
from level 1 to level 2, but no shift towa r d s level 3. 
0
 10
 20
 30
 40
 50
 60
 70
 Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post
 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3
 Per
 cen
 ta
 ge
 Day and Night
 Moon Phases
 Satellite Dish
  
Figure 5.15 Summary of percentage changes in levels for significant ideas and dominant 
artefact. 
159 
These shifts sugges t that (at the least) shor t-term increases in students? knowledge have 
occurr e d betwee n the pre- and post-v i s i t intervi e w s , and that it is likel y that the visit itself 
was respon s i b l e for the change . My result s c onfirm the findings of other researchers such 
as Anderson et al . (2000) and Lucas (2000) who have  also noted changes in knowledge 
due to scienc e centre visits . Howeve r , these Austra l i a n studie s also  involve d post-vi s i t 
activi t i e s condu c t e d by teache r s with stude nt s  on their return to the classro om , not 
availa b l e to the studen t s in my st udy. Falk and Dierkin g (2000) also relate various 
evalua t i o n studie s they have conduc t e d that demons t r a t e increa s e d learning by visitors on 
exiting mus e ums . 
The findin g s prese nt e d in this chapte r answe r my first rese a r c h quest i o n ?To what 
extent do students learn in the process of a vis it to a planet a ri u m and the visit or s ? cent r e of 
an astronomical observatory?? They demonstr a t e that learni n g does occur over the course 
of a visit , and that in the cas e of some Big Ideas (partic ul a r l y  gravity, stars, the Sun, size 
and scale , and the satel l i t e dish) the learni n g itsel f is signi fi c a n t . In terms of scien c e 
liter a c y , the lower dime n s i o n s (?kno w i n g a lot of science ? ) of Durant (1993) and Shamos 
(1995) are being addresse d by both the planetarium and HartRAO. 
Howev e r , in addit i o n to deter mi n i n g what is learnt durin g a visit, my inten t i o n in 
this thesi s is to demon s t r a t e how knowledge is acquired. The fo ll o w i n g chapt e r s return the 
reader to the theoretical fr ame w o r k of human const r u c t i v ism which underpins the study, 
analy s e the theor y in terms of the cogni t i ve , affec t i ve and conat i v e domai n s , and prese nt 
analyse s of learnin g by selecte d student s . 
160 
Chapter 6 
6  A Human Constructivist Analysis of Learning 
?That people learn in museums is easy to state, harder to prove? (Falk and 
Dierking 2000). Using Human Constructi vism as a framework for identifying 
how learning can occur, this chapter dem onstrates a typology of learning at my 
study sites. Combined with the notion of Big Ideas from Chapter 5, it provides 
a framework for categorising the students  according to their degree of learning 
about astronomy. 
6.1  Introduction 
C h a p t e r 4 provided a context for my study, showing the visi ts to the planetarium and 
HartRAO from the viewpoi n t of two student s . It also prese n t e d some findi n g s fr om the 
study in narrati v e form, enablin g the reader to gain some insight s into how student s 
experie n c e a visit to a science centre. Chapter 5 provided detailed findi ngs of what  student s 
collec t i ve l y lear n t , showi n g that the major i t y  of them were able to increa se thei r 
knowle d g e and unders t a n d i n g of Big Ideas in astrono my after their visit. Althoug h Big 
Ideas were used as an organis i n g tool fo r comp a r t me n t a l i si n g the astro n o my know l e d g e , 
the concep t knowle d g e levels I identi f i e d in Chapter 5 were derived from the data, using 
inductiv e coding of the Persona l Meani n g Maps and interv i e w transcripts. While this 
method was satisf a c t o r y for findin g out what students learnt, it wa s not so easy to tell how 
student s were learni n g . I theref o r e drew  on my theore t i c a l frame wo r k of Human 
Constr u c t i v i s m, and used catego r i e s of learni n g previo u s l y identi f i e d by other resea r c h e r s 
to classi f y how studen t s learnt during the course of their visit.  In other words, while I used 
bottom- u p coding to identi f y what studen t s we re learni n g , I used top-do w n coding to 
ascertain how they learnt. Henning (2004) stat es that ?the true test of a compe t e n t 
qualit a t i v e resear c h e r come s in the analysi s  of the data? (p 101), and suggests that the 
design logic of the study needs to relate to bot h the forms of analysi s and the actual data 
colle c t e d . This chapt e r descr i be s how I analys e d indivi d u a l studen t s ? learni n g and attempt s 
to explain what sort of ment al processes are taking plac e as the learning occurs. The 
chapt er intro d u c e s categ o r i e s of  knowledge construc t i o n infor med by the theory of huma n 
constru c t i v i s m , gives example s  of these grouping s from th e data, and ends by referri n g 
161 
back to Big Ideas to demons t r a t e how portr aits presented in Chapters 7 and 8 were 
select e d . 
6.2  Human Constructivism and Conceptual Change 
I n Chapter 2 I describe d how construc t i v i s m  is a powerful theory  which underpins muc h 
curre nt think i n g about learn i n g in museu ms . From a cognit i v e perspe c t i v e , I identi fi e d one 
variation of constructivism, Human Const r u c t i v i s m (HC), wh ich attempts to explain how 
people acquir e scient i f i c concep t s by a comb ina t i o n of gradual accreti o n of knowle d g e as 
well as signif i c a n t knowle d g e  restruc t u r i n g (Mintze s & Wandersee, 1998; Novak, 1988). 
Using the HC notions of subsumption, supero rdinate learning, progr essive differentiation 
and integrative reconciliation which were de scribe d in Chapter 2, as well as Anderson?s 
conceptions of knowledge ?transfo r ma t i o n ? (Ander s o n 1999, Anders o n et al. 2003) I 
analys e d my data to identi f y possi b l e HC cat egor i e s which could explai n how the student s 
in my study were learnin g concep t s associ a t e d  with astronomy at the study sites. However, 
as part of this analysis , I wa s mindfu l of Novak? s learni n g pr incipl e 8 which refers to the 
idea that ?thinkin g , feeling a nd acting are integrated? (Nov ak, 1988 p. 93), as well as 
subseque n t research on the role  of the affective and conative  in conce p t ua l chang e during 
informa l learni n g (Alsop & Watts, 1997). Using these human constr u c t i v i s t and concep t u a l 
chang e studi e s , I devise d a codi ng syste m accou n t i n g for cogni t i ve , affec t i v e and conat i ve 
proces s e s which is shown in Table 6.1. 
162 
Table 6.1 Knowledge Construction categories defined in my study 
Code Name and description Domain of 
?learning? 
Code Antecedents from the literature 
Addition ? a concept which is new knowledge to a 
learne r , incre me n t a l l y added . 
Cognit i v e Additi o n (Ander s o n 1999, Anders o n et al . 2003) 
Subsu mp t i o n (Ausu b e l et al . 1978, Mintzes et al . 1997, Pearsa l l et al . 
1997) 
Emerg e n c e ? a conce p t which emerg e s from a learn e r ? s 
me mory as a result of a subsequ e n t experi e n c e .  
Cognit i v e Emerge n c e (Ander s o n 1999, Anders o n et al . 2003) 
Differen t i a t i o n ? a process of  modifi c a t i o n of concept 
meani n g s . 
Cognit i v e Progre s si v e Diffe r e n t i a t i o n (Ausub e l et al. 1978, Mintzes at al. 1997, 
Pearsa l l et al. 1997, Anderson 1999, Anderson et al . 2003) 
Discr i mi n a t i o n ? dema r c a t i o n of simila r i t i e s and 
differ e n c e s among closel y relate d concep t s 
Cognit i v e Mergin g (Ander s o n 1999, Anders o n et al . 2003) 
Integrative Reconciliation (Ausubel et al . 1978, Mintzes et al. 1997, 
Pearsa l l et al. 1997) 
Recont e x t u a l i s a t i o n ? the underst a n d i n g of a concept 
modifie d by a changed contex t , but ?with no signi fi c a n t 
clarif i c a t i o n of meanin g ? (Ander s o n et al. 2003) 
Cognit i v e Recont e x t u a l i s a t i o n (Ander s o n 1999, Anders o n et al . 2003) 
Supero r d i n a t e Learni n g ? a new concept learnt which 
links to other concept s already part of a learner? s 
cognit i v e struct u r e 
Cogni t i v e Super o r d i n at e Learni n g (Ausu b e l et al . 1978, Mintzes et al . 1997, 
Pears a l l et al. 1997) 
Enjoyab l e ? the extent to wh ich the learn i n g expe r i e nc e is 
enjoya b l e 
Affec t i v e Palat a b l e (Also p and Wa tts 1997) 
Germa n e ? the extent to which the learn i n g expe r i e n c e is 
persona l l y relevan t 
Affec t i v e Germa n e (Also p and Wa tts 1997) 
Salie nt ? the exten t to which the learn i n g experi enc e is 
promin e n t or import a n t in the learne r ? s enviro n m e n t  
Affecti v e Salien t (Alsop and Watts 1997) 
Wonder ? the extent to which the learner is in awe or 
amaz e me n t 
Affec t i v e Not descr i bed 
Conat i v e - the extent to whic h the learni n g expe ri e n c e is 
put into action, is controll e d or trusted by the learner.  
Conativ e Action , Contro l , Trust (Alsop and Watts 1997) 
163 
 
In their study of infor ma l learni n g about ra diati o n and radioac t i v i t y , Alsop and Watts 
(1997) propos e d a new model for concept u a l change which involved 4 ?lenses? through 
which the learn i n g could be obser ve d : the cogni t i ve , affec t i v e , conat i ve and self- es t e e m. 
As I explai ne d in Chapte r 2, I have replac e d Alsop and Watts? cognit i v e categor i e s (which 
were taken from the Strike and Posner mo del of concep t u a l change ) with catego r i e s 
derived from human constructivism. I have  retaine d Alsop and Watts? affect i v e and 
conative lenses, but my data collection methods did not lend themse l v e s to meta-a w a r e n e s s 
to enabl e me to use the self- e s t e e m lens. Initi a l explan a t i o n s of the le nses were discuss e d in 
section 2.8.3 and I now describ e each of the cat egor i e s listed in Tabl e 6.1, with examples 
of data from my study corres p o n di ng to each co de. Throug ho u t the rema in d e r of this thesis 
I shall refe r to this frame w o r k as  the Human Const r u ct i vi s t model . 
6.3  Cognitive Domain 
6.3.1  Addition 
Like Anderson (1999) and Anderson et al. (2003), I found that student s acquir e d concep t s 
which were apparen t l y new to them over th e course of my study. By comparing the pre-
 v i s i t PMMs and inter vi ew s with the post- v i s i t data, student s appeare d to have taken up new 
knowle d g e after the visit, at least some of which was direct l y as a result of the visit itself . 
Huma n constr u c t i v i s m has used the term subsu mp t i o n for this proce ss , which has its 
antec e de n t s in Ausub e l ? s meani n gfu l learni n g (Ausu b e l et al ., 1978). I explained the 
process of subsumpt i o n in 2.8.2.1, and would pr efer to use Anderson? s simpl e r term of 
addition, as it captu r e s the essen c e of what appear s to be occurr i ng in the studen t ? s 
cognit i v e struct u r e : studen t s are assimil a t i n g or adding  new conce p t s to their exist i n g 
knowl e d g e witho u t any major re structuring process. Additio n was by far the most common 
for m of learnin g observ e d in my study, tw o example s of which are as follows .  
Brenda? s PMM shows severa l change s from the pre-visit to post-visit, including 
what I coded as 11 exa mpl e s of additi o n . A small portio n of her post-v i s i t PMM is shown 
in Figure 6.1, with the additio n s highlig h t e d . 
164 
 
 
Figure 6.1 Part of Brenda?s post-visit Personal Meaning Map 
A d d i t i o n 1: Brenda has added a fact about distance ? that the Earth is 3 light seconds from 
the Moon. 
Addit i o n 2: Brenda has added the fact th at there are about 100,000 million stars in the 
Milky Way. 
Additi o n 3: Jupite r has more  than enough gravity [in it]. 
Addit i o n 4: Origi n al l y in her pre-vi s i t PMM she wrote ?Ther e is no air or gravi t y in 
space? . In her post visit PMM she change d this to ?a littl e of? gravi t y in space . All four of 
these addi ti ons demon s t r a t e new facts that  Brenda has learnt during the visit. 
The other examp l e is from Sipho ? s inter v i e w . In his pre-vi s i t interv ie w, Sipho?s view of 
stars was relati v e l y na?ve , thus:  
Sipho:  They are also someth i n g like fire in the night ?  That makes a 
littl e bit ?  Shines so that it must, the Earth must not be total ly 
dark?? T h e y are made fr om the sun.  They are little pieces that 
are co ming from the sun. 
(swo 0 5 p r e i n t 126) 
Howeve r , in his post-v i sit interv i e w , Sipho had additi o na l knowle d g e about stars: 
165 
 
Sipho:   Stars are so me are things that, are object s that are made with 
gases and other things , with hot gases which also are very hot also 
like fire, also the same as the sun. 
(swo 0 5 p o s i n t 075) 
The fact that he now could describ e stars as being made of hot gases was an indication that 
he had acqui r e d some addit i o na l knowl e d g e about stars . In the first quota t i o n he refer r e d to 
stars as piece s comi n g from the Sun, in the second quotation he described them as being 
the same as the Sun. 
6.3.2  Emergence 
T h i s categor y of knowled g e constru c t i o n wa s identi f i e d by David Anders o n in his study 
and was not describe d by previous  research e r s using HC (Anderso n et al. 2003). When 
studen t s are tested or questi o n e d prior to a visi t to a scien ce centre , ther e is likel y to be 
conside r a b l e knowled g e in their mind about a phenomen o n that they are not aware of, and 
they do not refer to. During the visit, their e xperiences remi nd them of pr eviously-learned 
knowl e d g e , and after the visit th ey are able to refer to this knowle d g e when questi o n e d . 
The key differ e n c e betwee n emerge n c e and add ition is that students retrieve emergent 
knowle d g e from their me mory , wherea s add ition refers to knowle dge not previously 
known or reme mber e d . This category of ?emerg en c e ? correspo n d s to other HC processe s , 
in that knowledge is retrieved from the student? s me mory and is structu r e d toge ther with 
the new-expe r i e n c e knowledg e . This is likely to resul t in more subst a n t i a l lear n i n g , as the 
previously-learned knowledge is affir med by the new experi e nc e s . Martin Storksdieck 
suggest s that this in fact is  the commonest way in which peopl e learn in museu ms , he 
suggest s that visitor s learn ?by revisit i ng previously known infor ma tion, by bringing 
previou s but current l y embedde d know l e d g e to the forefr ont of thei r atte nt i o n , ther e by 
briefl y ?activ a t i n g ? this knowle d ge that was hidden in their long-t e r m me mory ? 
(Storksdieck, 2004 p. 10). Howeve r , althou g h this may apply to adults who have a greater 
amoun t of knowl e d g e store d in their long-t e r m me mory , this is  less likely with children, 
and both Anderson?s and my study suggest that  addition is the comm onest way in which 
children learn.  
Exampl e s of emerge n c e were found in a nu mber of studen t s in my study, the most 
promin e n t being John, who on severa l occas i on s referred to being reminded of things he 
had learnt about before: 
166 
 
Inter v i e w e r : you said the trip remin d e d you of those thing s .  Tell me a littl e bit 
abou t that . 
John:   Ja they remin d e d me of so me of the things that I was thinki ng 
about like I rememb e r e d the red giant was huge in size, it has way 
mor e mass th an our size and as the one lady said, she said that our 
sun couldn ? t have a supern o v a becaus e it? s too small co mp ar e d to 
those other stars and then the the white dwarf, it? s small in size 
and it spins faste r .  Quasar , I just reme mb e r when I used to 
reme m b e r I reme m b e r it becau s e I looke d it up in the dicti o n a ry 
so I could find out becau s e I was readi n g about it and I still didn? t 
get it and it said huge sourc e of energ y , light s , heat and that? s sort 
of round red giant s star . 
(vho 16 p r e p m m 03) 
In my study I have catego r i s e d knowle d g e  as having emerge d only when studen t s 
speci f i c a l l y state that the visit ?remi n d e d ? th em of someth i n g they previo u s l y knew. The 
distin c t i o n betwee n ad diti o n and emerge n c e was not always  clear - c ut , and this issu e is 
discuss e d further in section 6.7. 
6.3.3  Differentiation 
Ausubel?s original description of the proc ess of progressive di fferentiation was ?The 
proces s of subsump t i o n , occurr i n g one or more times, leads to progressive differentiation  
of the subsu mi n g conce p t or propo s i t i o n? (Ausu b e l et al ., 1978 p. 124 emphas i s in 
origin a l ) . This sugges t s there is an overa rc h i n g concept in the learner ? s mind which 
become s modif i e d or chang e d as a result of the addition of new (subsumed) conce pts, and 
indi c at e s that ther e is a hierar c h i ca l relat i onship between progressive  differentiation and 
subsump t i o n . More simp ly, Anders o n refers to progress i v e differen t i a t i o n as a clarification 
of concep t meanin g s (Ander s o n et al . , 2003), and in his paper does not attempt to identif y 
hiera r c h i e s betwe e n conce p t s . In line with more tradi t i o n a l HC think i n g I prefer to use the 
term diffe r e n t i a t i o n (with o u t the adjec t i v e progressive ) to refer to modific a t i o n of an 
overar c h i n g conce p t , for exampl e Sipho ? s kn owled g e of the Sun. Before his visit to 
HartRAO Sipho stated that the Sun is the bi ggest star, and didn?t know what we can use 
the Sun for. Howev e r , after the visit his knowledge appears to have been modified 
substant i a l l y as shown: 
Sipho :   `The sun.  The sun it is a star and it is born and also dies then 
after 500 billio n years the sun will die and explo d e to be hundr e d 
times bigger than it is then it will have to swallo w two planet s 
which are Mercu r y and Venus . 
Inter v i e w e r : Any th i n g else about the sun you can tell me? 
167 
 
Sipho :  I can say the sun is not the bigge s t star in our solar syste m, but 
there are other bigge r sun, but the sun gives us more heat. The sun 
looks more bigger becaus e it is the neares t star so the other stars 
look very , they are so far so they look you thin k that they are 
small , but they are not.  Some of the m ar e even more bigge r than 
the sun. 
(swo 0 5 p o s i n t 031 & 047-05 1 ) 
In this sequenc e Sipho discus s e s the idea of the Sun?s life histor y as well as the fact that it 
is not the biggest star, but the closest (altho u g h he makes the error of referri n g to our Solar 
Syste m) . Both of these ideas were covere d dur i ng the visit to HartR A O and it is very likel y 
that he differentiated his knowledge of the Sun di rec t l y as a result of th e visit. His error in 
referr i n g to the Sun not being the bigge s t star in our Solar Syste m was fairl y commo n in 
the study, with student s confus i n g the concept of Solar System with galaxy. 
I consid e r that the cruci a l diffe re n c e betwee n the proce s s e s of addit i o n and 
diffe r e n t i at i o n is the exten t to which new facts are integrated into a coherent whole (in the 
case of diffe r e n t i at i o n) or remai n as isolat ed fact s (in the case of addit i o n) . In 
differe n t i a t i n g their knowle d g e stud ents are able to show a greater degree of understanding 
of a concept. 
6.3.4  Discrimination 
H u ma n const r u c t i v i s t s refer to this proce ss as integra t i v e rec onciliation, but I here 
intr o d uc e the term discrimination as I cons i d er it bett er capt ure s the essenc e of the proce s s . 
In the case of discrimi n a t i o n ,  students begin to out line simila r i t i e s and differe n c e s among 
associ a t e d concep t s . The example given by Pear sal l and associa t e s in fact comes fro m 
astrono my : they suggest that integra t i v e reconc iliation occurs when ?i ndividual learn about 
diffe r e n c e s and simil a r i t i e s in the atmos p h e r e s or Earth, Venus and Mars for example, their 
knowle d g e struct u r e s become more interc o n n e c t e d , integr a t e d and cohesi v e ? (Pears a l l et 
al . , 1997 p. 196). Anderson identifi e s merging  as what he calls a simil a r proce s s to 
integra t i v e reconci l i a t i o n in hi s study , where 2 or more separ a t e  concepti o n s are mer ged to 
provid e an explan a t i o n of a newly encoun t e r e d phenome n o n . I did not encoun t e r exampl e s 
of mergin g in my study, and I question whether his process of  mergi n g is in fact very 
simila r to integr a t i v e reconc i l i a t i o n . The proces s of merging conceptions is quite different 
from iden t i fy i n g simi l a r i t i e s and diffe r e nces between associated conceptions. 
A common example of discrimi n a t i o n in my study was when st udents began to 
identi f y simila r i t i e s and differ e n c e s betwee n the plane t s of the solar syste m. For examp l e 
168 
 
Fatima and Judy referre d to the planet name s a nd a few facts about the planet s in their pre-
 v i s i t PMM. Howev e r , in their post- v i s i t P MM they both distin g u i s h e d betwee n the inner 
rocky planet s and the outer gaseou s planet s , as  well as other simil a r i t i e s and differ e n c e s 
betwe e n them. Anoth e r examp l e was when student s could discri m i n a t e betwee n the 
diffe r e n t sorts of stars accor d i n g to their size,  densi t y and other chara c t e r i st i c s . Accor d i n g 
to HC theory , discri mi n a t i o n promot e s linka ges between learners? knowled g e struct u r e s 
which enables more substan t i a l knowle d g e constr u c t i o n . 
6.3.5  Recontextualisation 
A n d e r s o n identi f i e d the proces s of recont e x t u a l i s a t i o n in his study where a studen t 
modifi e d a concep t as the result of the visit, bu t did not signific a n t l y cl ari fy or add to his or 
her meani n g of the conce p t ? the conce pt wa s merel y descr i be d in terms of the new 
contex t . It is distin g u i s h e d from diffe r e n t i a t i o n by the fact that mean ing (even if erroneous) 
is not enhanced. Like the ca tegory of emergence, this c ode was developed by Anderson 
and has no equivale n t in previou s HC researc h . 
I found relati v e l y few exampl e s of recont e x t u a l i s a t i o n in my study. One exampl e is 
Kitso who in her pre-vi si t inter vi e w expla i ne d how the Sun ?rotates around the Earth?. She 
repeat e d this idea in her post- v i s i t inter v i e w , and, using the model Earth and Sun: 
Kitso :   It?s the bigges t star.  It prov i d e s light to the Earth and it rotat e s 
[said slowl y ] aroun d the Earth [said as th oug h memor i s e d , in a 
sing- s o n g way ]. . 
Interv i e w e r : Okay .  Umm.  So why does the sun move across the sky every 
day?  What? s goin g on ther e ? 
Kitso:   Becaus e the sun, it rises. What?  The sun rises and it sets.  So, it 
move s. Bec a u s e ?  Isn? t it ??  It? That? s how life is, because 
there ? s the time.  So, it?s in th e morni n g the sun rises and then. 
Interv i e w e r : Okay .  Show me what? s goi n g on here [sho w i n g Kits o the mode l ].  
If we? re in Africa there, wh at? s going on if that is the sun?  
What? s happ en i n g ?  The risin g and sett i n g . 
Kitso :   That is Afric a , right , so, ?  Umm, let?s see? 
Interv i e w e r Show me ho w the sun rises and sets. 
Kitso :   No wait, the sun rises from the?  The sun sets fro m? .  The sun 
co mes this side, I know (//). So it?s when it sets, so then ?Oh, 
yes, then this side it?s night time then the ? so, as the sun is 
movin g from Afric a , we exper i e n c e ?  Like night time is co min g 
now, fouri s h , fivis h , and then it comes like this, then this part ? 
Inter v i e w e r : The sun goes aroun d the other side? 
Kitso:   Yes. 
[Stud e n t appea r s to demon s t r a t e that the sun moved round the Earth ]. 
169 
 
(scf1 7 p o s i n t 069- 08 7 ) 
In this case, she has not clarifie d her knowled g e about the cause of day and night, 
and the HartRAO context appears to have  produced uncertai n t y in her thinking .  
6.3.6  Superordinate Learning 
According to most reports of HC research, s uperor d i n a t e learning is an importan t concept 
of the learni n g proce s s in indi vidu a l s . It is therefor e striki ng to note that Anderson and 
associ a t e s in their paper on theore t i c a l persp e c t i v e s of learnin g ignor e it as a categor y of 
learning transfor ma tion. Further, they si mplify the concept map of HC (Figure 2.2) 
presente d by Mintzes and Wanderse e (19 98), and misrepr e s e n t the positio n of 
superor d i n a t e learni n g as resulti n g from s ubsumpt i o n . They also draw ambiguou s linking 
lines between progres s i v e differe n t i a t i o n and integ r a t i v e recon c i l i a tion and superordinate 
learni n g . This makes it unclear whethe r pr ogressive differentiation (and integrative 
reconc i l i a t i o n ) result s in supero r d i n a t e le arnin g or vice versa. Ausubel provide s the 
simpl e s t defin i t i o n of super o r d i n a t e learning : ?An inclusiv e ne w proposit i o n (or concept) is 
learned under which several establ i s h e d ideas are subsume d ? (Ausub e l et al ., 1978 p. 59). 
The princi p al featu re of this categ o r y is that  a new concep t is lear ned which includes other 
concepts, some of which may already be known. In HC terms this involves strong 
restr uc t ur i ng in the brai n in order to make sense of the new infor m ation learnt. Mintzes et 
al . descri b e supero r d i n a t e learni n g as follows : ?A new more general and inclusive concept 
is linked to more specific con cepts already a part of the l earner's cognitive structure; for 
example when students learn that visi ble light and radio wave s represent different 
freque n c i e s of electr o ma g n e t i c energy ? (Mintz e s et al . , 1997 p.420). In some ways 
super o r di n a t e learni n g corre s p o n d s to Piage t ? s equil i br a t i o n , where a new balan c e is struc k 
betwe e n exist i n g knowl e d g e and new evide n c e recei ve d from exper i e n c e (Rosc h e l l e , 
1995). 
I found few example s of superor d i n a t e learni ng in my study. I think this is because 
I was dealing with relati v e l y famili a r concep t s  such as the Sun, stars , space and plane t s , 
which the study participants already had so me knowledge about, from previous school 
learni n g . The possib i l i t y of the m learni n g a co mpletely new concept is therefore relatively 
unlik el y ; they are more likel y to diffe r e n t i a t e or discri mi n a t e existing concepts by a 
proces s of additi o n or emerge n c e . Howev e r , I show two contr a st i n g examp l e s . The first 
involves Kitso, who appears to have gained a completely di fferent understanding of the 
170 
 
planet s . She tries to descri b e her previo u s  understa n d i n g , and goes on to explain that her 
new understanding of what a planet  consist s of was a radical departur e from her previous 
knowledge: 
Kitso :   What actua l l y chang e d is ?.the plane t s .  Becaus e I though t 
plane t s ?. No, ?I thoug h t they were just a place.  I didn? t think 
of it as in ?. I don?t know which plan e t it is, but it?s got soil but 
the soil is not so good to plan t in and every th i n g but I never 
thou gh t that plane t s woul d have such thing s , y?kn o w ?  I just 
thought ? It was one of those places wh ere there was a whole lot 
of gravity and you see nothing , so Yeah, I?ve learnt like may b e 
the soil and mo st probab l y there ? s more to it and some of the 
planet s are very colour f u l and bright .  Quite attrac t i v e . 
(scf 1 7 p o s i n t 143) 
The second examp l e is of a stude nt learning how to tell time us ing a sundial. In a series of 
intervi e w probes, Batsile describ e s how he expe ri e n c e d the demons t r a t i o n of the sundia l at 
HartRAO: 
Batsi l e : I told them every t h in g which they heard and how that we can 
mea sure ti me . 
Inter v i e w e r : Tell me about that. 
Batsi l e :   It?s that there are, the proble m is I forgot the names 
Inter v i e w e r : Don? t worry about the name, just descr i b e it, just tell me. 
Batsi l e :   It shows there are hours , month s , ja there are hours , day s and 
nights and time, you see we have to put  it, the time only start at  
5 ja at 5 and I don? t know when it ends and then you have to put it 
on the mont h or in the midd l e of the mont h . 
Inte r v i e w e r : What do you put on the mont h ? 
Batsi l e :   There? s a steel which you have to put on the month and it will 
show what time is it. 
Inte r v i e w e r : It?s a stick you put on the mont h ? 
Batsile:   Ja. 
Intervi e w e r : Okay and then how does it show the time? 
Batsile:   By the shadow. 
Inter v i e w e r : Shado w okay, shado w of the stick .  So it will work when the sun 
is shini n g . 
Batsil e :   Ja only when the sun is shin i n g .  The only prob l e m whic h I don? t 
get it?s winte r.  I didn? t reall y ask the quest i o n but I don?t get it at 
winter there ? s no sun becaus e how do you get time? 
(swo 5 3 p o s i n t 250- 27 4 ) 
This student had no idea previou s l y that the S un can be used to tell time, and explai n s in 
some detail how this is done. He  previously knew a bout the Sun as a star, and giving light 
and heat to the Earth . He also had some knowle d g e about the relati v e move me n t of the 
Earth and Sun, though he showed some confusi o n regar d i n g this in his post-v i s i t interv i e w . 
171 
 
Howeve r , he used these exist i n g conce p t s to link to a new concept of  the Sun as a clock, 
provid i n g an exampl e of superor d i n a t e learni n g . 
6.4  Affective Domain 
F o r severa l years resear c h e r s have  noted the impor t a n ce of the affec t i v e do ma i n in lear n i n g 
both in the for mal schooli ng enviro n me n t (e.g. Alsop & Watts, 2003; Hargreaves, 1996) 
and in museu ms (Dier k i n g , 2005; Rame y - G a s s e r t et al . , 1994) . In my study I identi f i e d 4 
catego r i e s in which studen t s expres s e d differ e nt attitudes and degrees of interest and 
exciteme n t towards the topic of astronomy and their visit to one of the study sites. When 
examin i n g indivi d u a l studen t s in  the portr ai t s in Chapt e rs 7 and 8 I have combi n e d the 
differ e n t affec t i ve catego r i e s into one for the sake of clarit y . 
6.4.1  Enjoyable 
I n this categor y I identif i e d anythin g the stud ent enjoye d or dislike d about the visit or 
topic s they refer r e d to in their intervi e w . Alsop and Watts ident i f i e d a simila r categ o r y 
which they refer r e d to as palatable , in which they determined ?how agreeable how savory 
the materi a l is? (1997 p. 639). Howev e r , the noti on of enjoyment was a clear feature of the 
school visits in my study, as well as the enjoym e n t of a subjec t or t opic at school. Two of 
my interview questions were ?W ha t things did you most enjoy/ like most about the visit?? 
and ?What did you dislik e about th e visit? ? so I almost always heard about spec ific aspects 
that studen t s liked or dislik e d . Howeve r , so me studen t s also referr e d to enjoyme n t when 
asked about anything they had t hought about after the trip or  anyone they had told about 
the trip. For examp l e Fatima refe r r e d direct l y to enjoy me n t : 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Umm.  Other than today ? have you thoug h t about the 
trip since? 
Fati ma:   Yes, I did. 
Inte r v i e w e r : What do you thin k? 
Fati ma:   I though t it was very interes t i n g becau s e I learnt mor e things and 
it? s just very nice going there.  I enjoy ed it. 
(scf 1 5 p o s i n t 145) 
6.4.2  Germane 
T h i s categ o r y was adapte d from the same ele m e nt as Alsop and Watts (1997): they referre d 
to germa n e as being the extent to which some t h i n g is persona l l y relev an t . I used the code 
when studen t s were able to say why they were looking forwar d to going on the trip, why 
they might have an inter e s t in astro n o my as we ll as whethe r they t hought their ideas about 
172 
 
space had changed and how much they had tho ug h t about the visit af ter return. Any of 
these though t s sugges t e d an as pect of astronomy or the vis it which they found to be of 
parti c u l ar inter e st to thems e l v e s . For examp l e after the visit Carol i n e descr i b e d what she 
had told her mother and brothe r .  From this I infer that the act iv i t i e s she descr i be d had some 
impor t a n c e for her: 
Inter v i e w e r : No.  Okay.  When you cam e back have you told anyb o dy abou t 
the visit since? 
Carol i n e :   Yes.  I?ve told my broth e r , I told my mom. 
Inter v i e w e r : What did you just tell me  one or two thing s you actu a l ly told 
them spec i f i c thin g s you told them abou t . 
Carol i n e : I told them we went to go see this satel l i t e and moved and my 
frien d and I ? we walke d away .  I told them littl e jokes every 
now and again and I told them about the rocke t and the thing s we 
did and all that. 
(vho 11 p o s i n t 227- 23 3 ) 
6.4.3  Salient 
A l t h o u g h simil a r to the categ o r y of germa n e , this code was used by Alsop and Watts to 
refer to ?how promi n e n t , or arres t i n g [a topic ]  is withi n the learn i n g environme n t ? (1997 p. 
639). I used it where studen t s could refer to an  issue in the media or their envir o n me n t 
which they ident i fi e d as notic ea b l e or impor t a n t . For exampl e in his pre-vi s i t PMM Sipho 
wrote that ?Last year the scie nce people discover the other pl ane t which was the small e st 
planet in the univer s e and that means we ha ve 10 planets in our univers e ? (swo05p r e 20) . 
Here, the stude n t is refer r i n g to a recen t devel o p me n t in astro n o my that he has heard about , 
althou g h he is confus i n g the term  univer s e with solar system. 
6.4.4  Wonder 
A fourth catego r y within the affecti v e domain that I identifi e d was that of wonder, where 
studen t s showed amazeme n t or awe at someth i n g they had learne d about. This categor y is 
not refer r e d to by Alsop and Watts , and while it may be simi l a r to the categ o r y of germa n e , 
wonder shows a greater degree of incredul i t y or astonish me n t . For exampl e Thapis o ? s 
respon s e is typica l of ma ny studen t s : 
Inter v i e w e r :  No.  Okay.  Is there any th in g you saw at Harte b e e s t h o e k that 
reall y surpr i sed you or chang e d your idea about space or plane t s ?  
Thapis o :   It? s like when like there were differ e n t kind of scales on the floor.  
When you like you can measure yoursel f , the weight of yoursel f 
in differ e n t planet s and ja it was amazin g becaus e I didn? t know 
that we can even measu r e ourse l v e s even thoug h we?re here on 
Earth . (swo 5 9 p o s i n t 170) 
173 
 
The four catego r i e s descri b e d here consti t u t e  the affect i v e lens th rough which I viewed 
student utterances abou t their exper i e n ce s relat e d to thei r visit . The impor t a n c e of the 
affect i v e domain for learni n g cannot be undere s t i ma t e d , and althou g h empiri c a l studie s 
into the role of affect ar e relatively few (Alsop and Wa tts 2003), research in science 
educa t i o n needs to move away from the noti o n that cogni t i v e , affec t i v e and psych o mo t o r 
functions are all separate and able to be co mpa r t me n t a l i s e d . Advoc a t e s of affec t i v e study 
hold a view that ?affe c t surro u n d s cogni t i o n ? , and needs to be inves t i g a t e d as part of 
scien c e educa t i o n in the clas sr o o m , especi a l l y ?at a time wh en student s in the West are 
moving away from scienc e ? (Also p and Watts  2003 p 1046). Disench a n t me n t with science 
appear s to be a worldwi d e phenome n o n (Sj?be r g , 2000) so the need for studies of affect is 
just as true for under-d e v e l o p e d countr i e s as fo r develo p e d ones. Studie s of affect in the 
informa l scien c e educa t i o n envir o nme n t are even rarer than those in the classr o o m. 
Dierk i n g (2005 ) provi de s some recen t evide n c e for the importa n c e of affect in museum 
environments, and the portraits of students desc rib e d in Chapter s 7 an d 8 of this thesi s 
show how important it was in the vis its to the planet ar i u m and HartRA O . 
The genera l catego r y of affect relate s to anothe r typolo g y I referr e d to in Chapte r 2, 
that of scient i fi c litera c y. Shen (1975 ) refe rr e d  to a level of scient i fi c litera c y that enable s 
the person to appreciate (and po ssi bl y criti ci s e ) the achie veme n t s of scien c e . Eleme n t s of 
the affective dimension of knowledge constructi on enable learne r s to appreci a t e aspects of 
astron o my and theref o r e ?impro v e ? their level of  scientif i c literac y, which according to 
Lucas (1983) and Rennie (2001) mus e ums are uniquel y positio n e d to do. 
6.5  Conative Domain 
C o n a t i o n involv e s the desire to perfor m an ac tion , and togeth e r with cognit i o n and affect 
for ms the three compone n t s of mind (Hil gar d , 1980; Huitt, 1999). It relates to the 
intenti o n a l aspect of behavio u r and the free dom to make choices about action (Mischel, 
1996). The conativ e doma in is similar to Stri ke and Posner?s noti on of ?fruitfulness? 
within their cognit i v e concep tual change model (1985), but emphasises the idea of action 
to a greate r exten t . In thei r study of villa g e r s? under st a nd i n g of radi oac t i v i t y , Alsop 
ident i f i e d three facto r s which shape d how the parti ci p a n t s relat e d to the topic : actio n , 
contro l and trust (Alsop , 2000; Alsop & Watts, 1997). I have used simila r catego r i e s to 
identif y how the student s in my study could potentia l l y use their knowledg e of astronomy 
for a purpose , but have combine d the three fact o r s toge t h er as my da ta did not support a 
174 
 
separate category of ? control they perceive d infor mat i on allowed them? (Alsop, 2000 p. 
204) or learne r s ? trust in their own understa n d i n g s . In my  study I used the category when it 
was clear that student s actuall y  carried out an action related to the topic of astronomy, for 
examp l e some stude n t s used their visit exper i e nc e to follo w up direc t l y on somet h i n g they 
had been given or told about, such as Tlo tlo who made her own ho me- ma d e wate r rock e t : 
Tlotlo :  Ja, I did.  I tried the water rocke t , becau s e it ... ja ... 
Inter v i e w e r : You did. 
Tlotl o :   Ja.  I liked it so I did it at home. 
Inte r v i e w e r : Did you mana g e to.  Did it work ? 
Tlotl o : Ja, it worke d . 
Inter v i e w e r : How did you get the parts , the right parts to put it toget h e r ? 
Tlot l o : No, you know what I did I took a 2 litre bott l e and a pump, I did 
that and it just. ?..  It?s like I put water then I just made the pump 
there and then ...it went high. 
Inte r v i e w e r : Did you show any bo dy else ? 
Tlotlo : Ja I showed my friend s . 
(tsw 0 2 p o s i n t 025- 05 7 ) 
Simil a r l y , some stude n t s ident i f i e d that human s have the potent i a l for action with respec t 
to current discoveries that are being made. Fo r examp l e Nnani k i refer r e d to the fact that 
?We can do resear c h to see if  we can live in one of the pl anets e.g. Mars? (tsw15pos 9). 
Simil a r l y , I have adapt e d the idea of trust to includ e the extent to which studen t believ e in 
both scienti f i c and other knowled g e systems su ch as religion and astrology. How students 
grappl e with non-sc i e nt i f i c belief s can affect  the extent to which they will accept the 
scienti f i c knowle d g e which is  the focus of the presenta t i o n s at both study sites. For 
exampl e in some studen t s the visit brough t out a confl i c t in their belie fs, such as Brenda: 
[Int e r v i e w e r : And I can? t reme mb e r if you told me, are you reli g io u s?  Do you 
go to churc h and stuf f ? ] 
Brend a :   Ja I do go to churc h . 
Inter v i e w e r :   What do you think God has to do with the plane t s and space and 
sun? 
Brend a :   Since I went to the trip it? s very comp lica t e d because from what 
I?ve learn t in HSS the world start e d as a small , tiny littl e thing 
and the water , that was writt e n by I don?t know who somet h i n g 
Darwin , but in the Bible it say s it? s Adam and Eve and then 
co mes the plane t and all.  I think it?s a lot comp l i c a t i n g . 
Interv i e w e r : Hmmm.  So you? re not really sure. 
Brend a :   Ja I?m not sure.  I don?t know which one to belie v e , the Bible or 
evol u t i on or some t h i n g . 
(swo 7 0 p o s i n t 245- 24 9 ) 
175 
 
In order for students to think more deeply about the relationship be tween their religious 
belie fs and scient i fi c aspec t s of the Solar Syst em and universe, I cons i de r that the scien c e 
centr e s thems e l v e s shoul d assis t this proce s s . Just as constr u c t i v i s t s use ?discr e p a nt event s ? 
and ?crit i c al mo men t s ? in scienc e lesso n s to force stude n t s to confr o n t their own 
miscon c e p t i o n s (Chinn & Brewer , 1993; Koballa, n.d.), so sc ience centres can identify 
areas of astron o my and cosmol o g y which support or conflic t w ith religiou s beliefs. This 
might enable studen t s to begin thinki n g about  deeper issues of their own upbri n g i n g and 
cultur e in relat i o n to the scien ce that has been prese n t e d . 
6.6  Coding for Human Constructivist categories 
U s i n g the cogni t i ve , affec t i ve and conat i ve doma i n s I devel o p e d categ o r i e s of stude n t 
knowle d g e based on the human co nst r u c t i v i s t fra me w o r k descr i bed in this chapter and 
shown in Table 6.1. I coded each student? s PMM and intervie w s using the categor i e s as a 
typolo g y of learni n g , highli g h t i n g every phrase or utteran c e which could be classif i e d as an 
example of knowled g e constr u c t i o n . I provide  an example of such coding from the 
ATLAS. t i progra m in Append i x E. To ensure  rigour in my analysi s I asked anothe r 
resear c h e r to re-code a selecti o n of my data  based on the categor i e s I had defined in Table 
6.1. She initia l l y found this a diffic u l t proces s mainly becaus e she was not fa mili a r with the 
terms and conce p t s invol v e d in human c onstr u c t i v i s m. Althou g h at the start our 
congru e n c e in coding was only 72%, discus s i o n of the categori e s and theory of HC 
enabl e d compl e t e agree me n t betwe e n us by the end of the proce ss. I provide a brief 
refl e c t i o n on the proce s s of inter - r a t e r reliab i l i t y in section 7.3.  
6.7  Problems encountered with the coding process 
Whereas the coding system I developed for Bi g Ideas was an inductiv e process derived 
from the data, the procedu r e I followe d relatin g students? learning to  human constr u c t i v i s m 
was deducti v e , fitting observe d data to a pre-c o nc e i ve d theor y . On the basis of 
observ a t i o n s and field notes made at my st udy sites I consid e r e d there was a close fit 
between huma n constru c t i v i s m and the types of  processes in learning that I observed 
durin g the visits . Inevi t a b l y , there were times when data observ e d appear e d not to fit the 
categ or i e s descr i be d in HC, and this resul t e d in my adapt a t i o n of exist i n g categ or i es and 
the development of new ones. Further, there were times during the an alys i s when it was 
diffi c u l t to clas si fy stude n t s ? utter a nc e s into catego r i e s . For exampl e , in some cases, it is 
possib l e that what I have catego r i s e d as addi tion may be eme r gent knowledg e that students 
176 
 
were eithe r not aware of thems e l v e s , or did not voice as being somet h i n g they knew 
before. Much of the time, knowledge categorised  as addition is cl early new knowledge, for 
examp l e all four of Brend a ? s addit i o n exa mp l e s  in Figure 6.1 are facts that were referr e d to 
at HartRAO. There is a very st rong likelihood that she learnt th ese facts directly as a result 
of her visit . Howev e r , there are cases where it is more diffi c u l t to tell, for examp l e when I 
did not specifi c a l l y pr obe students on their sources of information. Although I often asked 
whethe r studen t s knew about a partic u l a r piece of knowle d g e prior to their visit, I did not 
alway s do so. In these cases , it is possi b l e that infor ma t i o n coded as addit i o n may in fact 
have been emerg e n t . Simil a r l y , the inter v i e w quest i o n i n g was some t i me s not suffi c i e n t l y 
probi n g , espec i a l l y with retic e n t studen t s , to later determi n e the extent of their knowle d g e 
const r u ct i o n. In order to reduc e the effec t s of  these codin g diffi c u l t i e s , I discu s s e d some o f 
the cases with colle a g ue s to deter mi n e wheth e r my categ o r i s a t i o n s were valid. I also 
presen t e d my work-i n - p r o g r e s s to resear c h  forums at my unive r s i t y and regio n a l 
confer e n c e s while my analysi s was proceed i n g , to enabl e me to be as object i ve and 
impar t i a l as possi b l e .  
The other main diffic u l t y associ a t e d  with coding was related to human 
constructivism and metacogniti o n . Metaco g n i t i o n refers to the abilit y of people to 
consciou s l y reflect on  their learni n g . The lit erature suggests that  although a form of 
metacog n i t i o n has been identif i e d in childre n as young as 5 years old (Larkin, 2000) it 
norma l l y devel o p s slowl y until the age of 12 years , and is infl u e n c e d by the learn i n g 
enviro n me n t and the extent to which it is consciously developed (Berk, 2003). There is 
evidence that South African students do not have well-dev e l o p e d metaco g n i t i v e abilit i es 
(e.g. Case & Gunston e , 2002; van der Riet, Di son & Quinn, 1998) and I did not devise my 
intervi e w questio n s to  invest i g a t e metaco g n i t i o n . Howeve r ,  concept u a l change and human 
const r u ct i vi sm both rely to some  exten t on the abili t y of the learne r to refl e c t on their own 
learni n g , and if I had probed mor e carefu l l y for metac o g n i t i o n during the inter vi e w s I 
might have found addition a l evidence for how studen t s were learni n g at the study sites.  
Despit e the proble ms encou n t e r e d in the codi ng proces s , the process e s of credibi l i t y 
and trust w o rt h i n e s s that I descri b e d in Chapt e r 3 and discussed in th is chapt e r demo n s t r at e 
that the analys i s of my data was rigoro u s e nough for me to make the claims that I put 
forward in subsequent chapters. The following two Chapters, 7 and 8, consist of portraits 
of seven students who show a range of lear ni ng . The chapte r s atte mpt to show not only 
177 
 
what the students learnt about aspects of astrono my , but also how they learnt, and how 
their prior knowled g e and interes t s affecte d their learnin g .  
178 
 
Chapter 7 
7 How individual students learnt about astronomy (1) 
This chapter describes the learning s hown by four students who visited the 
Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy Observator y. Each of these students showed 
different levels of prior knowledge, and the portraits demonstrate the extent to 
which this affected their learning at the science centre. 
7.1  Introduction 
I n order to demon s t r a t e how learn i n g occur r e d in my study , I prese n t a number of portra i t s 
of studen t s , with thei r learni n g descri b e d in cogni t i ve , affec t i v e and conat i v e terms . Using 
Table 7.1 I have selected portraits as char acterising each of the categories of student 
learni n g in terms of Big and Signifi c a n t Ideas. Having presented and discussed how 
learni n g occur s in each of these studen t s , I will demonst r a t e how student s ? prior knowled g e 
and interes t s have influen c e d their learnin g . I w ill then discus s the implic a t i o n s this has on 
learnin g at science centre s such  as those used in this study. 
7.2 The Schools 
7.2.1  Balfour Forest School (BFS) 
O r i g i n a l l y built and run as a school for white stude n t s only durin g the apart he i d era, in 
1990 Balfour Forest was declared a ?Model C?  school, whereby it had some autonomy 
from the provin c i a l depart me n t of educat i o n over its affairs, and was run by a Board of 
Gover n o r s . The schoo l close d as a ?Mode l C? in 1993, and reopened as a norma l public 
school the followi n g year with 420 student s , n one of whom had stayed on from the old 
schoo l . By 2003 there were 570 stude n t s (grade s 1 to 7), and 18 teach e r s . This is a ?Sect i o n 
21? school , which means that the School G over n i n g Body was grante d permi s s i o n by the 
Provin c i a l Depart me n t of Educat i o n to contro l their own finances . In 2003 the fees were 
R1600 1 3  per annum. The class teach e r , Mr. Pete r Malomba had booked for the two grade 7 
classes to visit Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronom y Observatory on 24 October. However, as 
discuss e d in Chapter s 3 and 4, only the student s who paid for the trip were allowe d to go. 
                                                 
 
13  $US 200 at exchange ra tes prevalent in 2006 
179 
 
In class 7x, out of a total of 36 stude n t s , 16 re turn e d informe d consen t forms, and onl y 10 
of these actua l l y parti ci p a t e d in the trip. Of the 39 student s in cl ass 7y, 21 signed consent 
for ms, and 20 of these took part in the visit.  During analys i s of the result s of my study I 
tried to get the academi c result s of the stude n t s who partic i p a t e d , but due to a stolen 
comput e r at the school and othe r relate d proble ms I did not ma nag e to obtai n them. It has 
not theref o r e been possi b l e to relate learni n g at the science centre to student s ? overal l 
acade mi c achie v e me n t . 
7.2.2 Lourdes Girls School (LGS) 
T h i s is a private Catholi c school situate d in  the Western suburb s of  Johannesburg. In 2003 
there were 300 girls from grades 1 to 12, with  160 from grade 8 to 12. The school fees in 
2003 were R1200 per month. The class teache r , Ms Charlene Pell had booked for her grade 
8 geogra p h y class of 21 studen t s to visit the pl aneta r i u m on 14 October . 16 partici p a t e d in 
the visit , all of whom compl e t e d the PMM, and I chose 8 studen t s to inter v i e w prior to and 
after thei r visit . I later obtai n e d acade mi c resul t s for all stude n t s in the class , and was able 
to compa r e their resul t s with thei r learni n g as a result of the visit. 
7.2.3  Achievement School (AS) 
T h i s private school started in 1997 as a prim ary school run on Christia n principl e s , and 
has, due to pressur e from parents , extend e d into  the seconda r y sector. I visited the school at 
its old premises during 2003, but it moved to new and la rger premises in 2004. Although 
students are required to wear uni form, it is relative l y informal , consisti n g of a blue navy T-
 shi r t and denim jeans . Class sizes are relat i v ely small ; the class I visi t e d consi st e d of 16 
studen t s (seven boys and nine girls) , but the classr o o m, due to the compac t nature of the 
premi s e s was cramp e d for this numbe r . The class teach e r , Mrs. Irene Baldw i n , was also the 
teache r of the topic on space and the solar syste m , which was comple t e d in term one 2003. 
14 of the 16 in the class signed consent fo rms , and all of them went on the visit to 
HartRAO on 20 October . Eight of these students were chosen for intervie w . 
7.2.4  Bokamoso School (BS) 
T h i s is a ?Middle School ? (grade s 7 to 9) in  a townsh i p North - We s t  of Pretoria. Under 
apart he i d the towns h i p was devel op e d for Se Tswana - s p e a k i n g people, and most of its 
inhabi t a n t s rema in so today. The school opene d in the early 1980s, and is a feeder school 
to a nearby seconda r y school. The student s partic ipating in the trip to HartRAO consisted 
180 
 
of the scienc e club, and anyone who was prepar e d  to pay the fee for the visit was allowe d 
to go. I met with 18 grade 7 and 8 students on 7 November, all of whom completed their 
PMM, and selecte d 5 for intervi e w . They visited HartRAO on 17 Nove mbe r , and I 
returne d to re-int e r vi e w on 3 Decemb e r . Howeve r , as it was near the end of term, only 7 
studen t s attend e d the post-v i s i t session, and I re-interviewed only 4 student s . I was unable 
to obtain academi c result s for 2003 for the st udents. Several student s in the Science Club 
enter e d the South e r n Skies astro n om y comp et i t i o n , and two won prizes, one of whom 
(Botho) was part of my sample. 
7.3  Big Ideas and Individual Learning 
I n order to examin e studen t s ?  individ u a l learnin g using a Human Const ru c t i vi s t frame w o r k 
I used my analysi s of student s ? knowled g e of Big Ideas in astrono m y as a basis for 
cate g or i si n g them as indiv i d u al s . Instea d of catego r i s i n g their collective knowledge of 
concept s as shown in Chapter 5, I produce d a summary of each individual?s knowledge of 
Big Ideas, the results of whic h are shown in Appendix F.  
For purpos e s of improv i n g resear c h ri gour, I asked a colleagu e knowledg e a b l e 
about astro no my and exper i e nc e d in teachi n g and resea r c h i ng basic astro n o my to asses s 
the extent to which I had allocat e d stude nt s corre ct l y into their pre-v i si t and post-v i s i t 
knowledge categories. She examined data from five students and agreed with 83% of my 
categ or i s at i on . Those she initi al l y did not agree with we discussed and reached a 
conse n s u s . In addit i o n , a teach e r exper i e nc e d in the teachi ng of basic astro n o my who has 
also done some resea r c h in the area also a ssist e d in the valida t i o n of my allocati o n to 
categ or i e s . The inter- r at e r relia bi l i t y betwe e n her and mysel f was 78% for data from six 
studen t s , with the main area of disagr e e me n t being in the domin a n t artefa ct of the 
parabo l i c / s a t e l l i t e dish. This turned out to be  differences in interpretation of categories 2 
and 3. Again, with discuss i o n we reached a cons e n s u s of nearl y 90%, and I am satis f i e d 
that my own codin g is relia b l e . Some quali t a t i v e researc h e r s such as Henning (2004) are 
highly scepti c a l of the notion of inter-r a t e r relia bility. Nigel King s uggests that such 
independ e n t scrutiny should happen early in the coding proces s to improve the coding 
templa t e and discussi o ns then take place betwee n the coders to furthe r revis e the templ a t e 
(King, n.d. retrie v e d 20 July 2006). Howeve r , I consid e r that even if this approa c h is taken, 
a check by an independ e n t coder late in the analysis process is a useful method of ensuring 
181 
 
a more rigoro u s study, as change s in a resear c h e r ? s mind occur s over tim e, with the result 
that the initial coding templat e can be altered without the research e r noticing . 
On the basis of the data in Append i x F, I then placed stude nts into categor i e s using 
the combin e d exten t of their pre- and post-v i s i t knowle d g e of Big Ideas . My first attempt at 
categor i s a t i o n split studen t s ? pre-visi t knowledg e into low, medium and high catego r i e s , 
and their post-visit knowledge into the degr e e to which they had decr ea s e d , rema i n e d the 
same or increase d their knowledg e . All student s showed either the same knowledg e or an 
increas e in knowled g e , and th e differ e n t i a t i o n betwee n categor ies was based on the number 
of knowledge levels by which st udents increased. This origin al categorisation scheme is 
shown in Appendix G, and was di scar d e d becau s e I conside r e d that it did not capture their 
change in knowle d g e in a suffic i e n t l y fine-g r a i n e d manne r . Inste a d , I decided to calculate 
the mean of the pre- and post-vi s i t knowl e d g e  categor i e s for each student , and these are 
shown in Appendi x F as mean pre- and mean post-sco r e s . I then plotted scatterg r a ms o f 
pre-v i s i t me an score s (x axis) versus post-v i s i t mean scores (y axis) for all studen t s (Figur e 
7.1).  
1. 0
 1. 5
 2. 0
 2. 5
 3. 0
 3. 5
 1. 0 1. 5 2. 0 2. 5 3. 0
 Mean score pre-visit
 Mea
 n 
sco
 re
  po
 st
 -vis
 it
  
Figure 7.1 Scattergram of students?  pre- and post-visit mean scores 
F i g u r e 7.1 shows that st udents? pre-visit score correlates close l y to their post- vi s i t score 
(correlation coefficient is 0.88). This suggest s that the extent of  students? pre-visit 
knowl e d g e deter mi n e s their post- v i s i t mean scor e, for exa mple if an individ u a l ? s pre-vi s i t 
score is low, then their post- v i sit score is likely also to be  low. Howev e r, the graph also 
shows that all post-vis i t score s are eithe r the same as or be tter than their pr e-visit scores. I 
182 
 
am using ordina l data and some statis t i c i a n s  do not recomme n d calcu l a t i n g a mean for such 
data (Gorard , 2001), as the differe n c e in sc or e betwe e n each ratin g is not equal . For 
example , in Table 5.3 the differe n c e betwee n kn owledge levels 1 and 2 is not the same as 
between levels 2 and 3. For this reason I re calc u l a t e d the mean scores using the Rasch 
technique (Boone & Rogan, 2005), which allows  non-line a r data to be converte d into 
linear data. Append i x H shows the same data as Figure 7.1, but recalculated using the 
Rasch proced u r e (resul t i n g in a different scale). However, the points plotted are very 
similar to those in Figure 7.1, an d the correlation coefficient is 0.90. On the basis of this 
simil a r i t y , I regar d the means calc ul a t e d in Ap pen d i x F as credi bl e , despi t e the fact that I 
have used ordin a l data to calcu l a t e them. I then used Figure 7.1 to identif y studen t s about 
whom I could write word portr ai t s which desc r i b e their detai l e d lear ning. Word portraits 
are used mainly in ethnographic research as  a techniq u e to presen t data ?in which the 
informa t i o n is integr a t e d in the format of a description of  a person, but with information 
about others and about the c ontext? (Henning 2004 p 112).  
I devise d a set of crite r i a which I used to  choose the most approp r i a t e studen t s for 
the writing of portrait s for Chapters 7 and 8: 
? A n approx i ma t e 20% sample 1 4  of the total student group. 
? S t u d e n t s who showed no change in learning . 
? S t u d e n t s who showed change in learning . 
? S t u d e n t s across the full range of mean scores, from low to high. 
? Both study sites should be represented. 
? I f I made a choice betwee n two or more simila r studen t s on the contin u u m , I chose 
the one who exempl i f i e d intere s t i n g or  unusua l huma n constr u c t i v i s t learni n g . 
On this basis I chose 7 studen t s (Fi gur e 7.2) identif i e d on the graph by their 
pseudon y ms . 
                                                 
 
14  This is in lin e with oth er simi lar stud ies e.g . And erson  (1999 ) wro te 5 portr aits out of a total of 28 stud en ts 
in the class. 
183 
 
Fatima
 Nonkululeko
 Neo
 Botho
 John
 HelenBrenda
 1. 0
 1. 5
 2. 0
 2. 5
 3. 0
 1. 0 1. 5 2. 0 2. 5 3. 0
 Mean Pre-visit Big Idea Score 
Mea
 n 
Pos
 t-vi
 si
 t 
Big
  Ide
 a 
Scor
 e
   
Figure 7.2 Graph showing students across the ra nge of learning about whom portraits are 
written. 
Although Figure 7.2 is a good graphical representa tion of how students fit on the learni n g 
conti n u u m, I also categ o r i s e d th e student s into a tabular scheme as a more conveni e n t way 
of descri b i n g their learni n g . This scheme (T able 7.1) shows the extent to which studen t s 
increas e d their knowled g e about Big Ideas. Th e pink-sh a d e d area shows that no student s 
demons t r a t e d an overall decrease in their knowle d g e a bout these ideas afte r the visit. This 
is signi fi c a nt in that the visit s did not impa rt substantial misconceptions to students which 
made the m change whatev e r correct knowled g e about  Big Ideas they might have had. The 
yello w - s h a de d secti o n of the tabl e ident i fi e s the 13 students (38%) who performe d at the 
same level in thei r pre- and post inter vi e ws, while the blue-shaded area shows the 21 
students (62%) who exhibited an increase in  their knowledge of Big Ideas. Students 
ident i f i e d in red are those whose portr a i t s appea r in this chapt e r and Chapt e r 8, and are 
refer r e d to as portrait students. 
184 
 
Table 7.1 Categorisation scheme for students (n=34) based on their pre- and post-
 visit knowledge of Big Ideas concepts. 
D 2.6-3.0  Brend a  Helen , Pau l, 
Judy 
John , Susan , 
Anto n ia , 
Richa r d 
C 2.1-2.5  Sibong ile, 
Sarah , 
Bhekiw e , 
Banyana, 
Julius, 
Thap iso, 
Nnan ik i, 
Vicky, Ross 
Neo , Lara, 
Sipho , 
Doug las, 
Phillip 
 
B 1.6-2.0 F a t i ma , Tlo tlo , 
Kitso , Zan ele, 
Mpho , 
Ntob e ko , 
Batsile, Fan e 
Botho , 
Caro lin e 
  
P
 os
 t Vi
 si
 t Me
 an
  S
 co
 re
 s 
A 1.0-1.5  Nonku lu le ko , 
Theresa 
   
1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
A B C D 
 
Pre Visit Mean Scores  
The mean pre-visit score was 1.9 (SD=0.46) while the mean post-visit score was 2.2 
(SD=0. 4 5 ) . Like the scatte r g r a ms in Figure 7.1 and Figure 7.2, the table provide s evidenc e 
that student s ? knowle d g e levels either rema in e d the same or increased as a result of their 
visit to one of the study sites . The lowes t knowle d g e leve l I assigne d to each Big Idea (as 
shown in Table 5.3) was level 1, represe n t i ng a relatively limited knowledge of the idea. 
Table 7.1 shows that even if student s have this relative l y limited prior knowl e d g e of Big 
Ideas (Pre- vi s i t A) it is possi b l e for them to move to the next level (Post - v i s i t B), which 
eight of the ten stude n t s at this level accomp l i sh e d . The table also shows that studen t s at 
the level of Pre-vis i t B (12 stude n t s , or one third of the total ) can readi l y move to Post-
 l e v e l C, as nine achiev e d this and one move d to post-D . The eight  students with more 
substa n t i a l prior knowle d g e (pre- v i s i t C) also appear to be  able to increase their knowledg e 
(altho u g h less easily than those in the pre-vis i t B categor y ) as 3 of them were able to move 
to the Post-v i s i t D catego r y . My study was not intend e d to be a compa r i s o n betwee n the 
planeta r i u m and HartRAO , and it is im por t a n t to note that  stude n t s visi t i ng either study site 
were able to increas e thei r knowle d g e of Big Ideas. Compar ing Table 7.1 with the schools 
at which the studen t s were studyi n g shows so me broad trends. Students from Balfour 
Forest (BFS) are distri but e d ac ros s the table , but with less repres e n t a t i o n at the post-D 
level. Lourde s Girls School (LGS) studen t s mostly show improv e me n t , and are found at 
the higher levels of the table. The eight students from Achieveme n t School (AS) are 
185 
 
dispersed across the table, while the four fr om Boka mos o Schoo l (BS) are at the lower 
levels . I ma ke these observ a t i o n s to demonst r a t e that a student?s positi on on the table is not 
solely a reflection of the sc hool at which they are studyin g . 
Table 7.2 Categorisation scheme showing student numbers from each school 
D 2.6-3.0  BFS 1 LGS 1 
AS 2 
LGS 2 
AS 2 
C 2.1-2.5  BFS 4 
LGS 2 
AS 2 
BS 1 
BFS 4 
LGS 1 
 
B 1.6-2.0 B F S 4 
LGS 2 
BS 2 
AS 1 
BS 1 
  
P
 os
 t Vi
 si
 t Me
 an
  S
 co
 re
 s 
A 1.0-1.5  BFS 1 
AS 1 
   
1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
A B C D 
 
Pre Visit Mean Scores  
The changes in knowledge shown in Table 7.1 are with respect to knowledge of Big Ideas , 
while my study aime d also to deter mi n e ch ange in astron o my knowl e d g e that studen t s 
themselves identif i e d , as shown in the PMMs. So althoug h the finding s about Big Ideas are 
interes t i n g , they lack theoret i c a l depth, and in my study I had planned to try and 
unders t a n d studen t learni n g about  basic astro n o my in a more  nuanced way. The value of 
Table 7.1 is to provide a cate gorisation scheme for a more  detail e d examin a t i o n of 
student s ? learnin g . A de tailed examinat i o n reveals not only what students learnt about the 
Big Ideas but also how students learn and what contributes to this learning (Chapters 7 and 
8).  
7.4  Use of Astronomy-Related Vocabulary 
Data on students? knowledge of Big Ideas was obtained mainly through the pre- and post-
 vi s i t inter vi e w s descri be d in Chapt e r 3. I analysed students? Personal Meaning Maps 
princ i p a l l y in order to deter m i n e what individ u a l studen t s l earnt during their visit, as 
described in Chapters 6 to 8. However, as recomme n d e d by John Falk (Falk 2003) I was 
also able to use the PMMs to determi n e the ch ange in students? use of vocabulary, as this 
should docume n t ?the extent of a person ?s awareness and unde rstanding of ?.. the 
conce p t ? (Adel ma n et al. 2000 p 39).  
Views on sc ient i f i c vocabu l a r y are varied . Pushki n (1996) sugges t s the need for the 
scienc e educat i o n commun i t y to use the exis tin g scient i f i c termin o l o g y approp r i a t e l y and 
186 
 
accura t e l y , while others propos e that ?the meani n g for terms is varied and always has to be 
negoti a t e d ? (Slisk o & Dykstr a , 1997 p. 656). Pr opone n t s of ?talkin g science ? suggest that 
students develop their science language in the same way as they find out their way through 
the real world, by develo p i n g it though their ex perien c e s (Roth, 2005). The extent to which 
students used scientific words in their PM Ms is a reflect i o n of their own ability to 
partic i p a t e in the scienc e class. Howeve r , it is possib l e for them to use astron o my - r e l a t e d 
words, yet only know them as words, and not be able to ascribe any meaning to them, as 
some stude n t s demo n s t r a t e d in their inter v i e w when I asked them about what they had 
written . 
I determi n e d the change by countin g astr onomy - r e l a t e d words used by students in 
their pre-visit PMM and counting the additiona l words used in thei r post-v i s i t PMM. The 
results for all 34 students are listed in Appe ndix I, and a summary is provided in Table 7.3. 
Table 7.3 Summary of increase in astron omy-related vocabulary shown by students 
(n=34) 
Number of words used 
in pre-visit PMMs 
Additional number of 
words used in post-visit 
PMMs 
M e a n Stand a r d 
Devia t i o n 
Mean Stand a r d 
Devia t i o n 
20. 3 7.5 6.8 4.0 
As the samp le was not randoml y collect e d I have  made no attempt to use stati s t i c al tests on 
my data, and they are diffi c u l t to compar e with report s fro m the litera t u r e , which do not 
provid e raw figure s , but only the result s of paired t-test s (Adelma n et al . 2000, Falk et al . 
1998). The standa r d deviat i o n s in Table 7.3 show that there is considerable variation in the 
mean numb er of words used by students both prior to and after their visit to the scien c e 
centre , reflec t i n g the very indivi dual nature of what students learnt. Students such as Lara, 
Douglas and Neo wrote down more words in th eir post-visit PMM than  they did in their 
initia l PMM, while Zanele , Bhekiw e and Caroli ne only increase d by on e word. Overal l , the 
mean figures suggest that student s increas ed their astronomy-rela t e d vocabula r y by one 
third, which is a useful benchmar k fo r futur e studi e s of this natur e . 
I mapped the extent of students ?  vocabulary from their PMMs on to the data summa r i s e d in 
Table 7.1, but this demons t r a t e s that the mean  number of words used by studen t s in the 
different categories shows little relationshi p to their prior knowledge. The only trend 
187 
 
evident is that student s w ith the highe s t level of pr ior knowledge (category D) do 
appar e n t l y use more words in  their PMMs and in crease d these by the greatest amount 
(Table 7.4). This suggests that counting words used by student s in their PMMs is not a 
reliable way of assessing th eir concept knowledg e or under standing, except perhaps for 
student s who use a large number of words. 
Table 7.4 Mean number of astronomy-related words used by students in the pre- 
visit and additional words used in post-visit PMMs categorised by Big Ideas scores 
   Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post Pre Post 
D 2.6-
 3.0 
    23 7 31 11 
C 2.1-
 2.5 
  23 5 13 9   
B 1.6-
 2.0 
1 7 6 15 5     
P
 os
 t Vi
 si
 t Me
 an
  
Sc
 or
 es
   (a
 dd
 it
 io
 na
 l 
w
 or
 ds
 ) 
A 1.0-
 1.5  
20 8       
1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
A B C D 
 
Pre Visit Mean Scores  
7.5  The Students 
T a b l e 7.5 (based on Table 7.1) shows the st udent s I selecte d whose learni n g about 
astro n o my I will descr i be in detai l .  
Table 7.5 Students selected for whom portraits were written, using the 
categorisation scheme of students developed in Chapter 6. 
D 2.6-3.0  Brend a 
 
Helen 
(3) 
John 
(4) 
C 2.1-2.5   (10) Neo 
(5) 
 
B 1.6-2.0 F a t i ma 
(8) 
Botho 
(2) 
  
P
 os
 t Vi
 si
 t Me
 an
  
Sc
 or
 es
  
A 1.0-1.5  Nonku lu le ko 
(2) 
   
1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
A B C D 
 
Pre Visit Mean Scores  
Number s in (brac ke t s) show total stude n t s in each categ or y  
O f the students who were selected for portrait s  6 of the 7 are female. This is a slightl y 
higher number than the propor t i o n of fe male s to males in the study (25/34, or 74%), but the 
alternat i v e choices for represen tative students also tend e d to be femal e . Two students (both 
from Lourdes Girls School) visited the planet a ri um , while the remain d e r visit e d Hart R A O . 
188 
 
For each stude n t I have descr i b e d in the portr a i t why they were chose n as being 
repres e n t a t i v e of their catego r y , and have pr ovided a reduced version of their post-vis i t 
PMM for exami n a t i o n (all 7 PMMs are shown fu ll- s i z e in Appen d i x J). Each stude n t ? s pre-
 vi s i t PMM was in blue pen, with red ink comm en t s by me, the intervi ew e r . Their post-v i s i t 
PMM was added to or edite d by the stude n t in penci l (grey ) , with addit i o n a l comme n t s by 
the intervi e w e r in green. In the portrai t s , all the studen t s ? words are quoted verba t i m, 
inclu di n g spell i n g error s from their PMMs and gramma t i c a l l y incor r e c t phras e s from their 
inter v i e w s . 
7.5.1  Portrait of Nonkululeko (swo26) 
Table 7.6 Position of No nkululeko on Big Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0    
C 2.1-2.5     
B 1.6-2.0    
A 1.0-1.5  Nonkululeko  
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-
 2.0 
2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
Table 7.7 Nonkululeko?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Grav ity Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Parab o lic/ 
Satellite Dish 
Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 n/a 2 2 1.0 1.2 
Nonkulu l e k o was chosen as being represe n t a t i v e of student s in the AA categor y . 
Accor d i n g to my analy s i s of her PMM and inter vi e w , she went on the trip to 
Hartebee s t h o e k Radio Astronomy Observat o r y with little knowle d g e about Big Ideas in 
space and astro n o my , and, compa r e d with the other stude nt s , gaine d littl e from the trip. 
Both the stude n t s in the ?AA? categ or y had the lowes t mean score for big ideas of all the 
partic i p a n t s in the study (Nonk u l u l e k o from 1.0 to 1.2, There s a rema i n e d at 1.2). Howev e r , 
it is import a n t to note that Nonkululeko did gain something  from the trip, which I hope to 
bring out in this portrait. 
Nonkulu l e k o is a 12-year - o l d grade 7 girl st uden t and has attend e d Balfo u r Fores t Schoo l 
since grade 1. Nonkululeko lives in a middle class suburb a few kilome t r e s North- W e s t of 
Johann e s b u r g city centre , and travel s to the school each day by minibu s taxi. At home she 
only speaks isiZul u , a languag e which is co mmonly used by nearly a quarter of the 
populati o n of South Afri ca (S tatistics South Africa 2001). 
189 
 
Nonkulu l e k o drew her PMM on 8 th  October and was intervi e w e d on 15 t h  Octob e r , 9 
days before she visited HartRA O on 24 th  Octob e r toget he r with the classe s who attend e d . 
On 30 th  October, all the students who had been on the field trip di d their repeat PMM 
during school time and I re-int e r v i e w e d  Nonkulu l e k o several days later on 10 th  Nove mbe r . 
Nonkululeko was one of a few st udents whos e interv i e w was over two weeks after the 
visit , and this may be partl y respo n s i b l e  for her appar e n t limit e d cogni t i ve uptak e . 
Howeve r , I do not consid e r it the primar y r eason, as other student s who were intervi e w e d 
at the same time as Nonku l u l e k o fell into  other catego r i e s (e.g. Ntobek o in AB and 
Thapiso in BC). 
While Nonkululeko knew roughly where she was going on the visit to HartRA O 
and that it was related to ?the planets?, she didn?t know why the class was going there . It is 
quite possible that her k nowle d g e of the venue for the visit was due to the filling in of her 
PMM for me, rather than anythin g provid e d by the school. She professe d to be looking 
forwa r d to the visit, as ?I want to know about plane t s and the ? the galaxy and all ?? 
(swo26 p r e i n t 043). She also agreed that she was interested in the topic, but appeared to be 
just saying this becaus e  she though t I wanted to  hear it, as her reason was another reference 
to knowing about planet s . She was able to give an example of astrono m y in the news, as 
she made a refere n c e to a newspa p e r report of  a new planet being discovered, which she 
reme mbe r e d as ?Clar a ? . Howev e r , she also went on to say that ?the other plane t s , they 
come and visit Earth? (swo26p r e i n t 175) and I could not interpre t wh at she meant by this. 
Nonkululeko had some experience of looking at  the stars and planet s, as she related a 
recent ?tour? she had been on where the organi s e rs pointed out the constellation of Scorpio 
and the plane t Mars. While she remembe r e d that  Mars ?looked like a star but it was red? 
(swo26 p r e pm m 059), she couldn ? t reme mbe r what  Scorp i o looke d like becau s e ?I didn? t 
see properly ? (swo26pr e p m m 067). 
Nonkululeko said that she liked sc hool because ?We learn many things? 
(swo26 p r e i n t 195). Her favour i t e su bjec t s were Englis h becaus e it ?teach[es] me how to 
read and write? (swo26 p r e i nt 207) and life orient a t i o n becau s e it?s about peopl e . In 
contras t her least favouri t e subject s were  economi c and ma nage me n t scienc e and human 
and social science, because they are both hard and she do esn ? t alway s under s t a n d them. 
When asked about her career plans, she said she would like to be a teacher ?but aih it?s 
hard? (swo26 p o s i n t 202). In her spare time, Non kulul e k o said she watched televisi o n (soap 
operas and childre n ? s ? progra m me s ) and wr ote in a small book, not a diary, but ?my 
190 
 
person a l detai l s and music ? (swo2 6 p r e i nt 231). The most recent book she read was a 
teenage novel in English. Nonkulul e k o is not sure if scienti s t s  have ever found life outside 
the Earth, and thinks that there might be life on Mars. She was emphati c that aliens do not 
exist ?There is nothing like aliens.  They are just made up? (swo26p r e i n t 297), and does 
not read the horosc o p e . Nonkul u l e k o consid e r s he rsel f religi o u s (a Christi a n ) but has never 
really thought about how her faith  might relate to the univers e . 
In her use of astrono my words and term s Nonkulul e k o ? s astronomy knowledg e was 
about averag e . While she used 18 astron o my - r e l a t e d words in her pre-vi s i t PMM (the mean 
for the study was 20, and for her school was 16) her post-visit PMM increased by 7 words, 
slightly above the school and study mean. All the vocabul a r y she used was common to 
other studen t s in the study, but on the basis of  her increase in vocabula r y she appeared to 
have gained some knowl e d g e from the trip. 
 
Figure 7.3 Nonkululeko?s pre-visit Personal Meaning Map 
191 
 
Nonkululeko?s pre-visit personal meaning map (Figure 7.3 and Appendix J) was 
simil a r to those of many other stude n t s in the study. She listed the nine planet s togeth e r 
with some brie f fact s abou t seve r al of them. Fo r examp l e that Jupit e r is the bigge st plane t 
and Mercury is the closest planet to the Sun. She referred to stars as being ?a lighting 
thing? (swo26 a p r e 11) created by God, and that  they are our ?friend s , family and ne gbour ? 
(swo26 a p r e 18). She also referr e d to stars being at the gala xy and Milky Way. She stated 
that space consis t s of open sp ace, contai ni n g planet s , stars,  galax y and the Milky Way. 
When probed about her PMM, she confirme d that ?God created stars so th at it can shine at 
night? (swo26 p r e p m m 003). Althou g h she knew th e term galaxy she wa s unable to explain 
its meani n g or its relat i on s h i p to the term Milky Way. She furth e r refe r re d to a space s h i p 
and rocket, althou g h she found diffic u l t y in expr ess i n g herself here. When I asked if she 
wanted to say it in is iZulu she said no, suggesting that she just found expression difficult, 
rather than it being a languag e proble m, thus: 
Intervi e w e r : Okay .  If you said that  in Zulu , could you tell me more ? 
Nonk u l u l e ko : No. 
Inte r v i e w e r : You woul d n? t be able to? 
Nonk u l u l e ko : Yes.  
(swo2 6 p r e p m m 081) 
She also appea r e d to have diffe r i ng ideas on aliens. Having said she doesn?t believe in 
them in the struct u r e d inter vi e w , she menti o n e d that some planet s have them. She could 
not explain the discrepa n c y . 
During the struct u r e d interv i e w relate d to Big Ideas, Nonkululeko demonstrated 
very limited unders t a n d i n g of many concept s . He r idea of stars was limit e d to them shini n g 
at night and their shape (?like starfishes?), but did not know their size or composition 
(level 1). Nonkululeko?s ideas about the sun we re fairly mi nimal and na?ve. She knew that 
it is big and yellow and can be used for drying things (level 1). Howeve r , her concep t of 
big was not probed , and in a subseq u e n t answ er she thought that the Sun and Moon are the 
same size; overa l l she was classi f i e d for size and scal e as being at level 1. She could not 
expla i n why the Sun moves acros s the sky each day, excep t to expre s s that ?beca u se the 
sun needs places to go?. She appear e d to be lieve that it really is the Sun moving to 
?Americ a and all those places ? (swo26preint 79, 83), and was classified at level 1. 
Nonkulul e k o could describe how the Moon cha nges shape over the course of a week, but 
did not know why this is (level 1). 
192 
 
When asked about a satellite , Nonkululeko said she had se en one, but struggled to 
expres s hersel f furthe r . When referr e d to a sate ll i t e dish, she was able to say that it is round 
and ?half? , and points towar d s a spaces h i p, ?beca u s e the spaces h i p gives us the 
programmes and all those things ?? (swo26preint  151), and was classified at level 2. She 
was the only studen t in the study who profes s e d both to not knowing what gravity is, and 
not knowing the word (level 0). 
After her visit to HartRA O , Nonku l u l e k o added considerably to her PMM (Figure 
7.4 and Appendi x J), filling the reverse side  of the paper with numerou s facts.  
 
Figure 7.4 Nonkululeko?s post-visit PMM 
S e v e r a l facts were a repetit i o n of her pre-vis i t PMM, such as her refere nc e to the 9 planet s , 
Pluto being the coldest , Mercur y being the hottest and stars being in the galaxy. However , 
she wrote down severa l new pieces of informa t i o n , inclu di n g the follow i n g: 
? Which bottles went high and low (refe r e n c e to the Coke bottl e rocket s ) 
193 
 
? A d d i t i o n al plane t s to th e nine named ones.  
? A d d i t i o n a l facts abou t the nine planet s 
? B l a c k spots on the Sun 
? V a r i o u s feature s of Mars: water, land, and orbit. 
? A description of the Moon landing a nd the time taken to get there 
? A star bigger than the Sun 
Further probing of several of the ideas was not  carried out to any gr eat exte n t , due to time 
constr a i n t s in her intervi e w . Her unders t a n di n g of a galaxy was still minima l : ?A galaxy I 
think is where the stars stays and the moon and the solar system and the everythi n g ? 
(swo26pospmm 35), but she understood that it w ould contain thousand s  and thousands of 
stars . Her belie f in alien s was still ambiv a l e n t : in the post-v i s i t PMM she wrote that she 
believ e d in them, but when questi o n e d she sa id she did not, althoug h she had read about 
them in a magazin e . I conside r that Nonkulu l e k o ? s change s to her PMM are suppleme n t a r y 
facts that she has accumul a t e d from her visit : examp l e s of addit i o n in human const r u c t i vi s t 
terms . 
During the structured interview, several of Nonkululeko?s ideas rema ined the same 
or very simila r to her pre-vi s i t ideas . These include d the follow i n g : 
? H e r concep t of the sun as be ing ?big?, shining by day and moving round the Earth each 
day; 
? A l t h o u g h she now thought the S un is bigger than the Moon, she could not tell which 
was closer to the Earth; 
? Stars as shining at night; 
? A satell i t e dish pointi ng to a spaceship in or der to give us progr a m me s on televi s i o n 
? A n inabili t y to talk about what gravity is or does 
 
I n these structur e d question s Nonkululeko was not able to arti culate clearly any significant 
change in her knowle d g e about the Sun, stars, satellites or gravity. She did however state 
that star s are bigger than the Earth, wherea s previo u s l y she c ould not tell their size. Table 
7.8 shows Nonkulu l e k o ? s learnin g  analy s e d using my human const r u c t i v i s t model . This 
and subseq u e n t tables for the portra i t studen t s shows the freque n c y of  HC codes ident i fi ed 
in students? pre- and post-vis i t PMMs and inter vi e w s . I incl ude the pre-vi s i t columns as it 
is possi b l e for studen t s to expres s affect i v e an d conative knowledge prio r to their visi t , for 
exampl e their excite me n t befor e going or their persona l prepara t i o n for the visit. 
194 
 
Table 7.8 Frequency of Human Constr uctivism codes for Nonkululeko 
Knowledge 
Construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview  
PostPMM & 
Interview  
Pre 
Interview 
Post 
Interview  
Totals 
A dd itio n n/a 10 n/a 4 14 
Emerge n c e n/a 1 n/a 0 1 
Differen tiatio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Discr imin a tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Reco n tex tu alisatio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Affective 0 0 5 4 9 
Conativ e 0 0 0 0 0 
Totals 0 11 5 8 24 
In Human Constr u c t i v i s t term s Nonkul u l e k o showed severa l additions to her knowledge 
after her visit , but no other examp l e s of cogni t i v e learni n g in the HC fr ame w o r k , such as 
superor d i n a t e learnin g or differe n t i a t i o n . If N onkulu l e k o had been gi ven a for mal written 
pre- and post- t e s t about simpl e conce p t s in astro n o my befor e and after her visit to 
HartR A O , the resul t s of her struc t ur e d inte rvi e w indicat e that she would have shown no 
gain in knowle d g e . From a non-co g n i t i ve view po i n t , Nonkul u l e k o admitt e d to enjoyi n g 
playi n g with the rocke t s and was surpr i s e d that  the whispering dishes worked and that the 
radio teles c o p e can receiv e messa g e s from sp a ce. She found the slide show on the Moon 
landings ?boring?. After the visi t she was unable to explain th e purpose of the visit, but 
when promp t e d she agree d that both learni n g and fun were related to  the visit?s purpose . 
She told me that although she would like  to visit Hartebee s t h o e k Radio Astronomy 
Observatory again, she had told nobody about the visit, and had not thoug ht about it since. 
In the study, she was the only student inte rviewed who professed to having not told 
anybod y , and one of 3 who had not though t about  the trip since (althoug h only 22 students 
were asked this question). These negative affectiv e aspects of her visit suggest that the visit 
did not have a strong emotio n a l effect on N onku l u l e k o , and may be part of the reaso n for 
her mini ma l learni n g of Big Ideas from the visit. 
However , the results of her P MM indicate that in fact she did learn somet h i n g from the 
visit. She was able to descri b e how Neil Armstr o n g was the first (?whit e ? ) man on the 
Moon, and that he left a footpri n t showing ?that he was here [on] the moon? (swo26 b p o s 
23). These are examp l e s of Shen? s cultur a l science literacy , wher e scie n c e achi e veme n t s 
are appreci a t e d (Shen, 1975). She also wrote (w ro n g l y ) that it takes mont h s to get to and 
from the Moon. She was able to br iefly descri b e a number of f eatur e s of the planet s : Mars 
takes 88 days to comp le t e [orbi t ] the sun, Saturn has rings, Pluto is the coldest and 
195 
 
Mercury the hottest planet , Jupite r is dange ro u s as ?when you put your foot on the ground 
you die becau s e of the atmos p h e r e ? (swo2 6 p o s p m m 07), and that there are more than 9 
planets. She also noted that the Sun has bl ack spots. It is lik ely, given Nonkululeko?s 
appa r e nt limi t e d inte r es t in astro no my that she learnt all these facts from the visit to 
HartRAO rather than fr om any other source s .  
Nonkul u l e ko is a good examp l e of a studen t who does not appea r to be acade m i c a l l y 
bright, althoug h I was unable to obtain her school result s. Her prior knowledg e and 
unders t a n d i n g of Big Ideas in basic astro n o my c oncep t s such as the sun, stars and gravity 
was very limite d , and appear e d to show very lit tle or no change as a result of the visit. For 
such a studen t , a visit to a scien c e centr e usi ng a tradi t i o na l pre-t e s t : p o s t - t e s t asses s me n t of 
learni n g would demon s t r a t e that the visit was not  ?succe s s f u l ? , as the studen t would appear 
not to have learnt anythi n g . Howeve r , what sh e did learn were the additi o n a l facts I have 
describ e d . For Nonkulu l e k o and other studen t s who showed limited ?forma l ? learnin g , the 
princi p a l (if not exclus i v e ) ty pe of learn i n g is the incre me n t a l additi o n of relativ e l y minor 
facts. Nonkululeko?s inabil i t y to link thes e with her existing knowledge structures (and 
show differ e n t i a t i o n or emerge n c e ) is likely to be mainly becaus e her prior knowle d g e of 
the topi c is very limit e d . A chall e n ge for bot h teache r s and scienc e centre educat or s is how 
to engage such a stude n t more effec t i ve l y during a school visit, pr obably by providing 
some pre-v i s i t activi t i e s that will engag e thei r inter e s t , or by ensur i n g that the prese nt a t i o ns 
at the centr e (situa t i o n a l inter e s t ) are suffi c i e n t l y stimu l a t i n g . 
7.5.2  Portrait of Botho (tsw04) 
Table 7.9 Position of Botho on Big Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0    
C 2.1-2.5     
B 1.6-2.0  Botho   
A 1.0-1.5    
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
Table 7.10 Botho?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Grav ity Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Parab o lic/ 
Satellite Dish 
Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
1 2 2 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1.6 1.9 
Botho represents category BB, which comp rises 2 students (Botho and Caroline) who 
visit e d the scien c e centr e with below - a v e r a g e knowledge of Big Id eas, and demonstrated 
196 
 
little change in their knowle d g e af ter the visit. Both student s in this category showed a very 
simil a r profi l e : their knowl e d g e level prior to the visit wa s a mea n of 1.6, and this 
increase d to 1.9 after the visit. They differe d howeve r in their use of astrono my - r e l a t e d 
vocabulary in their Personal Meaning Maps. While Botho increased hers from 11 to 20 
words, Carolin e only increas e d from 19 to 20 words. 
Botho is a 13-year- o l d grade 7 girl student  at Bokamos o School . Like most of the 
studen t s in this townsh i p Botho speaks SeTs wana at home, and no  other languages. 
SeTswana is spoken by just over 8% of th e South Africa n popula t i o n (Stati s t i c s South 
Africa, 2001). The school is relativ e l y close to  her home and she walks there every day. 
Botho drew her pre-visi t PMM on 7 Nove mber , and was interviewed on the same day, 10 
days before visiti n g Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astron o my Observ a t o r y on 17 Nove mbe r . She 
added to her PMM on 3 Decembe r , some 16 da ys after the visit. Unfort u n a t el y I was 
unable to obtain Botho? s acade mi c resul t s , but later found that she was awarded 3 rd  prize in 
the 2003 Southern Skies Challe n g e in the ?reme mbe r i n g ? essa y category. This involved 
writin g a story about the night sky which ha d origi n a t e d as a tradit i o n a l story in her 
community. 
Like the 3 other students at Bokamoso wh om I inter v i e w e d, Both o was a me mbe r 
of the science club at the school, and the vis it to HartRAO was arranged by the teacher in 
charge of the club. The visit wa s open to all members of the club; whoever was able to pay 
for the visit was allowe d to go. Botho knew the purpose of the visit, th at it was ?to learn 
more about astro n o mi e s and astro n o mi e s are pa rt of the galaxie s , stars and planets ? 
(tsw04 p r e i n t 017). As a science club, the student s had been preparin g for the visit; after 
school on Monday and Wednesd a y s . Botho he rself had ?been preparing more about 
planet s ? (tsw04 p r e i n t 025). This  was in contrast to almost all other students in the study, 
who had made no preparation at all. Botho said she was looking fo rward to the visit 
becaus e she wanted ?to learn more about  the Astronomy? (tsw04preint 045), but her 
manner did not sugges t much excite me n t rega rdi n g the trip. Botho professed to liking 
school , princi p a l l y for reason s of learni n g : ?I think that school is import a n t becaus e 
educat i o n is a key to succes s so withou t e ducat i o n there? s no life? (tsw04 p r e i n t 237). Her 
favourite subjects were scienc e and mathe ma t i c s and her re ason for liking them was that 
she wanted to become a doctor. Her least favour i t e subjec t was histor y ?becau s e I don?t 
like past things ? (tsw04 p r e i n t 257). My impre ssi o n of Botho was that  she felt school was 
impor t a n t as a means to furth e r i n g her futur e . She joine d the scien c e club becau s e she liked 
197 
 
science and wanted to know more  about it, and in her spare time  she said she studied. I got 
the impres si o n that she wanted to please me as the interv i e w e r , by appear i n g to be 
studiou s . She said that in her spare time she plays , watch e s telev i s i on (she likes cartoons) 
and reads novels (she wasn?t specif i c about a title, despit e my probing) . Another indicati o n 
of pleasin g an ?author i t y figure ? was her co mment about her father  and books: ?My father 
likes to go to the libra r y and when he comes from the library he gi ve me some books to 
read so I read? (tsw04pr e i nt 293). Although she wasn?t que stio n e d specifi c a l l y about 
aliens , Botho thoug h t scien t i s t s had found lif e ?at space? altho u g h she didn?t think it had 
been found elsewhe r e in the univers e . She pr ofess e d to being religi o u s , and, like most 
studen t s , believ e d that God create d the planet s , space and the univer s e . 
 
Figure 7.5 Botho?s pre-visit PMM 
B o t h o used only 11 astron o my- r e l a t e d words in her pre-vi s i t PMM, but increa s e d this by 9 
words after the visit . These numbe r s were  below-a v e r a g e for her school (18 and 11 
respectively) and for the study pr ior to the visit (20), but ju st above the study?s post-visit 
average (7). Botho?s pre-vis i t PMM (Figure 7.5  and Appendix J) was unusual in that she 
wrote substan t i a l sentenc e s about relativ e l y few ideas, and was one of only 3 students 
198 
 
(Nonkulul e k o was another) who referred to God in writin g. In her PMM Botho wrote 
about space as a place where there are plan et s and moons, and that if you go there you 
need clothe s to keep you war m and give you oxygen. She referre d to there being ?too 
much? gravit y there and people ?just move lik e they are flying ? (t sw04apre 25). She also 
discus s e d stars as small bright things that can  be seen using a telesc op e that were created 
by God to help ?the angels to go and see Je sus when he was born? (tsw04a p r e 21). She 
also wrote about the Sun as a st ar and its visibility, as well as the 9 planets, although the 
only one she named was Earth. 
Botho showed varying levels of knowledge  regar d i n g Big Ideas in astro n o my prior 
to her visit to HartRAO . Her knowled g e of gr avit y was minima l : she was able to say that 
gravity causes a pen to fall down towards th e ground, but had no idea about gravity on the 
Moon, Jupiter or the Sun. Her PM M indicat e d that she associat e d gravity with people 
appearin g to fly, whi ch suggests she had seen ima ges of pe ople encounte r i n g 
weightle s s n e s s . Her knowledg e of stars indicat e d no substan t i a l scient i f i c  understanding 
(ligh t s at night) , and a refere n c e to their being ma de by God to guide angel s to Jesus? birth . 
However, Botho did know that the Sun is a star  (and therefore presumab ly vice versa), that 
their apparent size is related to distance and that stars are about th e size of the Sun. For 
similar reasons, her knowledge of  the Sun was classi fi e d at le vel 2, and, with  the help of 
the model, she could explai n that day a nd night are caused by the Earth spinning. 
Regar d i n g her idea of size and scale in  the solar system, Bot ho showed some 
unders t a n d i n g that the appare n t size of stars is due to thei r distance, but when probed on 
this she couldn ? t explai n why. She also knew that  the Sun is larger than the Moon, and that 
the latter is closer although her explanation was quite interesting, as she invoked the idea 
that altho u g h we somet i me s see a half moon, we  never see a half sun. On these bases she 
was classified at level 2 for her knowledge of size and scale. Like a bout half the studen t s , 
when asked about the Moon, Botho could descri b e its phases but she didn?t know why they 
occur; she suggest e d the weathe r as a possible explanat i o n . Bot ho?s knowledge of satellites 
was minima l : she didn?t know what a satell i t e was, but could relate to a satell i t e dish. 
When questioned about a dish she knew it was relate d to televi s i o n but didn?t know the 
reason for its dish shape, in what direc ti o n it faced, or where it was pointin g to. 
After the visit Botho added severa l fact s to her PMM (Figure 7.6 and Appendix J), 
partic u l a r l y about stars. She st ated that stars are made of a gas called hydrog e n , and that 
you cannot use a telesco p e to l ook at stars on a cloudy day. She noted that orange stars ?is 
199 
 
gonna die soon? and that stars ?a re there during the mornin g ? , but  we can?t see them due to 
the bright n e s s of the Sun (tsw04 b p o s 10). She al so noted that stars make sounds , and when 
questioned about this she could describe th e sounds made by differen t types of stars: 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  And you see they make sound s .  How did you ...? 
Botho :   We have it at the other pl ace and they said it depen d s on how big 
is the star and if it is big it make like booo, booo, booo . 
Inte r v i e w e r : You heard the sound s ? 
Botho :   Ja. 
Inter v i e w e r : How did they , they recor d e d the sound s , what did they use to 
liste n ? 
Botho :   I think they used the satel l i t e . 
Intervie w e r : They used the satellit e .  You mean that big satel l i t e dish. 
Botho :   Ja. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  And you say you heard the big ones make that sound , what 
about a small one, did you hear any other s ? 
Both o :   Ja we heard and this make like ting , ting. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Did you know stars made sound s ? 
Both o :   No. 
(tsw0 4 p o s p m m 49-7 1 ) 
 
Figure 7.6 Botho?s post-visit PMM 
200 
 
Botho also made brief co mments about foot pri n t s on the Moon, spacecr a f t splashi n g down 
in water on retur n from space and three plane t s she had not referred to in the pre-visi t 
PMM: Pluto, Saturn and Mars. Whe n discussi n g  the latt er plan e t , she refer r e d to the ?War 
of the Worlds? slide show de monst r a t e d at HartRAO . Unlike her PMM, in her post-vi s i t 
struct u r e d inter vi e w , Botho changed few of her ideas.  
? S h e stated that there is a little gravit y  on the Moon (whereas previously she had 
not known this) but did not know about Jupite r or the Sun.  
? S h e noted that the Sun can be used as a sun dial to tell the time. 
? S h e thoug h t that the Moon chang e s shape ?becaus e of the seasons ? . 
? S h e made no change s to her ideas of star s (excep t those I have descri b e d from 
the PMM), size and scale or the satelli t e dish. 
Botho is a good example of a student who, althoug h she appeare d not to learn any 
addition a l knowledg e about Big Ideas, demons trated that she did learn a limited amount 
from the visit. This knowle d g e incr e a s e is mainly evide n t in her post-v i si t PMM and 
associ a t e d intervi e w . The facts she reme mbe r e d from the visit are highly persona l and 
relat e directl y to several of the exhi b i t s and demon s t r a t i o ns at Hart R A O . While Botho 
improv e d her basic scienc e liter a c y in the form of these new facts, she did not show any 
evidenc e of change in her cultur al science literacy (Shen, 1975). 
Using the human constructivist categorie s of knowledge constr uction, Table 7.11 
summar i s e s how the change s in Botho? s knowle dge occur r e d . From a cogni t i ve viewpoi n t , 
she demon s t r at e d severa l examp l e s of addit i on , wher e she incre me n t a l l y adde d smal l 
pieces of knowle d g e to her pr e-exi s t i n g knowle d g e . In most cases these additional facts 
were across a variety of concept s from grav ity to the Sun to the Moon landings. However, 
there was one area which appear s to have captu re d her attenti o n more than others: various 
facts about stars . Where a s I cat ego r i s e d Botho at knowle d g e leve l 2 for her concep t of stars 
prior to the visit , I consi d e r e d that the addi ti o n a l informa t i o n she provid e d in her post-v i s it 
PMM and interview enabled her to be placed at  level 3. For the same reason, I suggest that 
her concept of star was considerably extended as a result of the visit, and was actuall y 
differ e n t i a t e d , her only exampl e of cognit i ve le arni n g which was not cl assif i e d as additio n . 
Unlike mos t other student s except maybe Brenda, Botho showed only 3 examples of 
affectiv e learning after the visi t: surprise at the fact that stars die and the experienc e she 
told her parents about afterw a r d s : the whis p e r i n g dishe s . Simil a r l y , the visit did not appear 
201 
 
to have motiva t e d her much into findin g out more about astrono my (the conativ e aspect) . 
Howeve r , her father , who she mentio n e d prio r to her visit as bor rowing books from the 
library for her, brought her a book about astrono my and she said she read about stars from 
it. It is ther efo r e possi ble that in the fortn i ght between the visit and my interviewing her, 
Botho had gained some of the informa t i o n about  stars that she wrote in her post-vis i t PMM 
from the book that her fathe r got her from the librar y . This is itsel f of inter e s t , as it 
demons t r a t e s that a studen t who repres e n t s thos e who appear e d to gain  very litt l e from the 
visit did in fact acquire some  knowledg e about a specific concept which interest e d her: 
stars. A further conati v e aspect which did not emerg e from the inter v i ew data was the fact 
that Botho enter e d the Southe r n Skies essa y compe t i t i o n . This compet i t i o n was held in 
2003 as part of World Space Week to encour a ge peopl e betwe e n ages 5 and 23 years to 
partici p a t e in astrono m y . Botho won 3 rd  prize for the categor y which involve d telling a 
story about the night sky and stars which sh e had heard in her commun i t y , which furthe r 
rein f o r c es her inter es t in stars . 
Table 7.11 Frequency of Human Co nstructivism codes for Botho 
Knowledge 
construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview 
PostPMM & 
Interview 
Pre 
Interview 
Post 
Interview 
Totals 
A dd itio n n/a 8 n/a 5 13 
Emerg e n c e n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Diff er en tiatio n n/a 1 n/a 0 1 
Disc r imin a tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Reco n tex tu a lisa tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Affe c tiv e 0 0 2 3 5 
Cona tiv e 0 0 0 1 1 
Totals 0 9 2 9 20 
While Botho? s concep t u a l change in Big Ideas was limited , she mana ged to extend her 
knowledge of stars, and the vi sit clearly motivated her inte rest in the topic. Like 
Nonkululeko, her knowledge gain appears to  be mainly by additi o n , althou g h in her 
understanding of stars she show ed evidence of differentiation. From the affective and 
conativ e perspe c t i v e s , Botho shows slight l y mo re interes t in astrono my than Nonkulu l e k o , 
and her reading about stars and enterin g the competi t i o n shows th at her inter e s t in the topic 
was likely kindle d by the visit to HartRA O . Botho is an exampl e of a studen t whose 
affectiv e and conative experien c e s durin g the visit were proba b l y more impor t a n t than the 
scienc e learni n g about Big Ideas. Her intere s t in stars mo tivat e d her to enter and win a 
compe t i t i o n prize (which she did not discu s s with me in her inter v i ew ) . This is also an 
202 
 
example of the sort of post-vis i t learning wh ich is not fully captured in a study such as 
mine, which concentrated on learning during th e visit. Falk and Dier king (2000) note that 
if studen t s are remi nd e d of the visit during the subseq u e n t months afte r their return , this 
will furth er reinfor c e their learni ng , and it will become establ i s h e d in their long- t e r m 
me mory. Botho would therefor e have b een a good candida t e for later follow- u p . 
7.5.3  Portrait of Neo (swo42) 
Table 7.12 Position of Neo on Bi g Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0    
C 2.1-2.5   Neo  
B 1.6-2.0    
A 1.0-1.5    
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
Table 7.13 Neo?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Grav ity Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Parab o lic/ 
Satellite Dish 
Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
1 1 2 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 2.5 n/a 1.5 2 2.1 2.3 
Neo was chosen as an example of a student in the CC categ or y . Accor di ng to my analy s i s 
of her PMM and interv i e w , she went on th e trip to Hartebe e s t h o e k Radio Astron o my 
Observatory with above-average knowledge of  Big Ideas, and, like both Nonkululeko and 
Botho gained relat i v e l y littl e from the trip (her mean changed from 2.1 to 2.3). She is 
represe n t a t i v e of the five st uden t s in the CC catego r y , alt hou g h her post-vi s i t knowle d g e is 
slightl y lowe r than the other CC studen t s , whose post visit mean score was 2.4. 
Neo is a 13-year - o l d grade 7 girl student and has attended Balf our Forest School, a 
public prima r y schoo l in the northe r n subur b s of  Johann e s b u r g since grade 1. Like many of 
the student s at this school , Neo lives in a town ship in the northern part of Johanne s b u r g , 
and trave l s to the schoo l each day by minib u s ta xi. At home she speak s SePed i , a langu a g e 
spoken by 50% of the inhabi t a n t s of Limpop o Pr ovi n c e , and the mothe r tongu e of 9.4% of 
the South Africa n popula t i o n (Stati s t i c s Sout h Afric a , 2001) and some Engli s h . Neo drew 
her PMM on 8 th  October and was interviewed on 23 rd  Octobe r , the day before she visite d 
Hartebe e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Observatory together with the classes who attended. On 
30 th  Octobe r , all the studen t s who had been on the fiel d trip did th eir repea t PMM during 
school time and I re-inte r v i e w e d Neo on the same day.  
203 
 
Neo state d that she was inter e s t e d in space , and refer r e d to its appea l seve r a l time s 
durin g the inter vi e w . Her main inter e s t was in observi n g planet s and the fact that they have 
no life on them. She remembe r e d that one of the plane t s , eithe r Mars or Mercu r y had been 
in the news recent l y on TV (it was Mars). Neo enjoyed school, and regarde d it as being 
import a n t for going on to high school and beyond. Her favouri t e subjec t s were Science and 
Techno l o g y , and she relate d them to Mark Shu ttl e w o r t h and the fact that she too would 
like to go to space . Her least favou r i t e subj ec t s were Economi c and Manage me n t Scienc e 
and Human and Social Science: ?I think that I don?t like talk ing about the past because, 
what?s past is past.  We have to look at  the future?.? (swo42prei n t 231). Neo?s career 
plans are to be a scient i st , and quite ambit i o u s l y to ?be the first Scient i s t to find anythi n g 
that any Scientis t s haven?t found out of sp ace? (swo42po s i n t 185). Her main recreati o n is 
gymnas t i c s , and she likes to watch sport on te levi s i o n . She doesn? t ap pear to read much, 
and only referr e d to a book she read last ye ar, which covere d the sun and moon. Neo 
knows that extra- t e r r e s t r i a l life has never been  found, and thinks it unlikel y that it exists 
anywhe r e else in the univers e ; she does not believe in aliens . She reads the horoscope in 
magazin e s , and althoug h she doesn?t know ho w astrologers predict the future, she does 
belie ve that their predi ct i o n s are somet i me s ri ght . She consi d e r s hers el f as being religi o u s , 
and believes God?s relationship to the univer s e is such that: ?I think that God made space 
and all ?  Just for people to go and explore what?s happening in the ?  In the Earth and 
on space.  What?s going on? (swo42 p r e i nt 341). In  view of her own inter e s t in the topic , 
this suggest s a view of a created univers e for the purpose of explora t i o n by humans. 
In her use of astrono my words and terms Neo?s knowled g e of astrono m y , while not 
being very extensive, was fa irly sound. In her pre-visit PMM she used only 7 astronomy-
 r e l at e d words, which was consid er a bl y below bo th the avera ge for the study (20 words ) as 
well as her schoo l (16 words ) . Howev e r , her post-vi s i t vocabu l a r y increa s e d by 11 words, 
approx i ma t e l y twice the schoo l and study averag e . All the vocabul a r y she used was 
common to other studen t s in th e study, and Neo showed the cap acit y to express hersel f and 
her knowledg e of concepts in astronom y .  
As shown by the blue writing , Neo?s init ial personal meaning map (Figure 7.7 and 
Appendix J) was relatively lim ited , but she did describ e aspect s of each of the principal 
words space , stars and planet s . For exampl e , she descri b e d space as a place ?full of 
nothing ? , with ?no air or oxygen to breathe ? , ?no gravit y to pull th ings down? and where 
no one can live (swo42p r e 9, 16). Her descrip tion of the planets and stars was less 
204 
 
extens i v e , but includ e d the Earth rotati n g on it s axis (?the plant Earth roats on it aexs? ) to 
cause day and night (swo42pre 13 ), as well as the sun bei ng a very big star. In her 
interview based on her PMM, Ne o expanded br iefly on two of the planets: Pluto and 
Mercur y , but otherw i s e added li ttle to what she had drawn. 
 
Figure 7.7 Neo?s PMM (pre and post) 
D u r i n g the struct u r e d interv i e w relate d to Big Ideas, Neo demons t r a t e d her unders t a n d i n g 
of sever a l conce p t s in basic astro n o my . Although she showed an initial teleolo g i c a l 
explan a t i o n for the sun moving across the sky every day (?It mo ves across the sky 
everyday so that we can know if it?s day or  if it?s night? ) , sh e did provid e a clear 
explana t i o n of the Earth?s rotati o n as being the cause of the apparen t moveme n t 
(swo42pr e i n t 071), and was classi f i e d at level 3. Unlike most students in the study, she 
could also explai n that the moon?s shape cha nge over the course of the month was caused 
by the sun shinin g on the moon (level 3):  
Neo:  It? s becaus e of the sun.  If may b e the sun shines a little bit half or 
quart e r of the moon.  And that the moon is alway s full, it never 
cuts in half or anyth i n g .  It?s alway s full.  It?s only the sun, it 
depen d s on how the sun refle c t s on the moon. (swo4 2 p r e i n t 095) 
205 
 
Only one third of the stude n t s were able to ac count for the appar e n t chang e in shape of the 
moon in their pre-vi s i t interv i e w . Furthe r , Ne o descri b e d how the sun was larger than the 
moon, but that it is further away from the Eart h and hence looks a similar size in the sky.  
However, Neo?s knowled g e showed some clear limitat i o n s , for exampl e althoug h 
she knew that the sun is a star, she regar de d it as being ?bigg e r ? than  other stars, and did 
not elabor a t e on it furthe r . I classi f i e d this kno wle d g e on sun-st a r differ e n c e s as being at a 
low level . She had very littl e idea of tempe r a t u r e , eithe r room temp e r a t ur e or boili ng 
point s , and appea r e d to be guess i n g these and th e fact that the surfac e of the sun mi ght be 
360?C, and was classi f i e d for the Sun concep t as  level 2. Furthe r , Neo appear e d to have a 
fairly limite d concep t of what a star is: she knew a star is like  a sun, and that it is further 
away in space, but consid e r e d that the sun is one of the bigges t stars,  and think s that the 
sun can reflec t on the stars (level 2). Her knowle d g e of size and scale was her weakes t 
area, as was classifi e d as bei ng at level 1. She unders t o o d that a satelli t e is some sort of 
object outsid e the Earth, but c ould not explain it further . She also thoug h t that a satel l i t e 
dish would norma l l y point to the sun, becaus e ?m ost of the things need  the sun to work.  
So, withou t it, maybe, if it can point to space,  becaus e space is blank, I think there would 
be nothin g comi ng in or out? (s wo42preint 171) (level 2.5). 
Like most studen t s , Neo could provid e at  least a limite d defini t i o n of gravit y , as 
some sort of force that pu lls things down. However, be yond that idea her pre-visit 
inter v i e w shows minima l elabo r at i on of the conce p t : she belie v e d (like over 80% of the 
student s ) that there is no gravity either in space, on the moon, on Jupiter or on the sun 
(leve l 1). So we can see that prior to he r visit to HartRAO, Neo bot h possesse d some 
scienti f i c knowled g e of astr on o my concep t s (such as the m oon phases) and also a number 
of either limit e d conce p t i o n s or miscon c e p t i o ns (such as her understanding of stars and 
gravit y ) .  
After her visit to HartRAO, Neo added se veral sections to her PMM (the grey 
pencil writing in Figure 7.7 a nd Appendix J). First, it appear s that she now knew that there 
are such things as sunspots, and that although  they look small on the sun, they are massiv e 
in size. This knowledge , which I identifi e d as  the HC categor y of additio n , would have 
been a direct resul t of the visit to Hart RAO , as a sunspot was demonst r a t e d by the 
educ a t or . It is like l y that th e demons t r a t i o n made an impres s i o n on her, as she descri b e d 
the sunspot (or suns point as she initial l y ca lled it) in her post-v i s i t PMM as follow s : 
206 
 
Neo:  The sun has a dot in the mi ddl e of it, the dot is very small seein g 
it from the eath but its biger than the size of our Earth , and that 
dot ordat s [? ] are the suns point 
(swo4 2 p o s 20) 
However , her descrip t i o n of the sunspot dem ons t r a t e s her limited unders t a n d i n g of what it 
actual l y is. When quest i o n e d in the inter v i e w , she indica t e d that the dot is always there, 
and is the middle of the sun: 
Neo:  I think that is the middl e of the sun and it?s a bit cold in that sun 
spot and the sun spot is more bigge r than our Earth , may b e abou t 
100 Earth s can fit into that dot. 
(swo4 2 p o s pm m 11) 
It is impor t a n t to note that Neo acqui r e d a new conce p t as a result of the visit to HartR A O : 
the sunspot . In terms of hum an const r u ct i vi s t theor y , Neo added or subsumed the concept 
of sunspo t , and I regard this as an increm e n t a l increas e in her knowled g e of the Sun. She 
now had an additio n a l fact at her disposal regarding the Sun:  it has a sunspot. However, 
she also appear e d to understand the concept of scale with  respect to the sun, a sunspot and 
the Earth , in that she could relat e the size of th e sunspot on the sun to th e fact that it is so 
massiv e that 100 Earths can fit in to it. I conside r this to be  an example of differe n t i a t i o n , 
where her concep t of the sun has been extend e d to resul t in great e r understanding of scale 
with respect to the Sun, the Earth and sunspots. Ho wever, her idea of a s unspo t is that it is 
the centre of the sun, where it is cooler. This misconception she has acquired was probably 
the resul t of the fact that the part i c ul a r suns pot visible on that day happened to lie in the 
middle of the sun?s disc. 
After the visit , Neo made some refer e n c e to the Moon, which she did not do 
directly in the pre-vis it PMM. I regard her referen c e to a number of fact s about the Moon 
as being example s of human constru c t i v i s t addition, as follows, using her words: 
Neo: The moon has no oxyge n but it has abou t 5 to 6 gravi t y . Righ t 
know if we were to go to the [M oon ] we would stil l find the foot 
print of the first man who went to the moon becau s e the is no 
wind to blow it off. The [Moon ] has cr at e r s on it becau s e 
scienc e t i st say that the craters are being formed by the co mets 
which crash the moon. 
(swo4 2 p o s 15) 
She alrea d y had refer r e d to the fact that th ere is no oxygen in space in the pre-vi s i t PMM, 
and here she extends this idea to the Moon. Additi o na l facts which she has acqui re d are 
regardi n g gravity , the idea of the footp r i nt of the first man to  set foot ther e ,  and the reason 
207 
 
for craters on its surface. This descript i o n was a direct result of  the moon landing slide 
show presented to Neo and her classmates, in which the footp r i nt s we re stressed, and Neo 
has added to her knowled g e by re lating these facts on her PMM. 
The last way in which Neo added to he r PMM was to note some additional facts 
about the solar system, the Milky Way, comets and asteroids. In her interview she showed 
some concep t of the size of the Milky Way: 
Interv i e w e r : How many stars do you think there are in the Milky Way? 
Neo:  May b e about a billio n becau s e th ere are a lot of stars and some of 
the stars born some other stars so it grows every time. 
(swo4 2 p o s pm m 13-15 ) 
She was also able to explain what she thought  was the differe n c e betwee n come ts and 
aste r oi d s: 
Neo:  Co met s are li ke rocks .  I think that it?s somet h i n g maybe like 
when there was suppo s e d to be anoth e r plane t forme d , but it 
wasn? t forme d so it create a co met.  An d then asteroi d s it? s like 
so me sort of like a fla me or someth i n g .  And so somet i me s it just 
goes into flash and leaves some tail or some kind of colour 
espec i a l l y in the night. 
(swo4 2 p o s pm m 19) 
Her refer e nc e to the origi n of comet s indic at es that she has confl a t e d the conce pt s of 
comet , asteroi d and meteo r into one idea, and descr i be d a meteo r trail in the sky, even 
though she stated she has never actually seen one. 
During the struct u r e d inter v i e w , Neo furthe r demons t r a t e d that her visit to the radio 
teles c o p e did resul t in some limit e d chang e s to her conceptions, and she acquired some 
new knowledge she did not apparently previo usly possess. Further knowledge that Neo 
acquired with respect to sun wa s that the tempe r a t u r e on the sun?s surfa c e is five billio n 
degr e e s . Wh il e this is inac c u r at e (the core is 15.5 milli o n degre e s , but the surfa c e is only 
5800 degree s ) , her idea of the te mper a t u r e has m oved closer to the actual temper a t u r e tha n 
her previous idea, which was only 360 degrees. However, from her pre-visi t and post-vis i t 
respon s e s to questio n s , it appe ars she does not really have a scienti f i c concept i o n of 
tempe r a t u r e , as she refers to water boili n g at ?30 or minus 30? (swo4 2p o s i n t 037). In her 
post-v i s i t interv i e w , Neo also added the fact th at in the futur e the sun will expan d , contr a ct 
and ?there will be no sun anymor e ? (swo42 p o si n t 153). This is a direct recall of discus s i o n 
by the HartRAO educat o r s of what will happen to  the Sun in the futur e . I regar d all these 
ideas as being example s of additio n of concept s . 
208 
 
Neo?s concep t i o n of stars also differ e d after the visit to HartR A O . Prior to the visit , 
she unders t o o d the concep t that stars appear so tiny becaus e of their distanc e from the 
Earth (although this was not probe d in detail), but she believed  that most stars are smaller 
than the sun: ?Stars are ? Maybe a little bi t ? A little bit smaller than the sun, because 
the sun is one of the bigges t stars. ? (sw o42p r e i n t 131) However , after the visit she 
appear e d to unders t a n d that star size varies: ?Stars are in diffe r e n t sizes , some of them are 
small , some of them are big, some of them are med i u m and some of  them are bigger than 
the sun? (swo42 p o s i n t 077). She also reme mbe r e d  from the visit that stars are spinning , a 
fact she is unlikely to have encounte r e d pr eviously. Like the facts about the Sun, her 
increase in knowledg e here is incremen t a l , and appears to be occurring by addition. 
Neo?s underst a n d i n g of gravity showed little change across the vi sit to HartRAO as 
she still belie v e d that there is no gravi t y in space, or on Jupiter or the sun. She did however 
change her idea of gravity on the Moon. Whereas previously she believed that there is no 
gravity on the Moon, and that people  float there, after th e visit she stated ?t here ? s a little bit 
on the moon about five to six of gravi t y ?  (swo42 p o s i n t 109). At some point, probab l y 
during the moon talk during the visit, she heard that gravit y on the moon is one sixth of 
that on the Earth. She would al so have seen slides of peop le walking on the moon, and not 
floatin g . I regard this knowled g e acquisi t i o n as a further example of addition: she has 
acquired a new fact about gr avity, but her overall underst anding of gravity has not 
increa s e d . Her idea of gravi t y was diffi c u l t to establi s h , but showed a private theory related 
some ho w to heat, as when aske d about gravit y on th e sun, she state d ?warm air rises up? 
(swo42preint 195). Although aspects of her understanding of gr avity with respect to the 
Moon appeare d to change slightl y  as a result of the visit to HartRAO, he r overall theory of 
gravit y did not change . This is exempl i f i e d by her post-vis i t response to whether there is 
gravity on the sun:  
Neo: I think there is a bit of, no there is no gravity on the sun because 
usuall y war m air rises up, but if there is so me gravit y there is 
little becaus e war m air rises up so th at? s why there is no gravit y . 
(swo 4 2 p o s i n t 121) 
In HC theory David Anderso n refers to this type of knowl e dge construction as 
recontextualisation , whereby ?a previous l y identifi e d concep t ? is modifie d ?witho u t 
signif i c a nt clarif i c a t i o n of meaning ? due to the change d context (Anderso n et al. 2003 p 
193). In this examp l e from Neo, she strug g l e s to explain why there is no gravity on the Sun 
in terms of her own ?theory? of hot air rising, which she expres sed in her pre-visit 
209 
 
intervi e w . Essent i a l l y she has altered her idea s a little, but her underst a n d i n g of the concept 
of gravity has not been clarifi e d . 
Neo appear e d to show si mila r recont e x t u a l i s at i o n in her conce p t of a satel l i t e . Prior 
to the visit , she refer r e d to a satell i t e as being out in space. After the visit, she concent r a t e d 
her explana t i o n on the satelli t e dish, and struggl e d to explain what a radio telesc o p e does, 
as follo w s : 
Neo:  The word satel l i t e it mean s that, all I know is satel l i t e , but I don?t 
know the meani n g .  Satell it e we have satell i t e dishe s like here in 
South Afric a we have a sat ell i t e dish and it?s the only one in 
Africa in South Afric a so we get inform a t i o n so some of the, what 
they usual ly use it for it?s like may b e they turn it on and then 
may b e it search for the star and may b e that star the astron o my 
will study that star and tell us more about it, may b e that star is a 
plane t or somet h i n g . 
(swo 4 2 p o s i n t 093) 
I regar d e d this as recon t e x t u al i sa t i o n as Neo was able to modi fy her explanation of a 
satel l i t e dish in the context of the visit to Ha rtRA O . She had now seen a dish used in a new 
conte x t , and had some idea of what the astro n ome r s were using it for. She was howev e r , 
but was unable to further cl arify her understanding of sate llite. When asked about the 
shape of the dish, Neo was able to explain how the shape enables sounds to be captured. 
Neo:  It?s in that dish - s h a p e so that all the infor ma t i o n and all that 
co mes in can get in becaus e if it? s straig h t like that nothin g will 
co me in, all the sounds and every th i n g that they hear from space 
will just float aroun d so that dish  cause s the infor ma t i o n to come 
in and not to float arou n d . 
(swo 4 2 p o s i n t 097) 
I regard e d this explan a t i o n as an exampl e of  differe n t i a t i o n in which Neo was now able to 
explain how the shape of the dish enabled it to capture ?informa t i o n ? and ?sounds? . Her 
experi e n c e of using the whispe r dishes and the HartRAO educator ? s explanat i o n of how 
they work may have contribu t e d to her explan a t i o n , but she did not re fer to these dishes 
expli ci t l y in her accou nt . 
Neo showed relative l y few instance s of  non-cog n i t i ve knowle d g e constr u c t i o n as a 
resul t of her visit to Hart R A O . In the affec t i v e doma i n , prior to her visit, she stated that 
Natura l Scienc e and Techno l o g y were her favour i t e subject s at school, as one day she 
?want[s ] to go to space and be  like Mark Shuttl e w o r t h ? (swo4 2 p r e i nt 223). She also saw 
the trip as being persona l l y re levan t (germa n e ) , in that ?I  want to know more about our 
world and what?s happenin g outside of our world? (swo42pr e i n t 027). Her particul a r 
210 
 
intere s t in the topic of astron o my was ?The pl anet s .  What?s happen i n g on other planet s , 
becaus e there is no life on it? (swo42 p r e i nt 289). After the visit ?I told my mother that we 
had a lot of fun and we learnt  a lot? (swo42p o s i n t 169) and sh e relate d the use of rocket s 
and the exper i e nc e of star rotat i o n to her mother. Neo showed no examples of conative 
learni n g after her visit. 
Neo went on the visit to HartRAO with above-a v e r a g e prior knowle d g e in Big 
Ideas. Her pre-vi s i t vocab u l a r y and the id eas she de mo nst r a t e d on her PMM were 
relativ e l y restri c t e d , bu t her underst a n d i n g of th e cause of day and night and the phases of 
the moon were fairly sound. She was also able to provide a basic defi nition of gravity, and 
had a limited but satisfa c t o r y concept i o n of what  a star is. After the visit Neo?s ideas about 
the sun, mo on stars, aster o i d s and comets ha d all changed and been elabora t e d on. Like 
Botho, Neo improve d her basic scienc e litera c y , but did not demons t r a t e any change in her 
cultura l scienc e liter acy (Shen, 1975) as a result of the visit. 
Analysin g her learning us ing the human constructivi st categories of knowledge 
constr u c t i o n, Neo?s freque n c i e s for each code are shown in Table 7.14. Like both 
Nonkul u l e k o and Botho, from a Human Cons tructivist perspective Neo experienced 
mainl y as addit i o n , in which she has accumul a t e d addit i o na l small piece s of infor ma t i o n 
which she has fitte d in with her pre- e x i st i n g knowl e d g e . These incl u d e facts about the sun, 
the stars, come ts and asteroi d s. In several cases she has di fferentiated this knowledge to 
clarify her understa n d i n g of c oncep t s in a deeper way. For exampl e her unders t a n d i n g of 
the size of the Sun and sunspo t s in relati o n to  the Earth, her descript i o n of star sizes and 
her explanation of why the Sun and Moon look th e same size in the sky all demon s t r a t e an 
increase d understa n d i n g of size and scale. Also, as a direct result of the visi t, she 
developed a new but flawed unde rs t a n d i n g of sunspo t s . These ex ample s of differe n t i a t i o n 
sugge s t that she is at a differ e n t conce p t u a l level from both Nonkulul e k o and Botho, who 
showed only one example of differe n t i a t i o n betwee n them. 
Table 7.14 Frequency of Human Co nstructivism codes for Neo 
Knowledge 
construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview  
PostPMM & 
Interview  
Pre 
Interview 
Post 
Interview  
Totals 
A dd itio n n/a 7 n/a 7 14 
Emergen c e n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Differen tiatio n n/a 3 n/a 3 6 
Disc r imin a tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Reco n tex tu a lisa tio n n/a 0 n/a 2 2 
211 
 
Knowledge 
construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview  
PostPMM & 
Interview  
Pre 
Interview 
Post 
Interview  
Totals 
A ff e c tiv e 0 0 5 4 9 
Cona tiv e 0 0 1 0 1 
Totals 0 10 6 16 32 
Like John (section 7.5.4) and Helen (sectio n 8.1.3), Neo appears to have had some 
substantial prior experiences on which to base her additional knowledge. Yet she showed 
no evidenc e of her recolle c t i n g previou s l y - k n o w n infor ma t i o n (eme r g e n c e ) as a result of 
the visit . It is possi bl e that my post- vi s i t inter vi ew with her was not suffi c i e nt l y probi n g to 
differe n t i a t e betwee n additi o n and emergen c e .  However , Neo did use her prior knowled g e , 
but in two cases she recon t e x t ua l i se d it as a resul t of her visit exper i e n c e s rathe r than 
expli ci t l y stati n g that the visit remi n d e d he r of things she already knew. How should a 
scien ce centr e try to make Neo?s visit most benefi c i a l to her? While it was succe s sfu l in 
promoti n g her knowled g e constru c t i o n in the fo r m of differ e n t i a t i o n , I sugges t that it 
should explicitly aim to encourage emergent knowledge. Overt cues and remi nders of prior 
knowl e d g e would stimu l a t e a studen t such as Ne o to relate the new knowle d g e she is being 
prese n t e d with to her exist i n g conce p t u a l stru c t u r e s . This in turn is likel y to cause her 
existing knowledg e structur e s to  be further differentiated, a nd so promote more effective 
learni n g . 
7.5.4  Portrait of John (vho 16) 
Table 7.15 Position of John on Big Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0    John 
C 2.1-2.5     
B 1.6-2.0    
A 1.0-1.5    
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
Table 7.16 John?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Grav ity Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Parab o lic/ 
Satellite Dish 
Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 3 3 n/a 3 3 2.8 3.0 
 
John was chosen as an example of a stude nt in the DD category. This means that, 
accor di n g to my analys i s of his PMM and in tervi e w , he went on the trip to HartRA O 
already very knowled g e a b l e a bout Big Ideas in astron o my I am investi g a t i n g , and could 
not progres s any ?highe r ? in my classif i c a t i o n . Howeve r ,  I hope to demonstrate in this 
212 
 
portrait that John did still im prove his astronom y knowledg e , and was able to remember 
previously-learned knowledge as a result of the visit to HartRA O . His pre- and post-v i s i t 
mean scores are very simila r to the othe r 3 category DD students, although his post-visit 
score is higher (their mea n is 2.7). 
John is a white, 15-year - o l d grade 7 student  atten d i n g Achie v e me n t Schoo l , a small 
private school in a Western s uburb of Johannesburg. John live s in the same suburb as the 
school at which he has attended for four year s, and comes from an Englis h - s p e a k i n g home. 
John drew his PMM on 15 th  October and was interviewed the following day, in a spare 
office at the school. He then partici p a t e d  in the class visit to HartRAO on 20 th  Octob e r . I 
visite d the Achie v e me n t Schoo l on 21 st  Octobe r , and the whol e class compl e t e d their 
?repea t PMM? silent l y in th e classr o o m, with me presen t .  I re-inte r v i e w e d John on the 
same day, so the whole process of data collect ion was less than one week in his case. John 
is two to three years olde r than the other stude nt s in the class, and wa s not regarde d as an 
acade mi c achie v e r at schoo l . 
In his pre-vi s i t interv i e w, John was the onl y student who had the idea that the visit 
to one of the study sites would be both educatio n a l and fun. The majority of students 
visiti n g Harteb e e s t h o e k referr e d to the visit as being educ ational (88%, n=34), and only 
refer r e d to fun in the post- v i s i t intervi e w . Af ter the visit, John was also the only studen t 
who referr e d to the ?fun? aspect of the visit withou t being prompt e d . In the case of mos t 
other studen t s , I specif i c a l l y asked them if th ey thought having fun was part of the visit, 
after they had identif i e d the purpos e of the visit as being educa t i o n al . They all answe re d in 
the affirma t i v e , that having fun was one of th e visit? s purpos e s . It appear e d from John?s 
respons e s to questio n s that he was only motiv a t e d to learn things  that to him were 
interes t i n g and worthwh i l e , so from his point of  view the ?fun? aspec t of the trip was very 
impor t a n t . 
John stated he was interes t e d in the t opic of astron o my and space, and found the 
idea of space exploration fa scinating. He was aware that  during August 2003 Mars was 
going to be at its closest to Earth for a long time. He al so was aware that a comet or meteor 
had been close to Earth in the past few years,  and he attrib u t e d the recen t rash of films 
about astero i d impact to that fact. John did not sound very enthusi a s t i c about school , 
disliking Afrikaans mos t , and enjoying sc ience technol o g y and geograp h y (now Human 
and Social Scienc e s ) . As a career , John is  intere s t e d in becomi n g a geolog i s t . For 
recrea t i o n John said he likes to read non-fi c t i o n ,  and he stress e d that while he is happy to 
213 
 
watch ficti o n on telev i s i o n he doesn ? t like it in  book s as he said ?I don? t like imag i n i n g , I?d 
rather see it like on TV and stuff? (vho16p r e i nt 242). John thinks that primit i v e life such as 
micro organ i s ms has be en found on Mars as well as comet s . He belie v e s that extra -
 t e r re s t r i al life is possi bl e, on the basi s that the univ e r s e is so big, but he consi d e r e d that it is 
unlik el y that other life is as intel l i g ent as human s , so the idea of alien s visit i n g the Earth is 
implaus i b l e . John was fairly scathin g about the idea of astrology, hi s comme n t being ?No, 
I don?t like that, I think that?s actual l y r ubbish? (vho16pr e i n t 278). However, he consider e d 
himself religio u s , in that he does pray and believes in God, whom he considers created the 
unive r s e . 
In his use of astron o my words and terms John has both a broad and fairly deep 
knowledge of astronomy as compa red with all other students in my study. The broad 
nature is demonst r a t e d by his knowled g e of astrono my- r e l a t e d vocabu l a r y . On his pre-vis i t 
PMM he used 36 astron o my - r e l a t e d words, which was the sec ond highest of all students in 
the study (the highest being 37 words by a stude nt in the same school) , and substan t i a l l y 
higher than the average for the study (20 wo rds) . While his post-v i s i t vocabu l a r y only 
increas e d by 14 words, his total for both PMMs  was 50, the highest of any student in the 
study. Furthe r , John used seven words no other st udent used, such as dark matter, genesis 
and quasar , as well as six words only one othe r studen t used, such as nebula and neutron 
star. The signifi c a n c e of John?s substa n t i a l l y develope d vocabula r y is  that it provid e s a 
basis for him to articu l a t e his unders t a n d i n g of  concep t s as shown in the interv i e ws , and 
this is discus s e d in greate r detail in Ch apte r 9. John?s depth of knowle d g e was shown by 
his abilit y to discus s comple x aspect s of astron o m y during his interv i e w s . 
214 
 
 
Figure 7.8 John?s pre-visit PMM 
J o h n ? s initia l person a l meanin g map (Figur e 7.8 and Append i x J) was fairly extens i v e , but 
consis t e d mainly of astron o my - r e l a t e d vocabulary  and did not expand much on the words 
he listed. During the interview however, he de mon s t r a t e d knowle d g e of  star forma t i o n in 
nebulas, and explanat i o n s for quasars, black hole s and neutron stars. He  indicated that this 
knowle d g e was gained from readin g his fath er? s Nation a l Geogra p h i c magazi n e , and 
looking up the meaning of quasar in a dictionary. He did a pr oject on black holes at school 
the previous year, and suggested that st ars could possibly form within them. His 
descri p t i o n of star format i on, while it referred to ?gases and the nuclear reactions and 
certai n differ e n t gases reacti n g with each ot her? (vho16prepmm 07), did not relate this to 
any role played by gravity in the process .  When questioned about the planets he had 
writte n down, he listed facts about Jupite r , Saturn, Venus and Mercury . He had a fairly 
detaile d knowled g e of Mars, a nd again referred to National Ge ographic as a sour ce of his 
infor ma t i o n . 
In the remain d e r of his interv i e w his know le d g e of Big Ideas was substa n t i a l when 
compar e d to other studen t s . For exampl e , when question e d on the differen c e between the 
Sun and the stars many students re fe r to visi bl e diffe r e nc e s , such  as size, visibili t y by day 
and night, brightne s s and distance . John however , referr e d to Sun and stars being basica l l y 
215 
 
the same, but he additio n a l l y re ferr e d to nuclear reacti o n s causing them to ?burn? . His 
conception of the Sun was classifi ed as being at level 3, one of only 8 students in the study 
(24%) who were able to describe  the Sun in relat i v e l y sophi s t i c a t e d terms prior to the visit . 
John however was not scien tifically correct in all his pre-visit knowledge, for 
examp l e he (like many other stud en t s ) confus e d the term revolv e with rotate . In his pre-
 visit interview, when asked why the Sun move s across the sky every day, he stated that 
?Becau s e we are revolv i n g around , no we , the Earth revolv e s around the Sun? 
(vho16p r e i n t 082). As soon as he was presented with th e model Earth and Sun, he 
demons t r a t e d correc t l y that the Sun moving acr oss the sky was due to the Earth ?turni n g ? 
(rotat i n g ) , rather than the re volut i o n of the Earth around th e Sun. The introduction of a 
three dime nsio n a l mode l appeared to be the stimulus for John to be able to correct his 
initi a l incor r e c t respo n s e to th e questio n (level 2). Several other stude n t s in the study 
showe d this devel o p me n t in thei r thin k i n g as a resul t of the use of a model in the intervi e w , 
and the issue is discuss e d further in Chapter 9.  
Anothe r mis c on c e p t i o n John de mons t r a t e d was that of the cause of the phases of 
the moon. In his pre-vi s i t inter v iew he referred to the Earth?s shadow as being responsible 
for the change in moon shape over a month, and as  he talke d he also refer r e d to the phase s 
being caused by the Sun shining on the moon as  it orbits Earth. As he spoke during his 
explan a t i o n he decide d that the Earth? s sha dow was only involve d in lunar eclipse s . Like 
his explan a t i o n of the Sun?s move m e n t acr oss the sky, John changed his reason for the 
phenomenon as his account progressed (eve n though there was no model of the Moon 
involved). Unfortunately, I di dn?t question him about the m oon phases in his post-visit 
inter v i e w . 
John explained in his pre-visi t intervi e w that a satelli t e is a body orbiting another in 
space, and that it can be artific i a l or natural .  He understood that a sa tellite dish points to a 
satell i t e in space, and that the shape of the di sh is ?proba b l y the most effici e n t shape to 
receiv e radio signal s ? (vho16 p r e i n t 142). After the visit, John expand e d upon his previo u s 
knowledg e , for example ?the bigger the dish the further the dist ance can be so I think to 
pick up radio waves? (vho16po s i n t 079). He also was the only student to explain how the 
Earth is rotat i n g at the same speed that the satelli t e moves around the Earth, so that a dish 
can remai n in a fixed posit i o n (a geost a t i o n a r y  orbit) . His underst a n d i n g of size and scale 
was also classif i e d at level 3. 
216 
 
John?s underst a n d i n g of grav ity was extensiv e before the visit to HartRAO. 
Withou t any prompt i n g , he just wrote the word  gravity in his pre-visit PMM, but did not 
elabora t e on it. Howeve r, he explai ne d in his interv i e w that gravit y is a pullin g force, and, 
even before he was questi o n e d about the cause of  gravit y , he clarif i e d that the pulling force 
was relate d to the mass of the planet . He th eref o r e could explai n that gravit y on the moon 
would be much less than that of Earth, thus: 
John : I know it?s one- s i x t h of Eart h ? s , it?s obv i o u s ly not, you obvi ou s ly 
coul d jump furth e r and thing s like that. 
(vho 16 p r e i n t 178) 
He could also explain that gr avi t y on Jupite r would be much higher than that of Earth (or 
the other planet s ) due to Jupite r ? s great size. John was the only studen t in the sample who, 
in addition to his knowledge of gravity, could identify that the Eart h?s gravity caused the 
moon to remain in orbit about  the Earth, and that the moon ?s gravity caused tides on the 
Earth. Some studen t s (mostl y from John?s school ,  suggesting that had learnt this from the 
teacher) knew some of the effects of gravit y, but John was the only one who prior to the 
visit had such an extensi v e knowled g e .  
 
Figure 7.9 John?s post-visit PMM 
J o h n ? s pre-vis i t PMM was so full of words that in his post-visit PMM  he used the reverse 
side of the sheet to make sever a l addit i o n s (Figure 7.9 and Appendi x J). Six of these were 
217 
 
additi o n a l facts about the planet s Jupit e r , Eart h, Mars Venus Saturn and Mercury that he 
didn?t write on the pre-vis i t PMM. However , mo st of these new facts were not things he 
had learnt at HartRAO, as he did discuss some of them in his pre-vi s i t inter v i e w . The other 
addition s he made to his PMM were detail s about differe n t type s of stars and other 
cosmol o g i c a l phenome n a : red giants , ?wite dwa rfs?, ?quasars?, ?nubul as? and black holes. 
He wrote a senten c e about each one, and they were furthe r discus s e d in his interv i e w . 
What came out in the inter vi e w was that he alrea d y knew quite a lot about these 
phenome n a , and that the visit had remi nde d him of his pre-ex i s t i n g knowledge. In the 
Human Const r u ct i vi s t frame w o r k this is an example of emergenc e ,  which John showed 
more extensively than any other student in th e study. However, in some cases he combined 
his prior knowled g e with what he knew alread y and developed his knowl edg e still further. 
For exampl e , prior to the visit John alread y kne w about white dwarf stars, which he listed 
on his PMM but did not elabor a t e on during the pre-visi t intervie w , although he was asked 
about them. Howeve r , in his post visit PMM and interview John elab or a t e d on them as 
being dead stars and the fact th at they spin fast e r than bigger star s . This elabo ra t i o n was a 
result of the refere n c e made to them by the educator at HartRAO wh ich John referr e d to in 
his interview. At HartRAO stude nts were not only told about livin g and dead stars but also 
had the experie n c e of spinnin g like a star on a tu rnt a b l e . For the major i t y of stude nt s , this 
was merely ?fun? ? it was one of the aspects of the trip that thr ee stude n t s speci f i c a l l y 
referred to as being enjoyabl e ? but John could relate  his prior knowledge of dwarf stars as 
being just ?smal l ? to the idea that small , dead stars spin faste r than large r , livin g star s . He 
was then able to elabor a t e on this in hi s post-visit PMM and in terview, providing an 
exampl e of differ e n t i a t i o n in his unders t a n d i n g of stars. 
In his post-v i s i t interv i e w , John elabor a t e d on three aspect s of gravity . First he 
suggest e d that gravity on the Sun would be ve ry high, and referre d to the scales used at 
HartR A O that indi c at e a pers on?s weight on a number of di fferent planets (but not the 
Sun): 
John : Phoo o ? don? t reme mb e r the exac t amo u n t of grav i ty , but it?s 
way stronge r than the Earth? s becaus e if you check your scales 
there? s around thousa n d s of scale s to the Sun so that mu st be 
pulli n g of gravi ty . 
(vho 16 p o s i n t 099) 
218 
 
Second, when questio n e d on how the Earth?s gr avit y affect s the moon, John referre d to 
Newton (?the guy with the apple?) and how he postula t e d the way in which the moon 
orbitin g the Earth is held in place yet does not fall towards the Earth.  
John: But as he said the gravi ty of the Earth keeps on going fast I?ve 
forgo t t e n I? m try in g to think how to expla i n it ja.  The gravi ty of 
the Earth just keeps on the mass or obvio u s ly the apple is going to 
fall to the groun d so the gravi t y of the Earth .  And let? s say the 
moon has falle n and by the time the moon has got so far, where it 
would have falle d the Earth has alrea d y rotat e d undern e a t h it so 
then it just carri e s on.   
(vho16 p o s i n t 107) 
John was the only studen t in the study to be able  to describ e the concept of freefal l . Again, 
I consi d e r that this is anoth e r examp l e of emerg e n c e , where b y John is descr i b i n g the Earth -
 moon relationship which he has learnt about at some point prior to the visit to HartRAO, 
but which the conside r a t i o n of  gravi t y durin g the visit a nd my ques tioning about his 
understa n d i n g of gravity has enc oura g e d him to elabora t e on. 
Thirdl y , John was the only studen t who unders tood that everything has gravity, even a 
human being. In his post-v i s i t intervie w he stated: 
Inter v i e w e r : So what, does every t h in g ha ve gravi ty or is it just moons and 
plan e t s and Suns ? 
John:   Every th i n g has gravi t y . 
Inte r v i e w e r : So have you got grav i ty ? 
John:  Proba b ly a reall y , really minut e amoun t , but ja. 
(vho 16 p o s i n t 121- 12 7 ) 
I did not probe where this idea came from, so it is not possi b l e to tell whet h e r it was prior 
knowl e d g e (more likel y I think) or whethe r it developed during the visit or questioning. 
Regard i n g the Sun?s mo veme n t across the sky, John follow e d a train of though t simila r to 
the one he showed in his pre-vis i t intervi e w , where he starte d with the idea that the Earth 
orbiting the Sun causes the apparent move me n t , but change d his explana t i o n as he 
progre s s e d : 
Inter v i e w e r : And why does the sun move  across the sky every d ay ?  What? s 
goi n g on? 
John: Beca u s e the Sun is going arou n d , I mean the plane t is going 
arou n d the Sun. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay and so the night and day is the plan e t going aroun d the Sun. 
John:  Yes and the plane t turns . 
Interv i e w e r : Okay . 
219 
 
John:   No, the nigh t and day is the plane t turni ng and the years is the 
plan e t going arou n d the Sun. 
(vho 16 p o s i n t 041- 05 5 ) 
It seems that during the answer i n g of the quest i o n, once the inter vi e w e r repea t s the answe r , 
John reassesses his idea, and changes it to the correc t one, in a simila r way that he did in 
the pre-vis i t intervi e w . In Chapter 9 I suggest that the use of intervi e w and clarification by 
the inter vi ew e r of what the stude n t has said, al lows studen t s to consid e r  their answer and in 
some cases make chang e s to it. 
So what did John learn on th is visit to HartRAO?  Ta ble 7.17 provides a summary 
of his learning from the human constructivist frame work. As we have seen, prior to the 
visit he was probably the most  knowled g e a b l e student in the study. Yet, according to his 
PMMs and inter v i e w s , the visit did still enabl e hi m to expan d furth e r on his pre-v i s i t 
explan a t i o n s . Accord i n g to our definit i on of learning, this shows that John did learn during 
the visit, but for hi m, the learni n g was not so much an increa s e in factua l knowle d g e 
(addit i o n ) , but more an expans ion or reorganisation of his prior knowl e d g e . Using Braun d 
and Reiss?s defini t i o n of lear ning (2004), John deepened hi s awaren e s s and ideas, and 
reflec t e d on his visit in term s of his prior knowledge. In hu ma n construc t i v i s t terms John 
only added five new pieces of knowledge to his repert o i r e of facts, but he referre d to 12 
exampl e s of where the visit remi nd e d him of  his prior knowledge, which he then expanded 
on. In his post-visit interview, John twice explicitly stated that the visi t ?remin d e d ? him of 
things he already knew, but hadn?t spoken of durin g the pre-v i s i t inte rv i e w . Accord i n g to 
Falk and Dierking (1997, 2000), learn i n g in museums and scienc e centres is not only about 
the acquisi t i o n of new knowled g e , skills and va lues, but often acts as a prompt for 
previou sl y - l e a r n e d facts, which are reinfor c e d by the visit . Accor d i n g to Galla g h er (in Falk 
and Dierkin g 2000) the physica l contex t of th e visit is respon s i b l e for the remi nd e r of 
previou s l y - l e a r n e d informa t i o n . This appears to be what happen e d to John; he stated that 
the visit remin d e d him of facts a bout stars and related details: 
John: But some of the thing s chang e d like aroun d (inau d i b le ) becau s e I 
reme mb e r ag es ago I read about white dwarf s and all that stuff 
and black holes for a projec t .  Now I forgot about those quite a 
while ago so now I knew that stuff . 
(vho 16 p o s i n t 139) 
This also confor ms to the constr u c t i v i s t position on the re-const r uction of knowledge as 
me mori e s are create d , recon s t r u c t e d and recombi n e d (Rosch e l l e 1995) . John, as the studen t 
who visite d HartRA O with apparen t l y the gr eat e s t amoun t of prio r knowledge, was also 
220 
 
the studen t who drew on this prior knowle d g e to  context u a l i s e what he experie n c e d at the 
visit or s ? cent r e and radio teles c o pe . In Piage t i an terms John was assimilating  knowle d g e : 
inter p r e t i n g the visit exper i e n c e on the basis of his previou s  unders t a n d i n g , much of it not 
reme mbe r e d until the visit stimu l a t e d the me mo ri e s . Accor di n g to Piaget , John was fitting 
new sense impres s i o n s into a pre-exi s t i n g cognit i v e struct u r e , ?build i n g additi o na l 
unders t a n d i n g and reinfor c i n g known th ings ? (Falk &Dierk i n g 2000, p. 29).  
Table 7.17 Frequency of Human Co nstructivism codes for John 
Knowledge 
construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview  
PostPMM & 
Interview  
Pre 
Interview 
Post 
Interview 
Totals 
A dd itio n n/a 3 n/a 2 5 
Emer g e n c e 3* 7 n/a 2 12 
Diff er en tiatio n n/a 1 n/a 2 3 
Disc r imin a tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Reco n tex tu a lisa tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Affe c tiv e 1 0 6 3 10 
Cona tiv e 0 1 0 0 1 
Totals 4 12 6 9 31 
* in his pre-vis i t intervi e w John referre d to how  he had used books and other resource s in 
his accumul a t i o n of knowle d g e about astron o m y , and the quest i o n i n g remi n d e d him of 
this. 
 
But John also needed the approp r i a t e conte x t in  which to recall his previo u s me mori e s , and 
the context of HartRAO was appropri a t e for John?s emergent knowledge to be expressed. 
Falk and Dierkin g sugges t that context whic h mental l y cues one individ u a l ? s me mori e s 
might not promp t anoth e r perso n , as conte x t is ?always relativ e to the person? (Falk & 
Dierki n g , 2000 p. 32). It would follow that scie nc e centr e s will there f o r e promo t e emerg e nt 
learn i n g mai nl y in those peopl e who have e nough prior knowle d g e for the context u a l cues 
to have some effect. 
In conce p t ua l chang e terms , John showe d little affect i v e learni ng after the visit, 
with most of the affec t i v e categ or i e s evide n t in his pre- vi s i t inter v i ew . Simil a r l y , he 
showe d only one exa mp l e of conat i v e learn i n g , where he refer r e d to ?goin g on the Inter n e t ? 
and looki n g at the Hubbl e teles c o p e photo g r a p h s of  galax i e s . All three other stude n t s in the 
DD category showed a greater de gree of affectiv e le arning than John. They all expressed 
great inter e s t in astron o m y and had strong e r feelings regarding what they enjoyed about 
the visit than John. In this respec t theref o r e , John is unusual : he was excepti o n a l l y clear 
when it came to his emergent learning, but he showed less strong emo tions about what he 
had learnt . 
221 
 
In terms of his extens i v e background knowledge, John could be said to be already 
scient i f i c a l l y liter a t e (in astron o my ) befor e his visit , thoug h he did impro v e this sligh t l y ? 
?small stuff? in his own words. There? s no ev iden c e from his post-v i si t PMM or inter v i ew 
that he chang e d his cultu ra l scien t i fi c liter ac y to any exten t ; agai n , this was alrea d y 
subst a n t i al prior to the visit . 
7.6  Discussion 
 
This chapte r has descri b e d four studen t s in some det ail, from Nonkululeko who 
demon s t r a t e d very littl e prior knowledge of Big Ideas in as tro n o my , to John who displa y e d 
very substant i a l knowledg e . Alt hough categorised as being at di fferent levels, each student 
descri be d showed little change in their ow n knowle d g e of Big Ideas after their visit, 
sugge s t i n g that they hadn?t learn t much in th e traditio n a l sense of havin g assimi l a t e d facts 
about Big Ideas such as gravity and the Sun. With the except i o n of Nonkul u l e k o , the 
studen t s appear e d to have some person a l intere st in astronomy, and th e i r me mo r i e s of the 
visit relat e d to these inter e st s . For examp l e Botho was inter e st e d in stars and Neo in 
planets . There are however , differe n c e s between the students . Both Nonkulul e k o and 
Botho had relativ e l y little prior knowle d g e on which to build addit i o n a l compr e h e n s i o n of 
Big Ideas, and their learn i n g consi st e d mainl y  of additi o n , with mini ma l other cogni t i ve 
categories of learning. I would s uggest that their very lack of  prior knowledge in the area 
of astron o m y meant that they were only able to const r u c t addit i o na l incre me n t a l facts in 
their cognit i v e knowle d g e fra mew o r k . The same  students however, were both able to 
discuss enjoya b l e aspect s of the trip, although Nonkulul e k o ? s inter e s t in the visit appeared 
to be minima l . In contra s t , given that Botho entered and won a nationa l astrono my 
compe t i t i o n after the visit , it would appea r that stude n t s ? intere s t is not neces s a ri l y relat e d 
to academi c learni n g abili t y .  
Neo and John also showed some simila r f eatur e s of learni n g . With their greate r 
prior knowled g e than Nonkulu l e k o and Botho, both were able to build on what they 
already knew to not only add to their knowle d g e  struct u r e but to differe n t i a t e it. Neo 
differe n t i a t e d severa l areas of her knowled g e , in clud i n g the Sun, stars and the satelli t e dish, 
whereas John showed differentiation in his understanding of gravity and stars. Neo 
howeve r differ s from John in that she mainly acquir e d knowle d g e by additi o n , wherea s he 
drew on his very extensive previous readi n g about astron o my and found that what was 
presented at HartRAO reminded hi m of what he already knew, a nd extended it. In terms of 
222 
 
affectiv e and conativ e learnin g ,  Neo and John showed very sim ila r profil e s afte r the visit, 
althou g h Neo was mor e express i v e in terms of  enjoyin g the experi e n c e at the centre . 
Neithe r of them appear e d to have been motiv a t e d to act on what they had learnt after 
retur ni n g fro m the visit , but it is possi b l e that additio n a l data collect i o n might have 
detect e d this.  
In contras t to the student s descri b e d , w ho showe d no appar e n t change in their 
knowle d g e of Big Ideas after their visit, Chapter 8 desc ribe s three students who all 
demonst r a t e d change in their Big Ideas concep t s .  
223 
 
Chapter 8 
8 How individual students learnt about astronomy (2) 
This chapter describes the learning s hown by three students who visited the 
Johannesburg Planetarium and the Hartebeesthoek Radio Astronomy 
Observatory. Each of these students showed different levels of prior 
knowledge, and the portraits show how th ey improved their knowledge of Big 
Ideas as well as other aspects of astronomy. 
8.1  Introduction 
T h i s chapt er prese nt s portr ai t s of three stude n t s who all lie in the blue part of the 
classif i c a t i o n scheme based on their Bi g Idea knowled g e (Table 7.1), and who all 
improv e d their mean ?score ? after the visit compa r e d with their initi a l ideas . Chapt e r 7 
showed that even when student s did not apparently change their knowledge of Big Ideas, 
they were able to demo nst r a t e other types of learni n g as a result of their scien c e centr e 
experi e n c e . Such other types of learni n g might  norma l l y be ?hidde n ? if the studen t s are 
assessed only by traditional met ho d s such as tests or questio n n a i r e s of astrono my conten t . 
This chapt er shows that chang e s both in knowledge of Big Ideas and in other more 
indivi d u a l ways can occur in so me stud e n t s . As all thre e stude n t s in this chap t er chan g e d 
their knowl e d g e of Big Ideas , I have inclu d e d an addit i o na l table in each portr a i t which 
summa r i s e s these change s . 
8.1.1  Portrait of Fatima (scf15) 
Table 8.1 Position of Fatima on Big Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0    
C 2.1-2.5    
B 1.6-2.0 Fatima    
A 1.0-1.5     
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
Table 8.2 Fatima?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Solar 
Syste m 
Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
2 2 2 2 2 3 1 2 1 2 1 1 1.5 2.0 
224 
 
Fatima was chose n as an examp l e of a stude n t in the AB categ o r y . Accor d i n g to my 
analysi s of her PMM and interv i ew , she went on the trip to the Plane t a ri u m with littl e 
knowl e d g e about Big Ideas in astron o my and yet increa s e d her knowle d g e across nearly 
two categor i e s : her average Big Ideas knowle d g e level increa s e d by 0.5, fr om 1.5 to 2.0, on 
the borders of A and C. Category AB contains 8 student s , and it was difficult selecting one 
studen t to repres e n t the whole group. I sele cte d Fatima on the basis that she showed 
substa n t i a l change (only Tlotlo  in her categor y showed great e r change with an avera g e 
knowl e d g e incre a se of 0.6) and that she was initi a l l y not ignorant  about astron o my , but 
that she held a numbe r of misco n c e p t i o n s a bout the solar system and space, many of which 
change d after the visit.  
Fatima is a 14-year-old ?coloured? 1 5  grade 8 student and attends Lourdes Girls 
School , a private school in the western suburb s  of Johanne s b u r g . Fatima lives in a suburb 
in the western part of  Johann e s b u r g , appro x i ma t e l y 10km from the school, and comes fr om 
an Afrika a ns - s p e a ki n g famil y . Fatima drew her PMM on 9 th  Octob er and was inter vi e w ed 
on 13 th  October , the day before she visited the Johann e s b u r g Planet a r i u m as part of the 
class visit. On 16 th  October , all the student s who had been  on the field trip did their repeat 
PMM during school time and I re-int e r v i e w e d  Fatima on the same da y. Acade mi c a l l y , 
Fatima is a very high achiev e r , gettin g 85% in her year mark for all subjec t s in 2003, and it 
is inter e s t i ng that this high mark was not reflect e d in her knowle dge of Big Ideas in 
astron o my prior to her visit to the planet a r i u m. 
When Fatima was asked whethe r she was looki ng forwar d to the vi sit , she indic a t e d 
in the affirmat i v e , but did not appear particul a r l y excite d about the prospe c t of it. For her it 
seemed to be just a visi t the class was going on. She had heard the plan et Mars in the news 
recent l y ?tha t we would be ab le to see it like now and th en? (scf15 p r e i n t 134), but she 
hadn? t attempt e d to look for it herse l f. She al so reme mb e r e d Mark Shuttl e w o r t h going into 
space and how ?that looke d like reall y cool? (scf1 5 p r e i nt 134). 
Fatima said that the part of school she liked was having her fr iends around her and 
learnin g about some subjects, accounting (which she enjoyed) and history (which she 
found easy) being her favour i t e s . Conver s e l y she found biology, geography and science 
hard and not enjoya b l e . Her recrea t i o n a l activ i t i e s includ e gymnas t i c s and readin g fictio n . 
                                                 
 
15  Under the former aparth eid syste m coloured meant a pe rso n of mixe d race . The designatio n is used here to 
ind icate the commu n ity fro m which the studen t comes. 
225 
 
She thoug h t that there mi ght be life elsew h e r e in  the solar syste m, such as on Mars, but did 
not belie v e in life elsew he r e in the unive r s e , a nd regarded the idea of aliens as ?stupid? . She 
knew that astrolo g y is ?like how the stars a nd moon and the planets influe n c e their lives? 
(scf15 p r e i n t 240), and had some belief in it. As a Muslim she explai n e d how import a n t 
observations of the Moon are in working out the time for fastin g (Ramad a n ) , and stated 
that she believe s Allah created everyth i n g .  
In her use of astron o my words and term s Fatima?s prior knowledge of astronomy 
was above-a v e r a g e for the student s in the st udy. Her use of astronomy-related words was 
26 in her pre-vi s i t PMM (study avera g e was 20, school averag e 21), her post-v i s i t 
vocabu l a r y rose by 7, the same as the average of her school (7.6) a nd the study (7.2). She 
was one of only three students who used the wo rd s ?axi s ? , ?mete o r o i d ? and ?crat er ? , and the 
only student to use the terms ?heaven l y body?  and ?tides? on her PMM. Her pre-visit 
perso n a l meani n g map (Figur e 8.1 and Append i x J) was not extens i v e , but showed sever a l 
basic facts. She listed the nine planet s and stat e d that they rotat e on their axes and revol v e 
aroun d the Sun. She furth e r liste d what you find in space, includ i n g planet s , come ts and 
galaxies. She stated that star s are balls of burnin g gas, th at they form patte r n s calle d 
constellation and they influence your life. She believed that the biggest star  is the Sun. 
Unlike any of the other studen t s she stated th at the Moon has an influence on the tides on 
Earth . When quest i o n e d furth e r about this, she sa id that she learn t in geogr a p h y that ?some 
force s ?cent r i f u g a l force s , some t h i n g like th at? (scf15 p r e p m m 11) influen c e tides in the 
oceans. She could clearly explain the diffe r e n c e betwe e n astro n o my and astrol o gy , and 
knew that the former ?deal more with scient i f i c thing s ? while the latte r ?are more on the 
life? (scf15p r e p m m 31). She could state a fe w facts about the planets and knew that 
galaxies are ?clusters of st ars? (scf15 p r e p m m 59) and that  we ?live on? the Milky Way 
galaxy , a spiral . This PMM and her elabor a t i o n in the interv i e w shows that Fatima had 
clear l y learn t quite a lot a bout basic astron o m y , appare n t l y mainly from her school 
geography lessons, which she professed to find hard.  
226 
 
 
Figure 8.1 Fatima?s PMM (pre- and post-visit) 
In her pre-visit structured  inter v i ew Fatima appea r e d  to have a number of 
misconceptions about aspects of astronomy. Fi rst, although she knew about stars and the 
names of some conste l l a t i o n s , she was unawar e  that planets are visible in the night sky. 
Second , she referr e d to the Earth ?rotat i n g ar ound the Sun? (scf15 p r e i n t 086) as the reason 
for night and day. Interes t i n g l y, Fatima was the only student to corre ct l y stat e on her PMM 
that planets ?revolv e around the sun? and ?rotate on their own axes? (scf15pre 27 & 28). 
However, when asked to explain why the S un moves across the sky every day, she referre d 
to the Earth ?rotat i n g ? or ?moving ? around the Sun as being the reason. She further 
demons t r a t e d the Earth orbiti n g the Sun using the model, to explain the Sun?s appare n t 
move me n t daily across the sky. Third, althou g h she correct l y referre d to stars as being 
burnin g balls of gas, she then added (in respons e to ?what are stars??) ?And then, I think, I 
think because of the moon?s reflect i o n ? ? (s cf15preint 114) indicati ng quite na?ve thinking 
about the stars as somehow reflecting light from  the Moon. Fourthly, in terms of scale, she 
had an idea that it would take only about 2 days for a spac es h i p to reach the neares t star 
beyon d the solar syste m. The real ity is that you can reach the Moon in about 2 days, but it 
227 
 
would take thousan d s of years to reach the ne arest star. She also believed that stars are 
?smalle r than ?.the Moon? (scf15p r e i n t 118). 
Afte r her visit to the plane t a ri u m, Fati ma added sever a l phras e s to her PMM, some 
of which she elabo r at e d in her intervi e w , inclu di n g the follow i n g : She corre ct l y state d the 
length of rotati o n of the Earth being 24 hour s, and revolu t i o n around the Sun being 365? 
days. She referr e d to ?lots of ultra Plutos ? (scf15 p o s 15), which she explai n e d as ?there 
was like mo re Plutos that they found, like, othe r planet s ?.. I think in the solar system as 
well? (scf15p o s p m m 039). She also sugge s t e d that astero i d s might be the 10 th  plane t which 
disint e g r a t e d . She discus s e d the fact that Mars ? soil is being teste d to see wheth er it is 
?suita b l e for growin g crops becaus e they want to take some peopl e up into Mars? 
(scf15pospmm 053). When asked further about this , she descri b e d a probe sent to Mars as 
being ?what looked like a skateboa r d that  went into the rock? (scf15pospmm 057), 
referr i n g to the planeta r i u m photograph of the NASA Mars vehi cle sent in the 1990s. She 
gave the names of the two close s t stars to our solar syste m ? Proxima Centauri  and Alpha 
Centauri  ? and stated that it will take 4000 years 1 6  to reach the nearest one. She 
disting u i s h e d between the inne r rocky planets and the outer gaseous ones (althoug h Pluto 
is the odd one out), and said a body qualifies as a planet if it has a moon orbiting it. She 
referred to meteorit e s as shooting stars, and explained that it is not actually  a star but a 
meteor which burns in the Earth?s atmosphe r e . She also referr e d the ?South Africa n 
Largest Telescop e ? as being built, which she fo und inter e st i ng . Sever al of these facts show 
that Fatima developed a greater degree of cult ura l scient i f i c litera c y  (Shen, 1975) in that 
the visit stimu l a t e d her inter e s t in and appr eci a t i o n of the achieve me n t s of science . 
In her struc t u r e d inter v i e w Fatima also elabor ated on or changed a number of her pre-visit 
ideas (Table 8.3). 
Table 8.3 Ways in which Fatima ch anged her ideas about concepts 
Concept Pre-visit idea Post-visit idea 
P l a n e t s visibl e 
in night sky 
Plane t s not visib l e  Venus and Mars visib l e , appea r i n g 
like stars 
Const e l l a t i o n 
names  
Orion , South e r n Cross Orion , South e r n Cross , Sagit t a r i u s , 
Libra 
Solar sy ste m Sun with the nine planet s revol v i n g ; Plane t s ; elabo r a t e d on astero i d s 
                                                 
 
16  This figure is correct on ly if the space ship used can trav el 20 times faster than our curren t ones, as 
explained by the planetar iu m presenter . 
228 
 
Concept Pre-visit idea Post-visit idea 
o t h e r heave n l y bodie s : the moon, 
co met s , aster o i d s , met eo r o i d s , 
meteo r s . Shape unsur e . 
and moon s ; also incl u d e d star s . 
Shape unsur e . 
The Sun A star, gives heat and light. Made 
of gases.  
A ball of burni n g gas, a star and 
it?s made up of hy dro g e n and 
heliu m. 
Sun mov e me n t 
across sky 
Becau s e Eart h moves arou n d the 
Sun.  
Accep t e d scie n t i f i c expla n a t i o n of 
Earth rotat i o n 
Stars Balls of burnin g gas and the 
Moon? s refle c t i o n . Small e r than the 
Moon . 
As pre-vi s i t , excep t no refere n c e to 
Moon? s refl e c t i o n . 
Ti me to reach 
nearest st ar 
2 day s 4000 year s 
Moon phase s Unsur e of reaso n for ph ases Still unsure of reason for phases 
 
L i k e Helen from the same school , Fatima appear e d to have increa s e d her knowle d g e about 
seve r al conce p t s as a resul t of the visit . Most of this increas e appears to be her abili t y to 
reme mbe r facts she was told at the planet a r ium. Analysing her learning using the human 
const r u c t i vi st frame w o r k , Fatima ? s frequ e n c i e s for each code (Table 8.4) show that the 
main HC categor y of learnin g that Fatima de m ons tr a t e d was that of addition , as follows: 
Whereas previous l y she had not known that  planets are visible in the night sky, 
after the visit she state d that Venus and Mars ar e visib l e , and that they look ?like stars ? . In 
her post-visit PMM she referred to the ?ultra Pl uto s ? which she clarifi e d as being additional 
Pluto-l i k e planets found by astrono m e r s . A furthe r fact about planet s was her referen c e to 
possible crops on Mars and the idea that peopl e might be taken to Mars. As all of these 
piece s of infor ma t i o n featu r e d in  the plane t a r i u m show, I regard  these as new facts that she 
added to her existing knowledge as a direct  result of the visit to the planet a ri u m. 
Her knowle d g e of facts about the Sun increa s e d a little. In her pre-visit interview 
she had referr e d to the Sun being a star, givin g off light and heat, and made of gases, but 
she didn?t know which ones. On probing, she c ould also not explain how the Sun gives off 
light and heat. After the visit, Fatima used  the phrase ?ball of burning gas? (scf15p o s i n t 
053), and named the gases as helium and hydrog e n . Where a s she used the phrase ?ball of 
burning gas? for stars prior to he r visit, she did not use it to describe the Sun. After the visit 
she used the expres s i o n to refer to the sun as we ll. I consi de r that these addit i o na l facts that 
Fatima acqui r e d about the Sun are example s of the HC categor y ?a ddition?, in th at they are 
small additi o n a l facts about a concep t sh e already has some knowledge of. She 
229 
 
reme mbe r e d from the plane t a r i u m that it w ould take 4000 years in  the fastest possible 
spaces h i p we could build to reach the nearest st ar. She had previousl y stated that it would 
take 2 days to reach this star, and althou g h sh e now reme mb e r e d the new fact, she gave no 
other indicati o n that her unders tanding of the concept of the enormous scale of the solar 
system had increa s e d . In HC terms, she wa s adding a fact to he r knowledge, and showed 
no sign of knowledg e restruct u r i n g . Finally, in her post-vis i t P MM she noted the fact that 
?Sout h Afric a is makin g a teles c o p e calle d ?S ALT? South African Largest Telescop e ? , and 
she confirm e d in the intervi e w that this was new knowled g e .  
Table 8.4 Frequency of Human Cons tructivism codes for Fatima 
Knowledge 
Construction category 
PrePMM + Int PostPMM + 
Int  
Pre Int Post 
Int  
TOTALS: 
A dd itio n n/a 10 n/a 4 14 
Emerg e n c e n/a 1 n/a 1 2 
Diff er en tiatio n n/a 0 n/a 1 0 
Disc r imin a tio n n/a 1 n/a 1 2 
Reco n tex tu a lisa tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Affe c tiv e 0 0 4 4 8 
Cona tiv e 0 0 1 0 1 
TOTA L S : 0 1 5 8 27 
 
T a b l e 8.4 also shows that Fatima demons t r a t e d 2 example s of the HC categor y 
emerg e n c e : In one respe c t , Fatima made a s ubsta n t i a l shift in her knowle d g e , and that was 
her explanat i o n for why the Sun moves across th e sky ever day. In her pre-vis i t interv i e w , 
Fatima explai n e d the Sun?s appare n t move me n t  as being due to the Earth ?moving around 
the sun?. When handed the model Sun and Ea rth, Fatima both confirme d and confused her 
initia l explan a t i o n by referr i n g to  the Earth ?rotat i n g around th e sun?, but she demon s t r a t e d 
the Earth? s orbit around the S un while doing this, sugges t i n g she was confus i n g the term 
revolv e and rotate . Howeve r , in her post-v i s i t inter v i e w Fatima refe r r e d only to the Earth 
rotat i n g (this time she demon s t r a t e d it spinni ng on its axis), and in her PMM she described 
the ?Earth takes 24 hours to do a complet e rotation [therefore] a da y?. She further wrote 
?Earth takes 365?  days 4 a revolution round the sun [therefore] year? (scf15pos 9 & 16). 
Neithe r of these piece s of infor ma t i o n were descr i b e d in the plane t a r i um show, and the 
present e r never used the words rotate, rotati o n , revolve or revolut i o n . Unfortu n a t e l y , I did 
not probe Fatima about where these facts had co me from, but it is likel y that they were 
previ o u sl y learnt pieces of infor ma t i o n that she reme m b e r e d after the visi t to the 
plane t a r i u m. Both of the facts appea r to be the sort of infor ma t i o n one lear n s about in 
geogra p h y , and it may be (but cannot be proved ) that the visit trigge r e d their recall . I 
230 
 
therefo r e consid e r that her change in expl anation for the cause of day and night, from 
move me n t around the Sun to the Earth spinni n g was likely to be the HC category of 
emerg e n c e . 
Anoth e r addit i o na l fact that Fatima appea r s to have reme mbe r e d as a resul t of her 
visit was regardi n g a 10 th  plane t . In both her post- v i s i t inter v i e w s she refer r e d to the idea 
that aster o i ds were proba b l y the remai n s of a 10 th  planet, which disintegrated. While the 
planet a r i u m prese n t e r made severa l refer e n c e s to astero i d s , at no point did she state that 
they may have been the remain s of a 10 th  planet , and she made no refere nc e at all to a 10 th  
planet , althou g h she did talk a bout there being ?anoth e r 600 Plut os ? (Lour d e s pltm 349) as 
addition a l possible planets as part of the solar system. Again, Fatima may have heard about 
the 10 th  planet elsewh e r e , and when asked about it after her visit, reme mb e r e d that the 
disintegration of an additional pl ane t is one expl a n a t i o n for the prese nc e of aster o i d s in the 
solar syste m. This is again there f o r e a possi bl e exampl e of the HC categor y of emer gen c e 
in Fatima ? s exper i e n c e . 
Altho u g h Fatima showe d mainl y incre m e n t a l knowle d g e acquis i t i o n , and two 
possib l e examp l e s of emerge n c e , she demons t r a t e d one inst an c e of what I classi f i e d as 
discrimination, where she restructured her knowl ed g e to demons t r a t e the simila r i t i e s and 
differen c e s between planets. In her post- vis i t PMM and intervie w she explaine d the 
differ e n c e betwe e n the inner ?soli d? plane t s , and the outer gaseous planets , with Pluto 
being the ?odd one out? (scf15 p o s p m m 011), and referred to the planetarium presenter 
descri b i n g this. She stated ?The astron o me r at the Plane t a r i u m said Mercu r y , Venus and 
Mars, they all have one thing in commo n becau s e they are more solid , and then Jupit er , 
Saturn and Uranus and Neptun e , they are more gaseous and then Pluto they?re not so sure 
becau s e it?s like the odd one out.? (scf15p o s p m m 11). Accordi n g to human constru c t i vi s t 
theor y , Fatima has estab l i s h e d criter i a for de fining the different type s of planet. Whereas 
previ o u s l y she was able to list the nine plan et s , state that they revolv e around the Sun, and 
rotate on their axes, she noted  in her PMM her new understa n d i n g of the concept of a 
plane t .  
Fatima showed no other categor i e s of hu man constr u c t i vi s t cognit i ve learni n g , but 
demon s t r a t ed severa l ideas which fall into the affec t i v e domai n . In her pre-v i s i t intervi e w , 
Fatima made it clear that she disli k e d schoo l subject s related to as tronomy, notably science 
and geography. When asked why she didn?t lik e the subjects, she said ?I just don?t 
unders t a n d some ti me s and it?s harder for me to do? (scf15preint 162). Although she 
231 
 
profess e d to be looking forwar d to the visi t, she appeare d to be ?lukewa r m? about the 
prosp e c t . Howev e r , after the visit , Fatima expresse d that she had thorough l y enjoyed the 
visit: ?I though t it was very intere s t i n g becau s e I learn t more thing s and it?s just very nice 
going there.  I enjoyed it? (scf15 p o s i n t 145). It appear s that the visit reinfo r c e d Fatima ? s 
perso n a l inter e s t : prior to the visit she state d th at the stars inter e st e d her, and after the visit 
she regarded the stars in the dark as being th e most enjoya b l e part of the experience. For 
Fatima , the situat i o n a l intere s t (Hidi and Harach i e w i c z 2000) promot e d by the planet a r i u m 
experi e n c e was congru e n t with her own individual interest. 
There is little evidenc e that Fatima s howed much learni n g in the conati v e domain . 
She had an idea that if she was to teach this topic to grade 8 learne r s  she would do what her 
teache r was doing, and ?take them to the Planet a r i u m and I?d try to make these thing s more 
interest i n g ? (scf15pr e i n t 212). However, when  asked how she might do this, Fatima could 
not elabor a t e on what she might do. Unlike some  other studen t s , Fatima was not inspir e d 
by the visit to carry out any ac tivit i e s suggest e d by the planeta r i u m visit, althoug h she was 
interv i e w e d only 2 days after th e visit , givin g littl e time fo r follow-up on her own. In the 
categor y of ?trust? , Fatima expres s e d her Islami c belief s , and how import a nt it is to obse r ve 
the Moon in order to determi n e the Islami c months , especi a l l y the fast. 
In summa r y , Fatima ? s main abili t y after th e visit was to reme m b e r accur a t e l y many 
of the facts she was shown during the demonstr a t i o n . While her initi a l ideas about some of 
the astrono m y concep t s were wrong or misc onc e i v e d , she was able to correct them 
effec t i v e l y by the time of the post-v i s i t inter v i e w , as she was able to reme mbe r much of 
what she had been told durin g the visit . This sugge s t s that for visit o r s such as Fatima , the 
factual nature of the show was beneficial in terms of learni n g . Fatima also demons t r a t e d 
two exampl e s of HC discri mi n a t i o n which were not shown by student s in Chapter 7. 
8.1.2  Portrait of Brenda (swo70) 
Table 8.5 Position of Brenda on Big Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0  Brenda   
C 2.1-2.5     
B 1.6-2.0     
A 1.0-1.5     
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
232 
 
Table 8.6 Brenda?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Grav ity Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Parab o lic/ 
Satellite Dish 
Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
1 2 1 2 2 3 2 3 3 3 1 n/a 2.5 3 1.9 2.7 
Brenda was initially chosen as being repres e n t a t i v e of the 9 st udents in the largest 
catego r y : BC. These studen t s went to the sc ienc e centr e with aver age knowledge of big 
ideas and increased their knowledge still fu rther. It was difficu l t to choose a single 
indivi d u a l to repres e n t this group, and instea d of choosi n g a student from the BC category I 
select e d Brenda as a studen t who showed the large s t chang e in score , impro v e me n t s in 4 
big ideas and a signif i c a n t increa s e in her vocabul a r y . Brenda ? s knowle d g e level of big 
ideas change d from a mean of 1.9 to 2.7, while the vocabul a r y she used on her PMM 
increas e d from 19 to 28 words. 
Brenda is a 13-year - o l d grade 7 student studying at Balfour Fo rest School since 
grade 1. She is Sepedi-s p e a k i n g , and says her fa mil y also speak some Engli s h at home. She 
lives in a townsh i p in northe r n Johann e s b u r g and travel s to school by minibu s taxi each 
day. Brenda drew her PMM on 8 th  October and was interviewed on 21 st , 3 days before she 
visite d Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astron o m y Observa t o r y with her class on 24 th  October. She 
comple t e d her post-v i s i t PMM on 30 th  Octobe r , and was interv i e w e d the same day. 
Unfort u n a t e l y it was not possib l e to obtain Bren d a ? s acade mi c recor d from Balfou r Fores t 
School, as compute r s on which records were kept had been stolen in 2004. 
Brenda knew where she was going on her impend i n g visit, and that it was relate d to 
scienc e and techno l o g y , specif i c a l l y learni n g about practi c a l aspect s of the Earth, the 
planet s , stars and univer s e . She said she was lookin g forwar d to the visit as it would give 
her a chance to learn mo re about the univer s e . Br enda? s main interes t appeare d to be in the 
names of planet s and stars rathe r than scient i f i c aspects of the topi c. She said ?These 
planet s have got differe n t name s from ?  I don?t know, I think it?s about Jupite r or 
Uranus.  It was named after the Roman god of the sun or something.  I really find their 
name s inter e s t i n g ? (swo7 0 p r e i n t 305). She re me mb e r e d having heard about a meteor i t e 
possibly going to hit the Earth (t he previou s year) bu t hadn?t heard or r ead anyt h i ng else 
relat e d to the topic of astro n o my recen t l y . Prior to her visit and compa r e d to other stude n t s , 
Brenda did not show partic u l a r fasci n a t i o n or inter e s t in astr o n o my as a topic , but appea r e d 
to have a focus on astrolo g y and the na mes of constell a t i o n s and planets. 
233 
 
Brenda liked her school a lot, and explai n e d that she liked not only learni n g fro m 
the teacher s but also going to the librar y to look up information for herself. She said she 
liked all her school subjects, and did not have either a favouri t e or a least-favourite. In her 
spare time she has violin lessons , attend s church and does her homework. Her favourite 
televi s i o n progr a m me s are comed i e s and drama (soap opera s ) and the most recen t thing 
she had read for pleasure was a novel called Th e Witch Child . Brend a th oug h t that no other 
life has ever been found beyon d the Earth, but  that life mi ght be possib l e on Mars. She 
emphat i c a l l y did not believ e in aliens , but read s her horoscope and believes that what is 
predi c t e d does come true for he r. She believes in God, but does not thi nk that space and the 
univers e has much to do with God, except that he created them. She also thought that some 
of the names in the solar syste m were na med after gods, but did not specify which gods. 
Brenda ? s use of astrono my- r e l a t e d voca bu l a r y in her pre-vis i t PMM was about 
avera g e for the study at 19 words, while after the visit she used nine additi o n a l words, 
above the average for her school (5.4) and the study (6.8). She was the only student in the 
study who used the words ?horo s c o p e ? and ?Vir go? in her pre-vis i t PMM, confirmi n g her 
particular interest in astr ology. Her pre-visit PMM (Figur e 8.2 and Appendix J) was not 
very extensive: she named the nine planets, stated that th ey revolve aro und the Sun, gave 
some brief facts (e.g. relat i v e size, posit i o n) about Earth , Plut o , Mercu r y and Jupit e r , made 
some observations about space (no air or grav ity), name d the Milky Way  and referred to 
const el l at i o ns as the ?hor o s c o p e ? . In her in terview she could not elaborate on the Milky 
Way except that its stars are white ?and th at ? s why they? r e calle d the Milky Way? 
(swo70 p r e pm m 07). She gave some additi o na l facts about Jupite r , Mercur y and Pluto, and 
when asked what the Sun would look like view ed from Pluto she correc t l y stated ?Very 
small.  Like a small dot? (swo70 p r e pm m 31). This  indica t e s an abilit y to visual i s e posit i o n 
and scale from elsewh e r e in the solar syst em which is a sophist i c a t e d ability . Her 
capab i l i t y of rough l y estima t i n g scale was furthe r confi r me d by the fact that ?about ten 
Earths? could fit into Jupite r (swo70prepmm 53). Brenda re me mbered seeing the stars 
signs on a school visit to the bush (?at camp?) , includ i n g Capric o r n . She also referr e d to 
Mars being ?resea r c h e d ? to tell whethe r humans w ould be able to live there. Brenda stated 
that the topic of astron o my had been covere d ?usually every year we do it during the 
second term? (swo70 p r e p m m 75), and she had clea rl y learn t quite a few facts about the 
topic. Her enthus i a s m for school indica t e s  that she learns relatively easily. 
234 
 
 
Figure 8.2 Brenda?s PMM (pre and post) 
L i k e Botho, in terms of Big Ideas in astr on o my Brenda showed differ e n t levels of 
knowle d g e prior to her visit to HartRA O . Wh il e she didn? t know much factu a l info r ma t i o n 
about the Sun, she did know that day and night  are caused by the Earth spinning on its axis. 
She knew that the Sun is bigger than the Moon,  but was unable to explain why they look 
simil a r in size in the sky. Her conce p t of stars was quite na?ve : she refer r e d to them as 
?light s in the sky?, and althou g h she understood that they are much bigger ?up close? than 
they appear in the sky, her idea was that  are about ?as big as this room? in size 
(swo70 p r e i n t 165). Simi la r l y her id ea of what the stars are co mposed of ?When the planets 
are forme d , the piece s that were left over, fo r me d the stars ? (swo7 0 p r e i n t 169) indic a t e s an 
unsoph i s t i c a t e d notion , and she was classi f i e d at knowled g e level 1 for stars. Regardi n g 
gravity, Brenda?s knowledge was also at knowle d g e level 1, as she knew that it is some 
sort of pull (?the air that pulls us down fr om like float i n g aroun d ? ) an d that ?there?s no 
gravi t y is space ? (swo7 0p r e i nt 229 & 237). Becau s e of this idea, she did not think there 
would be any gravity on the Moon or planets. In contrast , her knowledg e  of satellites was 
close r to a scient i fi c notio n : she knew that a satel l i t e is an instr u me n t in space sendi n g 
signal s . She knew that a satell i t e dish points to  a satellite in space, but could not explain 
235 
 
how the shape of a dish is re late d to its functi o n . Brend a kne w that the phases of the Moon 
occurre d , but could not explain why.  
After her visit Brenda added severa l ac cur a t e l y - r e me mbe r e d facts to her PMM, 
including the following: 
? N e u t r o n stars as collapse d stars and re d giants which have expande d massiv e l y 
(?10 mill i o n time s diame t e r ?) . 
? S e v e r a l exampl e s of scale a nd number s in the univer s e : 
o T h e Earth ? s mass as 6x10 2 1  kg. 
o T h e relat i v e size of the Sun and the Earth (320 times in mass ) . 
o T h e Earth 1.3 light second s from the Moon and 8 light minute s from the 
Sun. 
o 1 0 0 , 0 0 0 million stars in the Milky Way 
? D i f f e r e n c e s betwee n the inner rocky planets and the outer gaseous ones. 
? T h e fact that Jupit e r has ?more than  enough? gravity and has 64 not 16 moons. 
Simil a r l y , in her post-v i s i t struc t u r e d in tervie w Brenda had changed some of her 
knowled g e about the big ideas and other con cepts in astronom y , summaris e d in Table 8.7. 
Table 8.7 Ways in which Brenda changed her ideas about concepts 
Concept Pre-visit idea Post-visit idea 
G r a v i t y Pulli n g force , no gravi t y in space or 
on Moon or Jupi t e r . 
Pulli n g force , low gravi ty on 
Moon, high grav i t y on Jupit e r & 
Sun, relat e d to size, Earth gravi ty 
does not affec t Moon. 
Star facts Lights in the sky , size of a room, 
co mpo s e d of pieces of planet s . 
Lights in the sky , simila r size to 
the Sun. 
Sun facts The bigges t star, Surfac e very hot, 
used for energy . 
Not the bigges t star, surfa c e 5 
milli o n degre e s , sun spot of 4500 
degrees . 
Size and scal e Star size due to distan c e , Sun 
bigges t star, stars siz e of a room 
Star size ~Su n , referenc e to light 
secon d s , minut e s and years. 
Day /n i g h t Scien t i f i c conce p t i o n Scien t i f i c conce p t i o n 
Moon phase s Descr i b e d phase s . No expl a n a t i o n . Not asked 
Satel l i t e & di sh Instr u m e n t in space , dish point s to 
satelli t e in space. 
As pre-vi s i t . 
236 
 
Her PMM shows that Brenda appear e d to be very adept at reme mbe r i n g facts and figures, 
and most of the knowled g e increa s e she demons t r a t e d on her post- v i s i t PMM I classi f i e d as 
additio n : she added severa l facts, partic u l a r l y  nu mbe r s , about stars , the Earth and the Sun 
to her existin g knowle d g e . When ta ken togeth e r , I sugges t that her unders t a n d i n g of size 
and scale in the solar syst e m has been consid e r a bl y enhan c e d by the additional facts learnt. 
Brend a herse l f confi r me d this later in her inter v i e w , where she refer r e d to the size of the 
Earth in relati o n to the Sun as being amazi n g : ?I told them about  the Earth.  Like it?s about 
3 and 24 ze ros and like the sun eh 320 times bi gger than the Earth, it?s very, very big.  I 
couldn? t believ e it? (swo70 p o s i n t 217). Although Brenda doe s not quite reme mb er the 
mass of the Earth correc t l y , and refers to it as 6 x 10 21  kg in her PMM and ?3 and 24 zeros? 
in her inter vi e w , her conce p t i o n of the size of  the Earth and also the size of the Earth and 
Sun in relati o n to each other have chang e d subst a n t i a l l y from her pre-v i s i t inter vi e w, where 
these were not mentio n e d . Simila r l y , Brenda  has modified her unde rstanding of scale by 
her reference to light seconds and light minutes. She remember s correctly that the Earth is 
1.3 light second s from the Moon and 8 light minutes from the Sun; facts which were 
referred to during the HartRAO vi sit. When questio n e d about th is she demonst r a t e s only a 
limited unders t a n d i n g of the con cept of light seconds and mi nut es , but appears to have the 
basic unders t a n d i n g that they ar e units of distance, as shown in the following exchange. 
Inter v i e w e r : If you say the sun is 8 light minut e s fro m the Earth .  What does 
that mean? 
Brend a :   A light minut e is equal to 300 000 secon d s per minut e I? m not 
sure.  Ja. 
Inter v i e w e r : So what does a light minut e mean?  What do you actua l l y mean 
by that? 
Brenda :   The distan c e betwee n the sun and light becaus e we do not use 
kilomet r e s an d metr es in space.  Ja.  We use light years and light 
minut e s and light secon d s in space . 
Interv i e w e r : Right.  So that? s a distan c e .  It? s actual l y a distan c e .  Okay .  And 
it?s a measur e of, if it?s a light minut e it?s the one min u t e times 
what?  Time s ? 
Bren d a :   Times 3, I think it?s 300 000 . 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  And 300 000 is what? 
Brend a :   Is 1 light secon d .  It? s equal to 1 light secon d . 
(swo 7 0 p o s i n t 103- 11 7 ) 
Togeth e r , I sugges t that these changes in her understanding of  size and scale are examples 
of human constructivist differentiation and superordinate learni ng. Brenda now has a 
signif i c a nt l y chang e d and enhanc e d under s t a ndi n g which const i t ut e s a differ e n t i a t e d 
237 
 
underst a n d i n g of size and scal e. Brenda has also acquired a complete l y new concept 
(supero r d i n a t e learnin g ) of distanc e being measured in light units. This new concept 
includes (or subsumes ) concepts of light a nd time (years, minutes and seconds ) which 
Brend a was previ o u sl y famil i a r with. Altho u g h I am not conv i n c e d that Bren d a can expl a i n 
how her new unders t a n d i n g of light units is a measur e of distan c e , I consider that she 
shows substa n t i a l knowle d g e rest ructuring. The new concept of  light units links to the 
fa mili a r conce p t s of distan c e , light and units of time. 
Brenda? s knowled g e of gravity changed quite su bsta n t i a l l y after the visit. While she still 
referr e d to gravity as ?air th at pulls us down?, she now knew that there is a small amount 
of gravit y in space, on the Moon and a large amount of gravity on Ju pit e r and the Sun. I 
regarde d her learnin g about gravity as a form  of differe n t i a t i o n , where she has learnt (by 
additi o n ) some separa t e facts about gravity, and has modified her conce p t of it as a result . 
However , her true underst a n d i n g of gravity is still quite limite d , as she demons t r a t e s in the 
follow i n g exchan g e : 
Brend a :   Ja Jupit e r has more than enoug h gravi ty .  Ja it?s not suita b l e for 
human s to live in?. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  So what would it be like if we were there ? 
Bren d a :   It would just push us down and up. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  So if we have gr avit y like we have here, the moon has a 
littl e bit of gravi t y and Jupit e r has a lot of gravi ty , what? s gravi ty 
related to?  What causes it? 
Brend a :   I think it? s big and there? s enoug h space for a lot of  gravit y .  You 
can hold up more than enou g h grav i ty . 
Interv i e w e r : Okay .  Does the sun have gravit y ? 
Bren d a :   I think on the atmo s p h e r e of the sun becaus e there ? s a lot of 
gravi ty . 
Interv i e w e r : Right.  Okay .  Does the Ea rth ? s gravity have any affec t on the 
moo n ? 
Bren d a :   I don?t thin k so.  I don? t know . 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  Does the moon? s gr avi t y have any affec t on the Earth ? 
Bren d a :   I don?t thin k so. 
(swo 7 0 p o s i n t 149- 16 5 ) 
Like her under s t a n d i n g of size and scale , her apprec i a t i o n of gravit y has been enhanc e d by 
the experi e nc e at HartRA O , but she still appear s to retai n misco n c e pt i o ns , such as the fact 
that it is related some ho w to an atmosph e r e .  
Regarding stars, Brenda had a mo re scient i fi c under s t a n di n g after the visit in that she now 
knew that their size is simil a r to that of the Sun rather than  a room: ?Some of them might 
238 
 
be big as the sun [some ] might be sma ller? (swo70po s i n t 085). She was not question e d 
about their compo s i t i o n or positi o n but she had referr e d to neutro n stars and red giants in 
her PMM. Although this collective increase in her factual knowledge about stars might be 
consid e r e d an exampl e of differ e n t i a t i o n , I do not  think there is suffi c i e n t evide n c e in the 
data to strong l y sugges t this form of learni ng. As it stands Brenda?s knowledge of stars has 
increa s e d , but they still appea r to remai n separ a t e isolat e d facts, rather than compre h e n s i v e 
modifi c a t i o n of a subsumi n g concep t . In a si milar vein, Brenda? s knowled g e of the Sun 
also increa se d by the additi o n of new fact s. She gave some figure s for the Sun?s 
tempe r a t u r e (rang i n g from 5 millio n to 15 bil lio n degree s Celsiu s ) which she did not know 
in her pre-vi s i t inter v i ew . She also refer r e d to  the presenc e of a sun spot (coole r that the 
rest of the Sun) and the fact that the Sun is not the biggest star, but there are others of a 
simil a r size. Again , I regar d these as addit i o na l incre me n t a l facts that Brend a has learnt 
about the Sun. They may for m the basis  for a changed underst a n d i n g of the concept ?Sun?, 
but currentl y they remain separate facts about it that she has learnt. Brenda?s knowledge of 
day and night and satelli t e s did not change afte r the visit, and I did not ask her about Moon 
phases in her post-vi s i t interv i e w .  
Like most other studen t s in the study Br enda mainly showed examples of addition 
in her knowle d g e const r u c t i o n . She accumul a t e d numerous factual pieces of knowledg e 
over the period betwe e n her pre- and post interv i e w , and it is likel y that the visit to 
HartRAO was princip a l l y respons i b l e for this; only six days elapsed between the visit and 
the interv i ew . In additi o n to the exampl e s of differe n t i a t i o n descri b e d , Brenda 
demons t r a t e d one instan c e of discri mi n a t i o n . In human constr u c t i v i s t terms discri mi n a t i o n 
is where a lear n e r can ident i fy simil a r i t i e s  and differences betw ee n close l y relat e d 
concep t s . Brenda demons t r a t e d this in her PMM, were she stated that the 4 inner planet s 
are compos e d of dust and rock, while the 4 outer ones (excep t for Pluto) are ?made fro m 
ice and gas? (swo70 p o s 12). Brenda did not de mons t r a t e ma ny exampl e s of learni n g in the 
affect i v e domai n : she expres s e d enjoy me n t at  using the rocket s and ?cellp h o n e s ? (the 
whisp e r i n g dishe s ) , as well as wonde r at the size of the Earth and Sun. Finally, Brenda did 
show two examples of learning related to the conative doma i n. She took some pa mp hlets 
which were handed to students at HartRAO and said that she had read them, and that ?I 
think I?ve got a lot of infor ma t i o n about th e galaxies and the stars? (swo70po s i n t 233). 
Signifi c a n t l y , her visit appeare d  to provi d e some confl i c t re garding Brenda?s religious 
belief s and the scienc e she learnt at the centre .  In her pre-vis i t inter v i e w she did not think 
239 
 
there is any relationship between God and sp ace, planets and the un iverse , although she 
believe d that ?He created them? (swo70p r e i n t 35 3). Howeve r , after th e visit she was much 
less certain of her own views in this regar d . She said: 
Brend a :  Since I went to the trip it ? s very comp lic a t e d because from what 
I? ve learnt in HSS [Hu man and Social Scienc e s ] the world starte d 
as a small, tiny little thing and the water, that was writt e n by I 
don?t know who somet h i n g Darw i n , but in the Bible it say s it?s 
Ada m and Eve and then comes the plan e t and all.  I think it?s a lot 
co mp li c a t i n g . 
Interv i e w e r : Hmmm.  So you? re not really sure. 
Brend a :  Ja I?m not sure.  I don?t know which one to belie v e , the Bible or 
evol u t i on or some t h i n g . 
(swo 7 0 p o s i n t 245- 24 9 ) 
This suggests that her trust in the science pres ented at HartRAO, as well as what she has 
learnt at school confli c t s with her religi o u s  belief. Alsop and Watts (1997) include the 
concep t of trust in the conati v e domai n , and I regar d her views as signi fi c a n t in that the 
confl i c t she is expr e s si ng has the poten t i a l fo r further action or consideration on her part. 
Table 8.8 summari s e s how Bre nda?s knowledge was transfor me d between the pre- and 
post-v i s i t inter v i e w s . 
Table 8.8 Frequency of Human Cons tructivism codes for Brenda 
Knowledge 
construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview  
PostPMM & 
Interview 
Pre Interview Post 
Interview 
Totals 
A dd itio n n/a 13 n/a 5 18 
Emergen c e n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Differen tiatio n n/a 0 n/a 2 2 
Discr imin a tio n n/a 1 n/a 0 1 
Recon tex tu a lisa tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Affective 0 0 2 2 4 
Supero rd in ate 
Lear n in g 
0 0 0 1 1 
Conativ e 0 0 0 2 2 
Totals 0 14 2 12 28 
In summa r y Brend a demon s t r a t e d some subst a n t i al lear n i ng from her visi t to Hart RA O . In 
additi o n to the large numbe r of individ u a l fact s she learn t and an  exten s i o n of her 
astro n o my - r e l a t e d vocab u l a r y , Brend a (like Fatima ) showe d examp l e s of both 
differ e n t i a t i o n and discri mi n a t i o n . In huma n cons t r u ct i vi s t terms thes e examp l e s of 
learnin g sugges t greate r knowle d g e restru c t u r i n g , which is likely to result in more long 
term learni n g . Combin e d with her propen s i t y fo r me mor i s i n g facts , Brend a can be said to 
240 
 
have learned very effectively at HartRAO.  She mainly improv e d her ?knowi n g a lot of 
scienc e ? litera c y (Duran t, 1993) rather than showing any change in her cultural science 
literacy (Shen, 1975). Brenda?s interest in the topic appear e d to be mainly confi n e d to 
astrol o g y , which was reinfo r c e d to a limite d extent by the display s at HartRAO , but 
compa r e d with her consi d e r a bl e cogni t i ve le arn i n g , Brend a did not show much in the 
affec t i v e doma i n . She did howev e r , demon s tr a t e conative aspects which, like her 
knowledge restructuring, suggest a deeper engageme nt with the issues presented to her at 
HartRAO. As a student who went to the cent re with some prior knowledg e of big and 
signifi c a n t ideas in astrono my , Bre nda demons t r a t e d an ability not only to extend her 
knowle d g e of these ideas still furthe r , but also to combi ne the increme n t a l additi o nal 
knowle d g e she acquir e d into more s ubstan t i a l knowled g e restruc t u r i n g . 
Both Fatim a and Brend a demon s t r a t e that the wealt h of detai l e d infor ma t i o n 
presente d at the study sites is  accessi b l e enough for some stude n t s to remembe r after their 
visit. In Braund and Reiss? s terms (2004) both Fatima and Brenda engaged with their 
experien c e and deepened their knowledg e a nd understanding of astron o m y concep t s . 
8.1.3  Portrait of Helen (scf11) 
Table 8.9 Position of Helen on Big Ideas classification table 
D 2.6-3.0   Helen 
C 2.1-2.5     
B 1.6-2.0     
A 1.0-1.5     
 Post A B C D 
Pre Mean  1.0-1.5 1.6-2.0 2.1-2.5 2.6-3.0 
Table 8.10 Helen?s knowledge of Big Ideas 
Solar 
Syste m 
Star 
concep t 
Sun 
concep t 
Size/ 
Scale 
Sun 
mov e men t 
Moon 
phase s 
Grav ity Aver ag e 
pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 1 3 3 3 2 3 2.1 2.6 
Helen was chosen as an exampl e of a stude n t in the CD catego r y . Accord i n g to my 
analy si s of her PMM and inter vi e w s , like Neo sh e went on the trip to the Plane t a r i u m with 
above-a v e r a g e knowled g e about Big Ideas but  unlike Neo increased her knowledge more 
substan t i a l l y : her knowled g e leve l of Big Ideas change d from a mean of 2.1 to 2.6. This 
increa s e was sligh t l y more than eithe r of the other two stude n t s in this categ or y . Her pre-
 vi s i t interv i e w s show that she held variou s  misconceptions about the solar system and 
space, some of which change d after the visit, wherea s othe rs did not.  
241 
 
Helen is a 14-year-old white grade 8 st udent and attends L ourdes Girls School, a 
private school in the western suburbs of Johannesburg. Helen lives in a suburb in the 
western part of Johanne s b u r g , approx i ma t e l y 10km from the scho o l , and comes from an 
English - s p e a k i n g family . Academi c a l l y Helen is a high achiever, getting an overall year 
mark of 78% in 2003. 
Helen drew her PMM on 9th Octobe r and was interviewed on 13th October, the 
day before she visited the Johannesburg Planet ar i u m as part of the class visit. On 16th 
October, all the students who ha d been on the field trip did their repeat P MM during school 
time and I re-int e r v i e w e d Helen on the same da y. Helen was looking forward to the visit as 
she had been told by others that ?it?s fun?  and she express e d a moderat e interes t in 
astron o my , findin g ?some of it? inte res t i n g , such as the following extract from her pre-visit 
inter v i e w : 
Helen : That how big the unive r s e is a nd that we don?t even know how far 
it stretch e s .  I mean we hav e only disco ver e d Plut o and there ? s 
gonna be more plane t s after that.  We?ll just have to look more to 
find more thin g s . 
(scf1 1 p r e i n t 254) 
She didn?t recal l any space- r e l a t e d items in th e news recent l y . Helen did not profes s to 
enjoying school greatly, and found that being with her friends was the most appealing part 
of it, as well as writing tests, and receiv ing (good) marks back from her teacher. Her 
favouri t e subjec t s , in which she gets good mark s were Mathema t i c s a nd Account i n g , while 
Biolog y and Englis h were her le ast favour i t e , and she receive d the lowest marks for these 
subjects. It appears that, for Helen, enjoyme n t of a subjec t wa s close l y relat e d to her own 
achie v e me n t in it. She attac he d great impo r t a n c e to her friends, and referred to them on 
several occasio n s , includi n g ?goi ng out? with them as one of her principal two recreational 
activit i e s , the other one being reading fiction . Helen?s career pl ans are to become a lawyer . 
Althou g h Helen knows that extra- t e r r e s t r i a l life has not been discov e r e d , she thinks there is 
the possib i l i t y of life elsewh e r e , but does not believ e in aliens . She unders t a n d s that 
astrol o g e r s use the stars accor d i n g to the time of year to predi c t the futur e , and altho u g h 
she state s that what they predi c t is ?abso lu t e rubbis h ? she does read her horosco p e , and 
thinks they someti me s ?gue ss correctl y ? (scf11pr e i n t 278). Helen conside r s herself 
relig i o u s , and like most studen t s belie v e s th at God created everything, including the solar 
syste m. 
242 
 
In her use of astro n o my words and terms Helen ? s prior knowl e d g e of astro n o my 
was reason a b l e , but not substa n t i a l . While he r use of astro no my - r e l at e d words was below 
the average at 16 in her pre-vis i t PMM (s tudy average was 20, school average 21), her 
post-visit vocabulary rose by 9, which was abov e that of her school (7.6) and the study 
(7.2) . Her person a l meani n g map (Figur e 8.3) s howed a number of interes t i n g concep t s . 
She refer r e d to the phrase ?ligh t year? in her pre-visit PMM which was used by only three 
others students prior to their visit, and although she couldn?t explain why it is called a light 
year, did unders t a n d that it is a measur e of dist an c e . She made refere n c e to distan c e in 
space and the solar syst e m four times in he r PMM, indica t i n g an unders t a n di n g of the 
import a n c e of scale in astron o my . She also st ated several facts about various planets, 
inclu di n g the inter e st i ng obser vation that ?each planet is tilte d at a speci fi c degr ee? 
(scf11pre 32). In the interview, she stated that  this is so that the planets don?t ?bash into 
each other? (scf11prepmm 03) and also result s in the seasons . When probed how this 
causes seaso n s she gave a partia l l y corre c t sc ientif i c explanat i o n regardin g how the sun 
shines on either the northe r n or southe r n hemi sp h e r e . Her underst a n d i n g of gravity was 
simil a r to that of many other stude nt s : she unders t o o d that gravit y is  a pulling force, but 
conside r e d that there is no gravity in space,  on the moon or the sun. Her only refere n c e to 
stars in her PMM was that th ey produce their own light. 
243 
 
 
Figure 8.3 Helen?s PMM (pre and post) 
I n her pre-vi s i t interv i e w Helen demons t r a t e d a number of aspect s of her knowle d g e of 
astron o my . She knew that planet s such as Mars, Jupite r and Saturn are visibl e in the night 
sky, and that they appear like st ars. Her concept of the solar sy ste m was that it is round like 
a plum, and consis t s of the sun, nine planet s and their moons (level  2). Like many other 
stude n t s , she also consi d e r e d that ?the stars ?  are part of the solar system. She had a basic 
unders t a n d i n g of the sun: that it  consist s of gas, and release s heat and light (also level 2). 
When asked to expla i n why the sun moves acros s the sky, she became very confu s e d , and 
could not provide a coheren t  explan a t i o n for the phenome n on. Her initial reasoning was 
that the Earth revolv e s around the sun, but when  provided with a model sun and Earth, she 
found it difficu l t to elabora t e , ex cept to state that the sun move s from West to East across 
the sky, and was theref o r e class i f i e d as level 1. She was however , able to explain that the 
sun and moon look roughly the same size because the moon is a lot closer to us than the 
sun. Her concept of stars was quite well develop e d , that they are mas ses which produc e 
their own light and heat, and that they are very  distan t from us, result i n g in their appare n t 
tiny size (although there are st ars the size of our sun). However, she did have one 
miscon c e p t i o n about stars : she t hought that they might be made  of rock, and did not relate 
them to being simil a r to the sun in co mposi t i o n , so was classif i e d as level 2. 
244 
 
Like many students Helen struggled to explain why the moon shows phases, but 
she did appear to have a scient i f i c id ea (level 3), though not well expresse d : 
Helen: Becaus e, umm, it refl ec t s like the sun? s light and its shadow , 
so me, so me parts are differ e n t , like it revo lv e s around the sun, the 
Earth ? a n d then the sun? s light cover s some parts of the moon 
and somet i m e s other parts like you can see only one part of the 
sun. 
(scf 1 1 p r e i n t 182- 1 86 ) 
Helen? s concep t of distan c e , referr e d to in her pre-vi s i t PMM was ag ain demons t r a t e d in 
her interv i e w , and she guesse d that it might take a spaces h i p about 100 years to reach the 
neares t star outsid e our solar syste m. This indi ca t e s at l east a partial unde rst a n d i n g of the 
scale in space, which is supporte d by her concepts of light year  and that fact that starlight 
takes a long time to reach us  here on Earth (level 3) 
After her visit to the plane t ar i u m, Helen elab or ated on a number of her pre-visi t ideas. In 
Human Const r u c t i v i s t terms her post-v i s i t P MM (see Figure 8.3) showed severa l exampl e s 
of addition , where she listed new knowledg e such as: 
? T h e star Proxi ma Centa u r i being the close st star to the Sun 
? S t a r s as being burning balls of gas 
? S o me planet s having very thick atmo sp h e r e s 
Severa l of these additi o n a l fact s and ideas were elaborat e d u pon in the interview relating to 
her PMM. She did howeve r , discus s severa l idea s in her PMM intervi e w which appear e d to 
show more substan t i a l knowle d g e restru c t u r i n g rather than simple additi on .  The main one 
of these was regardi n g the c oncep t of gravit y . Prior to th e visit she had a very limit e d 
concep t i o n of gravit y : that it was a pullin g force (on Earth), but that there is no gravity in 
space, on the moon or elsewh e r e in the sola r system. Her post-vis i t ideas were very 
different. First, she explained in her PMM and elabo r a t e d in her inter vi e w that the sun is 
held in place by means of the gr avi t y of the plane t s , as well as the sun?s gravity holding the 
planets in orbit. Although I us ed this as an exam ple in section 5.6.1, it is worth repeating 
here : 
Helen :  Okay . The sun doesn ? t stand still .  It moves aroun d .  If you look 
throug h a telesc o p e you can actual l y see it wobble a bit from ti me 
to time.  And becau s e of our gravi t y .  The sun ?  Hold.  The 
sun? s gravi t a t i o n holds us in place , but we also hold the sun in 
place by our, by the means of our gravi t a t i o n a l pull.  And the 
plane t s . 
(scf1 1 p o s p m m 07) 
245 
 
It is highl y likel y that Helen learn t this fr om the visit to the planet a r i u m. The presen t er 
spent some time explain i n g how the sun?s gravit y keeps the planets in  orbit around it, as 
well as the idea that the sun is not stati o n ar y , but that it is moving in  circle s pulled by the 
gravity of the orbiting planets. Secondly, He len change d her ideas about gravity on the 
plane t s in the solar syste m. In her post- v i si t PMM she refer r e d to the plane t s having 
gravity, and in her interview she expanded on  this idea. As she was probe d about the 
concep t , her idea develo p e d , and she realis e d that she ?knew? , at least partia l l y , what 
caused gravit y : 
Interv i e w e r : And ?  Do they have the sa me amoun t of gravi ty or is it diffe r e n t 
on diffe r e n t plane t s ? 
Helen :   They?r e differ e n t . 
Inter v i e w e r :   Okay .  Why is that?  What?s it relat e d to? 
Helen :   I?m not sure. 
Inte r v i e w e r :   Okay .  Like the moon or ? you don? t know ? 
Helen :   The moon is ?  If we wen t like, right here ?  Here ? Umm, we 
?  If, let?s say, I weigh 40kg here [on Earth ], on the moon I?ll 
weig h abou t 10. 
Interv i e w e r :   Okay .   
Helen: Becaus e the gravit a t i o n a l pull is less. 
Interv i e w e r : Okay . 
Helen :   And it pulls you more, yeah, it pulls you in. 
Inter v i e w e r : Okay .  And suppo s i ng you are on Jupit e r ?  Would it be great e r or 
lesser? 
Helen :   Greate r , I think.  The bigger the planet is I think, the mor e gravit y 
you? v e got. 
(scf1 1 p o s p m m 45-6 7 ) 
When asked where she learned what she was expla i ni n g , she admit t e d that she didn? t know 
where, she just ?knew?. What  she explain e d in this seque nce was not covered during the 
visit to the plane t a r i u m. Like some other stud en t s in the study (for exampl e John and to a 
lesser extent Fatima and Nonkulul eko), the visit app eared to remi nd Helen of previous l y -
 learned knowledge, which she coul d now rela t e to the inte r vi e wer, but may not have been 
aware that she ?knew?. This is  emerge n t knowle d g e in human constr u c t i v i s t terms. Furthe r 
her relati ve l y sophis t i c a t e d unders t a n d i n g of th e Sun holdin g the planet s in place as well 
vice versa and additi o nal ideas about gravi t y on the planet s sugge s t s that she has change d 
her own unders t a n di n g of gravit y suffi c i e n t l y  that differentiation has taken place. Table 
8.11 shows Helen?s learning classified in HC terms. 
246 
 
In the struc t u r e d post- v i s i t inter vi e w ,  Helen appeared to have changed her 
unders t a n d i n g of severa l concep t s as a result of the visit. So me of this appear s to be the 
result of a good me mory , for exampl e she reme mbered the figure given by the planetarium 
presen t e r for the time it might take a spaces h i p to  reach the neares t star : 4000 years, as well 
as the names of stars refer r e d to in the prese n t a t i o n : Alpha and Proxi ma Centa u r i . As in her 
PMM, I regard these as exampl e s of additi o n , as they involv e new facts learnt during the 
visit . Howev e r , Hele n also showe d two areas in which her expla n at i o n for a pheno me n o n 
chang e d consi d e r a bl y . The first of these was her explanation of w hy the sun moves across 
the sky. In her pre-vi s i t interv i e w she strugg l e d  to provid e a clear explan a t i o n for this, and 
was at a loss to demons t r a t e using the m ode l of the Earth and Sun, despite probing. 
However, she managed to explain the concept with ease in her post-v i s i t inter vi e w , as 
shown in the follo w i n g trans c r i pt : 
Interv i e w e r :  Okay .  Umm.  Why does the sun move across the sky every day? 
Helen:   It? s becaus e we rotate . 
Interv i e w e r :   Uh-hmm.  Okay .  So, with the model we got here.  If the sun is 
there , what? s the Earth doing ? 
Helen :   The Earth is going this way like a? 
Intervi e w e r : Okay . So what? s actuall y caus i n g the sun to move?  Appar e n t l y 
?  Is it the movi n g roun d of the sun or is it the rotat i n g ? 
Helen :   Rotati n g . 
(scf 1 1 p o s i n t 63-6 5 ) 
This change in her explan a t i o n was not probed durin g the interv i e w , but there are a number 
of possib l e reason s for it:  
? T h e pre-visi t intervie w resulted in her clarif y i n g her own unders t a n d i n g after the 
visit was over; 
? S h e sought out the explana t i o n for hersel f after the pre-visit interview; or 
? T h e visit helped her to clarify her own underst a n d i n g of the phenome n o n or 
remi nde d her of what she had learnt previo u s l y . 
It is not possi b l e to determi n e whic h of thes e appli e d in Helen ? s case, as I did not probe her 
in the second interview regarding her change in explanation. The fi rst two reasons would 
be due to the resea r c h proc e s s itse l f, whil e th e third coul d be claime d be a direct resul t of 
the visit, possib l y an ex amp l e of emerg e n c e . Wh at is clear however, is that her explanation 
for this phenome n o n did change after the first intervi e w . 
The second change in Helen? s explan a t i o n was regardin g the moon phas e s. While 
her pre-visit explanation had some scientific ba si s to it, she strugg l e d  to explain what she 
meant so that it was diffi c u l t to determi n e how well sh e understood the phenome non. 
247 
 
However , her post-v i s i t unders t a n d i n g was made quite explic i t , and was as clear as anyone 
could be without the benef i t of models or drawin g s : 
Helen : Becau s e ?  Umm, the mo on revolv e s aroun d us, the Earth .  And 
the sun?s light ?  Umm how can  I say , the sun? s light ?OK  
You can only see the parts of the moon that ?  Umm, the sun, 
that recei v e s the sunli g h t. The sun? s light.  The shado w part you 
can? t see. 
(scf 1 1 p o s i n t 094) 
As with her clarification of  the sun?s passage across the sky, something intervened to 
improve her explanation, but wh ether it was the visit or some  other intervention prompted 
by the resear c h cannot be determi n e d . 
Table 8.11 Frequency of Human Co nstructivism codes for Helen 
Knowledge 
construction 
category 
PrePMM & 
Interview 
PostPMM & 
Interview 
Pre 
Interview 
Post 
Interview 
Totals 
A dd itio n n/a 7 n/a 4 11 
Emerg e n c e n/a 2 n/a 0 2 
Diff er en tiatio n n/a 3 n/a 2 5 
Disc r imin a tio n n/a 1 n/a 0 1 
Reco n tex tu a lisa tio n n/a 0 n/a 0 0 
Affe c tiv e 0 0 4 3 7 
Cona tiv e 0 2 0 1 3 
Totals 0 15 4 10 29 
From an affec t i v e persp e c t i ve , Helen most  enjoyed the zodiacal constellation pictures, 
when they were superim p o s e d on to the stars,  and the exper i e nc e of the plane t a r i um itsel f . 
She state d : 
Helen : I actua l l y liked the way they made you go insid e and that you 
could actua l l y ?  Like you felt like you could feel the plane t s .  
That they were right there . 
(scf 1 1 p o s i n t 110) 
In other respects however, Helen did not show a stro n g inter e s t in astr ono my eith e r befor e 
her visit or when reflec t i n g on it afterw a r d s . The visit did however prompt Helen to take 
action (the conativ e dimensi o n ) . She used the map handed out at the pl aneta r i u m to look at 
the stars with her fa mily after the visit. She said ?It took me a while to figure it out, after I 
figured it out and it was very nice.  I showed my fa mily how to work out the stars and 
everyt h i n g ? (scf11 p o s i n t line 162). She also report e d that she saw the planet Mars. 
Although not voiced in her interview, this suggest s at least a moderate interest in the topic 
248 
 
of the visit, as relatively fe w studen t s were motiva t e d to use the star charts provid e d by the 
plane t a r i u m. 
Early on in the analysis He len was chosen as an exampl e of a studen t with 
considerable prior knowledge. However, she actual l y showe d a relati v e l y incom p l et e 
understa n d i n g of several astronomy concepts such as the Sun?s appare n t move me n t , 
gravity and star compos ition. A lthough she appeared to be mode rately interested in space 
and astron o m y , Helen? s person a l inter e st in the topic was not  as high as several other 
studen t s , such as Fatima or Botho. Despi t e this, the vis it some h o w made a signifi c a nt 
differ e n c e to her unders t a n d i n g of aspect s of  astro n o my . As well as a numbe r of basic 
?fact s ? that she reme mbe r e d as a result of the visit, such as names of st ars and facts about 
the planet s , Helen seemed to have acquir e d a greater understa n d i n g of (and an ability to 
explai n the move me n t s of) the sun and the mo on and how gravity works within the solar 
system. In human constr u c t i v i s t terms he r increa s e d knowle d g e and unders t a n d i n g by 
process e s of additio n , emergen c e and diffe ren t i a t i o n were built  on a relatively sound 
foundation of prior knowledge which was more s ubsta n t i a l than most other student s . Given 
Helen ? s high academi c achie v e me n t , it is like ly that her eme r gent knowledg e was greater 
than describe d here, as (unlike John) she didn?t voice it. 
8.2 Discussion 
B r e n d a , Fatima and Helen show some si mila r i t i e s in their lear n i ng in that they all 
demonst r a t e d greate r change in their knowle d g e of Big Ideas than the studen t s portra y e d in 
Chapte r 7. While the great majori t y of thei r knowled g e was constru c t e d using the process 
of addition , they also showed examples of differentiation a nd/or discri mi n a t i o n . They all 
appear e d good at rememb e r i n g specif i c facts pr ese n t e d to them durin g the visit , not just 
about Big Ideas, but in other areas relat e d to as tron o my . The y also showed some intere s t in 
the visit, but none had a strong  interest or fascination wi th astrono my . There were also 
some differ e n c e s betwe e n them: while Fatima and Helen showe d examp l e s of emerge n c e 
in which their pre-ex i st i n g experi e nc e of astron o my was recal l e d as a resul t of their visit to 
the scienc e centr e , Brend a did not. Fatima a nd Brenda did not produce very detaile d pre-
 visit PMMs, and they presente d basic facts only. In cont rast Helen?s PMM was mor e 
extensi v e contain i n g referen c e s to more unusual concepts such as light years, distances and 
numerou s facts about planet s .  The main differen c e between the three of them wa s that 
249 
 
Fatima had many miscon c e p t i o n s prior to her vi sit, Brenda had fewer misconc e p t i o n s but 
limited knowle d g e , while Helen had a substan t i a l knowle d g e base.  
Looking across all seven portrait students ,  Brenda and Helen?s (and to a lesser 
extent Fatima?s) depth of knowledge of Big Id eas such as gravity, day and night and the 
Sun has shown greater change than Nonkululeko?s and Botho?s.  This change has resulted 
in limite d but crucia l l y differ e n t learni n g . In additio n to the accumul a t i o n of several 
additi o n a l facts about astron o my both Brenda and Helen have manage d to build on their 
knowl e d g e struc t u r e in limit e d areas , enabl i n g a greater degree of restructuring than was 
demonstrated by Nonkululeko and Botho. Neo and John, although th ey did not change 
their knowled g e of Big Ideas to any great extent, had sufficien t prior knowl e d g e also to be 
able to learn using diffe r e n t i at i o n . While  addition was common to all students , only 
students with sufficient prior know ledge were able to restruct ure their conceptions in the 
relati v e l y compl e x ways demand e d by differ e n t i a t i o n and discri mi n a t i o n . Nonkul u l e k o , 
Fatima and Botho showed the least prior know ledg e of Big Ideas, while Neo, John, Brenda 
and Helen showed conside r a b l y mo re. I consid e r this diffe r e n c e a signif i c a n t one which 
differen t i a t e s students who gain more from a science centre visit from those who show 
more limited knowledg e construc t i o n . Both type s of students learn by the process of 
additi o n , but those who are able to learn by the complex process e s of differe n t i a t i o n (and 
discri mi n a t i o n ) appear to show  greate r learn i n g , and this is depend e n t on the degree of 
prior knowle d g e the studen t posses s e s . Howeve r , indivi d u a l s learn in very differ e n t ways, 
and although prior knowledge pl ays a part, students? own pe rsonal interests and other 
facto r s not examin e d here also influe n c e their learni n g . With  the except i o n of 
Nonkulul e ko , all students had some degree of in ter e s t in the visit , and the conte xt of each 
study site reinfo r c e d their intere s t to some extent . In terms of scien t i f i c  literacy, all students 
improv e d their basic knowle d g e of astron o m y (?know i n g a lot of science ? ) while only 
Fatima (and possib l y Nonkul u l e k o ) showed any improv e m e n t in cultur a l scienc e litera c y 
(Shen, 1975). 
This chapt er has descr i be d in detai l three students in terms of both wha t they knew and 
how they learned about astronomy during their vi sit to a science centr e . It compl e t e s the 
main findings of the study which show the comp lexity and idiosyncratic nature of learning 
during a school visit. Learni n g is such a comple x phenome n o n that althou g h I have been 
able to identif y trends in le arning and relate them to asp ects of students? prior knowledge 
and inter e s t , I have not deter mi n e d strai ght for w a r d relation s h i p s between particul a r 
250 
 
feature s of learne r s (e.g. the comple x i t y of the PMM) and how they lear n. Chapt e r 9 draws 
the thread s of the findin g s presen t e d in Chapte r s 4 to 8 togeth e r , a nd highligh t s issues of 
impor t a n c e ident i fi e d . 
251 
 
Chapter 9 
9  Discussion and Implications 
This chapter provides a summary of my study which draws discussion from the 
preceding chapters together and examine s implications for the fields of 
astronomy education and museum research, for the science centres involved in 
the study and for methodology. It ends w ith a list of recommendations derived 
from the implications and sugge stions for future research. 
9.1  Introduction 
Since Champagne?s article three decades ago (Cha mp a g n e , 1975) there have been 
questio n s regard i n g whethe r people visiti n g scienc e centre s actua l l y learn anythi n g or 
whether they ?just? enjoy themsel v e s . Diffe ring views on learning have been presented, 
from those of Uzzell (1993) and McClaf f e r t y (1995) , who sugges t e d that tradit i o nal 
learnin g of facts does not occu r, to those of Fa lk & Dierkin g (1992) and Anderson & Lucas 
(1997 ) who sugge s t e d that to look for tradi t i o n a l learn i n g is inapp r o p r i a t e for such conte x t s 
and the definition of learning should be broa dene d . Although the latte r view has had more 
influe n c e over the past decade , some scholars still cont est whether learning does occur and 
have propos e d using the term ?outco me s ? instea d of ?learn i n g ? (Ansba c h e r , 1998b, 2002a) . 
The main aim of my study was to determi n e th e extent to which learning occurs during a 
school visit to a science centre. My finding s demons t r a t e eviden c e for learnin g by a 
number of psycho l o g i c a l proce sses in the cognitive, affec t i v e and conati v e doma i n s . 
My thesis has highligh t e d aspects of l earni n g about astro n o my and proces s e s of 
learni n g in museu ms and scien c e centr e s . Disc ussion of my findings is prese n t e d in the 
followi n g sectio n s , in order of impor ta n c e : 
? B i g Ideas in astronomy : how they can be us ed to structur e lear ning the content of 
astron o my in the contex t of a science centre .  
? H o w learnin g occurs during a visit to a scienc e centre from a human constr u c t i vi s t 
persp e c t i v e and the role  of prior knowledge a nd interest in learning . 
? M e t h o d o l o g i c a l Findin g s 
252 
 
? M i s c o n c e p t i o n s in astrono my and how a scien c e centr e can addre s s (or impar t ) 
such miscon c e p t i o ns . 
? S c i e n c e Litera c y . 
? T h e role of vocabula r y in learning . 
In this chapt e r I exami n e each of the above issues and demonstrate how the findings in 
previ o u s chapt e r s help to answe r my resea r c h quest i o n s and illumi n a t e impor t a n t topic s 
and concer ns in the areas of astron om y and informa l learni ng . In the penult i ma t e sectio n , I 
addres s the implic a t i o n s of the findin g s for the science centres involve d in the study and 
for the school curriculum. Fi nally I reflect on my study, bo th from a methodo l o g i c a l and 
persona l viewpo i n t . 
9.2  Big Ideas in astronomy 
9.2.1  Gravity 
As explained in Chapter 5, I used the notion of Big Ideas in astronom y as a way to 
struct u r e how I exa min e d studen t learni n g in a science centre . For a full underst a n d i n g of 
astro n o my , by far the most impor t a n t conce p t for studen t s to learn about is the concep t of 
gravit y (Zeili k , 1994). Gravit y differ s from the ot her Big Ideas in that it is the only concep t 
which is a true theor e t i c a l notio n , rathe r than  a tangib l e objec t (e.g. a star) or a compar a t i v e 
perception (e.g. size and scale). Gravity was addressed differently  in the two study sites: at 
the planetar i u m there were a number of vi deo sequences showing its effects and the 
presen t e r refer r e d to the concept many ti mes.  At HartRAO there were several activities 
which involv e d gravit y , but the educato r s did not  draw as much attent i o n to its import a n c e 
in astronomical processes as was done in the planetarium.  My discussion of how the 
studen t s colle c t i ve l y learn t about gravi t y con centr a t e s on HartRAO as the students visiting 
there were the ones who were questio n e d a bout their knowledge of gravity, while only 
Helen at the planet a ri u m discuss e d gravit y in de tai l , as the resul t of her writi n g about it in 
her PMM.  
I found that all studen t s except Nonkul u l e k o had some idea about gravit y as a pullin g (or in 
some cases they refer r e d to it as a pushi n g ) forc e prior to their visit to the centr e , and that 
they did not signi fi c a nt l y chan g e this idea after the visit . I attri b u t e d this to the fact that the 
centres dealt with the effects of gravit y rather than attempt i n g to provid e a defini t i o n for it. 
What did change after the visit was that as a gro up the student s moved to a more scienti f i c 
253 
 
knowle d g e of gravit y : prior to the visit 93% were  classifi e d as being at knowledg e levels 1 
and 2, where a s after the visit 77% were at levels 2 and 3. Si milar l y , there was a trend 
towards knowing that gravity on Jupiter is high and that the Moon has relatively low 
gravity . However , althoug h there was a trend toward s greate r scient i f i c knowle d g e of 
gravity , few student s after the visit really understood what causes gravity. These findings 
sugges t that by a series of activi t i e s and de mons t r a t i o n s , studen t s ? unders t a n d i n g of gravit y 
can be encouraged to change, but that a more coherent and integrat e d  effort needs to be 
made by the centre if true concep t u a l chang e is to occur. This suggest i o n reinfo r c e s the 
view of Feher (1993) who, in her critique of Borun ? s work on gravi t y misco n c e p t i o n s in a 
museu m (Bor u n et al . 1993) sugges t s that only by using ? networks  of exhibi t s ? (p 247) can 
visit or s make bette r conne c t i o ns for thems e l v e s and ?reor ga n i z e their ideas and const r u ct 
new unders t a n d i n g s ? (p 247). In this respec t , HartRAO is making a good contri b u t i o n to 
this type of learning : it provi des a series of related exhibits  on the theme of gravi t y which 
appear to assist students to make some of the connecti o n s suggested by Feher. However, 
my view is that the exhi b i t s could thems e l v e s  be more concept u a l l y connec t e d with each 
other , and encou r a g e the learne r to relat e (for examp l e ) thei r experie n c e with the rockets to 
the Coke tins and the gravit y scales . In the contex t of visits by school group s , this would 
mean that the HartRA O educa t or s could draw at ten t i o n to the relat e d n e ss of the activ i t i e s 
and encoura g e the student s to themsel v e s form connections. I don? t suggest this would 
guarantee  a greater underst a n d i n g of gravity , as such a comp lex and non-int u i t i v e concept 
is always going to be difficult to comprehe nd. Howeve r , it does give  a greater chance for 
underst a n di n g to take place, especi a l l y if bui lt upon by the teacher on return to school. 
9.2.2  Stars and the Sun 
T h e stars and the Sun were addres s e d in very differ e n t ways in the two study sites: they 
for me d a consta n t backd r o p at the plane t a r i u m, and aspec t s such as names , compo s i t i o n , 
for mat i o n and distan c e were discus s e d . In cont ra s t at HartRA O activi t i e s using the Sun?s 
shado w and image as well as an activi t y relat e d to star size and densi t y were demon s t r a t e d. 
My study found that while student s were able to provi d e a great e r scientific knowledge 
about the Sun after their visit (change from 26% to 50% of st udents at knowledge level 3) 
there was less chang e in the case of stars (c hang e from 6% to 9% at knowle d g e level 3) 
althoug h student s did move away fr om a mini ma l knowle d g e level. To some extent this is 
to be expected, as my criteri a for knowledge level 3 for star s was more rigorous than for 
the Sun, and while both study sites provid e d dem on s t r a t i o n s or activ i t i e s on both, studen t s 
254 
 
are likel y to be able to relat e more easil y to the Sun given its impor t a n c e in daily life in 
compari s o n to stars (Sharp 1996). For student s to gain a full scien tific understanding of 
stars in terms of their compo s i t i o n , posit i o n a nd size is likely to be beyond the scope of 
either the planet a r i u m or HartRA O unless these concep t s are priorit i s e d as being import a n t 
issues for school groups to learn about, which is probabl y not the case. In my analysi s I 
have used stars and the Sun as a Big Idea in astronomy which I consider crucial for 
studen t s to know about. I would sugge s t that the centr e s id ent i f y funda me n t a l knowl e d g e 
about the star-S u n conce p t that they want student s to gain unde rs t a n d i n g of and use this as 
a basis for the devel o pme n t of a web of activ i t i e s relat e d to the concept . In the same way 
that a series of relat e d exhib i t s or activi t i e s on gravi t y appea r e d to chang e stude n t s ? 
knowledg e quite effectiv e l y , so too could a similar theme d set of items on stars and the 
Sun. These might include existing presentati o n s such as star a nd Sun distance and 
appea r a nc e and uses, as well as addit i o na l evidenc e for their simi l a r i t y (sta r s as suns and 
vice versa) and their massiven e s s and the gravity they possess . Again, I strongl y 
recomme n d follow - u p at school by the teacher . 
9.2.3  The Solar System 
Whi l e I ident i fi e d the Solar Syst e m as a Big Idea, both the planetarium and HartRAO used 
it more as an organ i si ng prin c i pl e than as a speci fi c topic with i n thei r displ a y s or as a 
concep t in astro n o my . At the plane t a r i u m the present a t i o n was called the Solar System 
show, while at HartRAO facts about the planet s and the relative distances was presented 
when the Solar System was ?taken for a walk? to demons t r a t e its scale. My specif i c 
quest i o n i n g about the Solar Syste m of the st uden t s who visite d the planet a r i u m was not 
very effec t i ve , and the resul t s  do not demonstr a t e any insights into the eight stud e nt s ? idea s 
of the concep t ?Solar System? . Instea d , mu ch of what students know about the Solar 
Syste m was captu r e d fro m their Perso n a l Meani n g Maps, where the major i t y liste d the 
names of the nine planet s and someti me s some facts associ a t e d with each. In these, 
stude n t s showe d they could reme mb e r a numbe r of addit i o na l facts about the plane t s , such 
as Mercur y ? s proxi mi t y to the Sun, Jupite r ? s size and Pluto?s remotene s s . There is very 
little research published on peopl e?s knowle d g e of the Solar Sy ste m, so compar i s o n of my 
findings with the literature is  not possi b l e . My resul t s ther efore do provide some baseline 
data on childre n ? s knowle d g e of the Solar System to add to the work by Treagus t and 
Smith (1989 ) and Sharp (1996 ) . 
255 
 
As a Big Idea, I would sugges t that the con cept of the Solar System not be confine d 
to factual infor mation about the planets which make up our Solar System, but stress it as a 
princi p l e in cosmol o g y , as was done in the planet a r i u m, that other stars are likely to have 
their own solar systems . This is  relativ e l y recent science whic h is not in the current school 
curric u l u m but is highly releva n t to both the pl ane t a r i u m and the sort of resea r c h carri e d 
out at HartRAO. 
9.2.4  Size and Scale 
I consi de r that if stude n t s leave an as tronomy science centre with only two major 
impre s s i o n s they should have a better under s t a nding of both gravity a nd the size and scale 
of our plane t , the solar syste m and unive r s e .  From a concep t u a l viewpo i n t theref o r e , I 
belie ve that gravi t y and size and scale are the most import a n t Big Ideas relate d to 
astron o my for studen t s aged 12 to 15 years. Both  the study sites incorpor ated size and scale 
into sever al aspec t s of their work, with the ca lculat i o n of the distance  to the near est star 
(outsi de the solar syst em) at the planet a r i u m and the pacing out of the distan c e s betwee n 
the solar syst e m plane t s (at HartR AO ) being th e most promin e n t . My findin g s showe d that 
students? appreciation of aspect s of size and scale change d quite  substantially from the pre- 
to the post-v i s i t . Prior to the visit 80% were at knowle d g e level s 1 and 2, while after the 
visit 53% were at levels 2.5 and 3, sugges t i n g that  the sites were able to  effect not just an 
increas e in knowled g e (such as the relativ e sizes of the Earth, Sun and Moon) but a deeper 
understanding of scale. A clear example of this is where Julius referre d to  the Sun being 
much bigge r than the star s in his pre- v i s i t inte r vi e w , where a s after the visi t he state d ?[th e 
Sun is] much closer than other stars that? s why we see them as if they? r e very small 
becau s e they are much furth e r away from the Earth and the sun is the close st to the Earth 
that?s why we see so bright lights? (swo36posint 030).  
As I discussed in section 5.4.4, there are di fferent views in the literature whether 
students of age 12 to 15 year s are capable of comprehending the massi v e sizes and scale 
involve d in astrono my . Given my finding s th at student s are able to improve their 
unders t a n d i n g of size and scale af ter the visit I would claim th at for students of this age 
group it is appropriate to cover this  concept . Sadler (1998) disputed this, but provided 
limit e d evide n c e in his paper as suppo r t , and examin a t i o n of the test he conduc t e d has 
proved diffi c u l t , as it was never ma de widely  available (Hufnagel, 2002). Like the concept 
of gravity , the approac h by the study sites to provide a variety of e xperiences related to 
256 
 
size and scale is likely to be the most appr opriate way of building students? knowledge and 
underst a n d i n g of size and scale. Such an appr oach is supported by constructivist pedagogy, 
in which new knowledge is mediated by an edu cato r , who provide s a form of scaffol d to 
enabl e learni n g to take place . 
9.2.5  Day and Night 
I n Chapte r 5 I identi f i e d the concep t of day and night as a signif i c a n t idea in astron o my 
which, in contra s t to the Big Ideas, has been extensiv e l y research e d over three decades. At 
both the study sites, the spinnin g of the Earth wa s referr e d to but not ove rtly stressed: at the 
planet a r i u m the star projec t o r speede d up the moveme n t of stars acros s the sky on sever a l 
occasi o n s to demons t r a t e a partic u l a r po int. At HartRA O the spinni n g was briefl y 
discu s s e d when the ima ge of the Sun was projec t e d , and when the sundia l was being 
demons t r a t e d . My findin g s show that there wa s little differe n c e in student s ? underst a n d i n g 
of day and night as being due to  the Earth?s spin from pre- to post-visit, with only two 
studen t s mo ving from knowle d g e le vel 1 to eithe r level 2 or 3. I believe that part of the 
reason for this limite d change was becaus e pr ior to the visit 74% of the student s alrea d y 
underst o o d the cause of day and night. Further , as the demonstr a t i o n s and activiti e s at the 
study sites did not highlight th e Earth?s spin with respect to night and day, students who 
struggled with the concept prior to their visit were un likely to change it as a result of the 
visit. The fact that three studen t s did change  their views may have had more to do with 
emerge n t knowle d g e than knowle d g e at the visit being impar t e d . It is also impor t a n t to 
note that one student (Batsile ) changed from level 3 to level 1; a rare case of Big Idea 
knowle d g e being confus e d by the visit. My findings confirme d those of several other 
resear c h e r s (e.g. Sadler 1998; D ove 2002) that student s of earl y teenage are aware of the 
cause of day and night, and that the most co mmon miscon c e p t i o n is that it is caused by the 
Earth orbiting the Sun. My findings on this Bi g Idea however are more  revea l i n g in terms 
of the use of a model in the resear c h proces s , and the use of terms such as rotate and 
revolve , and these issues are di scussed further in section 9.4.1.1. 
9.2.6  Moon Phases 
L i k e ?day and night? peopl e ? s under s t a n d ing of the phases of the Moon has been 
extensi v e l y resea r c h e d over the past few deca des, with most studies showing that true 
unders t a n d i n g of why they occu r is rare unless specific in struction has been given. 
Although the Moon was discussed at both study sites the pha ses themselves were not 
257 
 
explain e d by the presen t e r s , and I though t it un likely that students would have a differen t 
underst a n d i n g after the visit of why the phases occur. This was borne out by the findings, 
which show that 73% of the st udents (n=34) are at knowledge level 1 and 2 prior to the 
visit, and 74% (n=19) at the same knowle d g e  level after the vis it. Findings by other 
researc h e r s show that people hold a number of misconce p t i o n s about  the Moon phases 
which are persist e n t despit e instru c t i o n , and I discuss some of these misconceptions in 
section 9.5. 
9.2.7  Satellite/Parabolic Dish 
H a r t e b e e s t h o e k Radio Astrono m y Observatory is dominated by the presence of the radio 
telesc o p e , and part of my invest i g a t i o n involv e d whethe r stud ents? knowledge of a 
parabo l i c dish would impro v e as a result of th e visit. At HartRAO , as well as being shown 
how the radio telescope works students were given the opportunity to use whisper dishes 
whose parab o l i c shape works in a simil a r way to the telescope. The only published 
resear c h study using whispe r dishes determi n e d  that many visitors were unable to use their 
prior knowle d g e of reflec t i o n and focuss i n g to  explain how they work (McClaf f e r t y 1995). 
My findings show that while 92 % of students were at knowledge levels 1 and 2 prior to the 
visit, 42% had changed to levels 2.5 and 3 af ter the visit, sugges t i n g some shift in their 
own conceptions. Of the 42%, 23% (5 students)  did need consid e r a b l e probin g for me to 
determi n e the extent of their knowl e d g e about th e dish, and it is possibl e that the student s 
were more conduc i v e to probin g in the interview af te r the visit. They  were now meetin g 
me for the thir d time , and mayb e felt more at  ease to explai n their thinki n g to me . This 
issue is explore d furt her in section 9.4.1.3.  
My sugges t i o n is that HartRA O should take  the opportu n i t y to work more with the 
artefac t of the parabol i c dish so that student s leave the site with a greater underst a n d i n g of 
how it works . Durin g the whisp e r dishe s activi t y , the educa t o r does relat e the shape to the 
same shape of the reflec t o r in  a torch and car headli g ht s, but also brings in the idea 
(joki n g l y to some exten t ) th at stude n t s are using a cell phone. Jokes howeve r , can have a 
detrime n t a l effect on learnin g . Examin i n g lear ning in a planetar i u m, Fisher (1997) found 
that visito r s who saw a humoro u s show scored lower in a post- v i si t test than those who saw 
a non-hu mo r o u s show. The educat o r also provi de s a small sign which explain s how the 
whispe r dish works, as well as pointi n g at the radio telesc o p e and making a referen c e to 
DSTV. Howeve r , I sugges t that additi o na l activities could be provi de d that relat e the 
258 
 
whispe r dishe s to DSTV satell i t e dishe s (w hich all students are fa mili a r with), other 
satel l i t e dishe s (such as those of telec o m m u n i c a t i o n s as seen on the hill when appro a c h i n g 
HartRA O ) , parabo l i c solar cooke rs, as well as the 26 metre telescope itself. Depending on 
the grade level of the children , discussi o n co uld relate the light refl ecte d by a torch 
reflec t o r to sounds reflec t e d by the whisper dis h, electro ma g n e t i c signals of satelli t e dishes, 
heat reflect e d for cooking , to the faint signal s fro m stars. In this way a series of activit i e s 
(like those I have explai n e d for gravit y or  stars and the Sun) could build students 
knowledge of parabolic dishes fr om a variet y of perspe c t i v e s to enable them to leave the 
site with an enhanced knowledg e of dishes. 
9.3  How Learning Occurs 
C h a p t e r 6 explain e d how I used my fra mewo r k of human cons t r u ct i vi s m theo r y to analy se 
my data, and Chapte r s 7 and 8 descri b e d in detail how seven case study students learned 
duri n g thei r visi t . In human cons t r uc t i vi s t th eor y learn i n g takes place by small incre me n t a l 
additio n s of knowled g e by people, and, le ss commonly , by more substant i a l knowledg e 
restr u c t ur i n g (Mint z e s et al . 1997).  
9.3.1  Cognitive Learning 
F r o m a cogni t i ve viewpoi n t and based on previ o u s huma n const r u ct i vi st studi es I used 6 
categor i e s of learnin g : additio n , emerge nce, differentiation, discrimi nation, 
recont e x t u a l i s a t i o n and superor d i na t e lear ning . Table 9.1 shows the total number of 
instanc e s of these categor i e s of  learning as shown by the case study student s . Each instanc e 
of learni n g was record e d in either the studen t ? s PMM or intervi e w , and in a few cases an 
examp l e of learn i n g might be recor de d twi ce . For examp l e Fatima noted on her PMM that 
the closes t stars to our solar syst e m were Al pha and Proxim a Centau r i and that fact was 
coded as addition. Further, she also briefl y discus s e d the same issue in her inter vi e w , and it 
was again coded as addition . However, the table gives an approximate estimation of the 
frequen c y of cogniti v e lear ning categories identified. 
Table 9.1 Summary of cognitive learning by case study students 
Learning 
category 
Nonku
 luleko 
Botho Neo John Fatima Brenda Helen Totals Percen
 tage 
A dd itio n 14 13 14 5 14 18 11 89 68 
Emer g e n c e 2 0 0 12 2 0 2 18 14 
Dif f er en tiatio n 0 1 6 3 0 2 5 17 13 
Disc r imin a tio n 0 0 0 0 2 1 1 4 3 
259 
 
Learning 
category 
Nonku
 luleko 
Botho Neo John Fatima Brenda Helen Totals Percen
 tage 
Reco n tex tu alisat
 io n 
0 0 2 0 0 0 0 2 1.5 
Sup ero rd in ate 
Lear n in g 
0 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0.8 
Total  16 14 22 20 18 21 19 131 ~100 
T h e first featur e that Table 9.1 demons t r a t e s is  that learn i n g at th e study sites was highly 
indivi d ua l i se d and variab l e across the case st udy students, most of whom showe d a range 
of learnin g types across the cogniti v e , aff ect i v e and conativ e domain s . In the cogniti ve 
doma i n addi t i o n refe rs to simpl e acqu i s i t i o n of facts about pheno me n a which a learner 
acqui r e s incre me n t a l l y over a period of time.  In my study, this was by far the most 
common for m of learning at both study sites.  With the except i o n of John, all case study 
studen t s showed from 11 to 18 exampl e s of a ddit i o n as a resul t of thei r visit , and this 
category forms two-thirds of the learning types obs erved across all the case study students. 
The impor t a n c e of the proce s s of addit i o n cannot be undere st i ma t e d . One of the common 
percept i o n s of learnin g in scie nc e cent r e s is that ?real ? lear ning as shown by the acquisition 
of knowle d g e is mini ma l , while other forms of learni n g such as a change in attitu d e or 
feeli n g s is more commo n (e.g. Ansba c h e r , 2002b; Jarvis & Pell, 2005 ). Howev e r , my study 
confir ms what Anders o n (1999) demons t r a t e d in his Austra l i a n study, that the increme n t a l 
learn i n g of numero u s indiv i d u a l facts accoun t s for a substan t i a l propor t i o n of the learnin g 
when a school stude n t visit s a scienc e centr e .  Further , my study suggest s that these facts 
are accumul a t e d as a direc t resul t of the visit ,  witho u t any prepar a t i o n by the teache r or any 
follow-u p activiti e s after return to school . Others (e.g. Braund, 2004) have recommended 
that follow- u p activi t i e s are an importa n t pa rt of the learni n g proce s s , but my study 
sugges t s that learni n g in the form of additi o n will still occur in scienc e cent r e s such as 
those visi t e d as stand- a l one visit s . 
Discounting John?s figures, the second most  common category of learning was 
differ e n t i a t i o n , in which the learne r crea tes a more complex understa n d i n g of a 
phenome n o n by acquiri n g new concept s subsume d  under a more inclu s i v e conce p t . With 
the exception of Nonkululeko and Fatima , all case study stude nts showed at least one 
example of differe n t i a t i o n , with two student s , Neo and Helen, demonstr a t i n g 6 and 5 
insta nc e s respe c t i v e l y . As I expla i ne d in Chapt e r 6 the main diffe r e n c e betwe e n addit i o n 
and diffe r e nt i at i o n is that in the forme r , the f acts are learnt in isol ati o n, and the student 
does not make any conne c t i o n betwe e n them, so they remai n isola t ed addit i o na l facts . 
260 
 
However , in the case of differe n t i a t i o n , the knowl e d g e is combi n e d into a whole resul t i n g 
in the devel o p me n t of under s t a nding of a concept in terms of  the subsu me d parts . Huma n 
const r u c t i vi st resea r c he r s refer s to a categ or y such as (progre s s i v e ) differen t i a t i o n as 
involvin g a greater degree of knowledge restructuri ng, unlike the weak restructuring of 
additi o n . Howeve r , it is the proces s of additi o n which initia l l y allows the develo p m e n t of 
differentiation, as the different pieces of knowle dge first need to be acquired before the 
restru c t ur i n g can take place. My study sugges t s that diffe r e nt i a t i o n is not a very commo n 
for m of learn i n g in the study sites , but it does occur , and this is again impor t a n t for the 
kind of learni n g that takes place in scienc e cen tres. A challenge for them would be to focus 
on encoura g i n g this type of learnin g . Data from my study sugges t s that studen t s can 
substan t i a l l y restru c t u r e their knowle d g e , partic u l a r l y in th e areas of gravity, the Sun, stars 
and the Solar System. 
Emerg e n c e , refer r i n g to recal l of previ o u sl y  learne d knowle d g e from me mory , was the 
catego r y of learni n g with the same percen t a g e frequen c y as di fferentiation, but its 
relativ e l y high frequen c y is due to one stude nt, John, who showed the highest number of 
instan c e s of emerge n c e of a ny students across the study. Disc oun t i n g John, emergen c e is a 
relati v e l y rare for m of learni n g , on a par with  both discrimi n a t i o n and reconte x t u a l i s a t i o n . 
As explain e d in Chapter 6, it is possibl e that some instan c e s of emerge n c e were classi f i e d 
as additio n , as I normall y only coded items as emerge n c e when student s specifi c a l l y 
refer r e d to reme m b e r i n g pheno me n a from previ ous experience. In contrast, Anderson in 
his study of school studen t s learni n g about elect r i c i t y and magne t i s m in a scienc e centre 
(Ander s o n 1999) made conject u r e s when he identifi e d emergenc e , assumi ng tha t the 
knowle d g e was pre-ex i s t i n g in th e student?s memory, and that the activities students were 
undert a k i n g cause d it to be re trieved. Although the frequency of emergence was low in my 
case study studen t s (excep t John), the proces s of  emerge n c e is one which is likely to be 
impor t a n t in museu m learn i n g and needs furt her investi g a t i o n . John is clearly a special 
case. His portrai t in Chapter 7 clearly s hows severa l inst an c e s of how the interv i e w 
question s remi nd hi m of not only what he lear n t in the scien c e centr e, but also how the 
centre exhibi t s remi nd e d him of pr evio u s l y learnt knowledge. For example when he is 
asked about some of wha t he ha s written in his post-vis i t PMM: 
Interviewer: Uh hmmm.  Okay really you? v e writt e n quite a few things here, 
you menti o n e d that the red giant s , the white dwarf s an d the 
Quasa r and you said the trip remin d e d you of those thing s .  Tell 
me a littl e bit about that. 
261 
 
John:   Ja they remin d e d me of so me of the things that I was thinki ng 
about like I rememb e r e d the red giant was huge in size, it has way 
mor e mass th an our size and as the one lady said, she said that our 
sun couldn ? t have a supern o v a becaus e it? s too small co mp ar e d to 
those other stars and then the the white dwarf, it? s small in size 
and it spins faste r . 
(vho 16 p o s p mm 001- 0 0 3 ) 
In this sequenc e , John combine s knowle d g e that  he acquired from the Ha rtRAO (?lady said 
? that our sun couldn?t have a supernov a ? and the experie n c e ?being a star? on the 
turnt a b l e) toget h e r with his prior knowledge about the size of a red giant to explai n why 
the small dense star spins faster  than the larger star. What is  partic u l a r l y in teresting about 
John is that he is both knowled g e a b l e and arti culate enough to explain how he used his 
prior knowle d g e and what he had learnt at HartRAO to explain a new phenome n o n .  
Discri mi n a t i o n , where stude n t s ident i f i e d simil a rities and differences between associated 
concepts, was rarely shown by students in my study. However, all students who showed 
discri mi n a t i o n were studen t s who demons t r a t e d  improv e me n t in their learni n g or were at 
level D in their pre- and post-vi s i t analysi s  of Big Ideas (Table 7.1). These studen t s 
include d Fatima, Helen and Brenda, as well as  Paul and Richar d . The princi p a l way in 
which studen t s showed discri mi n a t i o n was by specif y i n g simil a r i t i e s and differ e n c e s 
betwee n the planet s in the solar syst em, for exampl e Fatima stated this in her post-v i s i t 
inter v i e w : 
Fatima: The astro n o m e r at the Plane t a r i u m said Mercu r y , Venus and 
Mars, they all have one thing in common becaus e they are more 
solid , and then Jupit e r , Satur n and Uranu s and Neptu n e , they are 
mor e gaseou s and then Pluto they ? r e not so sure becau s e it? s like 
the odd one out.  
(scf1 5 p o s p m m 11) 
The other main way in which discri mi n a t i o n wa s demons t r a t e d was by studen t s noting the 
differ e n c e betwee n ?livin g ? and ?dead? stars when these were discussed at HartRAO during 
the turnta b l e activi t y . Althou g h I have identi f i e d discrimi nation as a relatively uncommon 
for m of knowled g e restru c t u r i n g it is (like di fferen t i a t i o n ) likely to be an importan t type. 
Huma n constr u c t i v i s t resear c h sugges t s that  greater knowled g e rest ru c t u r i n g result s in 
conce p t s being retai n e d in the long- t er m me mor y (Pear s al l et al . , 1997). 
Recon t e x t u al i s at i o n is the proces s in which a student modifies thei r understanding of a 
concept as the result of a vi sit, but does not improve on their knowledge or understanding 
in the process (Anderson 1999). In my study it  occurred relatively uncommonly and, of the 
262 
 
case study studen t s , only Neo showed exampl e s of it. Of the other parti c i p a n t s in my study, 
recontex t u a l i s a t i o n mainly occurred in the A and B Big Idea categor i e s . 
Reconte x t u a l i s a t i o n , althoug h not me rely the addition of new fact s, does not appear to be a 
strong form of knowledge restructuring in  the same way that differentiation and 
discrimi n a t i o n are. Anderson (1999), although he id enti f i e d it in his st udy, sugges t e d that it 
could be argued that it is mere ly a form of differ e n t i a t i o n , an d not a separate category of its 
own. However , I do find it a useful catego r y in th at it ident i fi e s when stude n t s are thin k i n g 
about a conce p t in the conte x t of the visit , but not making any substan t i a l change to their 
knowle d g e of that concept . For example Ntobe ko knew in the pre-vis i t intervi e w that a 
satel l i t e dish point s to a satel l i t e in space . Howeve r , after visiti n g HartRA O and seeing the 
radio telesc o p e she was less sure about where a DSTV dish points t o, she thought it might 
be ?space and stars?, or to a spaceshi p ? she wa sn?t reall y sure. This is an example of how 
the visit has caused some confusion in the student?s mind about some thing they did know 
beforeha n d , and are now less certain. This sort  of concept u a l confus i o n is discuss e d in 
some detail in section 9.5. 
Among the portra i t studen t s supero r d i n a t e learni n g was demonst r a t e d only by Brenda 
(Table 9.1), and I found only two example s of it in other 27 students (section 6.3.6), 
sugge s t i n g it is the least co mmon form of cogniti v e learnin g in the study. Anderso n did not 
use a categor y of superor d i n a t e learnin g in his study, and my findings suggest that it is not 
easily demo n s t r a t e d in an informa l learn i n g enviro n me n t . Supero r d i n a t e learni n g involv e s 
a significant and rapid shift in understanding of  a new and inclus i v e co ncep t , and this is 
diffi c u l t to achie v e in a museu m durin g a short visit (Renn i e , 2001; Welli n g t o n , 1990) .  
One of the implic a t i o n s for scien c e centr e s which emerg e s from my study is that 
while additio n is likely to be the domi nan t for m of cognitive learni n g , centre s should make 
effor t s also to promo t e lear nin g which involve s more substa ntial knowledge restructuring. 
Althoug h my study does not make any claims for long-term learning, other research 
suggests that significant knowle dge restructuring is likely to promo t e more perma n e n t 
learni n g in the long-te r m me mory (Pears a l l et al. , 1997). 
 
9.3.2  Affective and Conative Learning 
A l l studen t s in the study showed severa l ex ampl e s of affect i v e learni n g (Table 9.2). 
The main types of affect not ed in the case study students were enjoyment of (or wonder 
263 
 
about) differ e n t aspect s of their visit, specif i c aspect s of the trip that they found person a l l y 
impo r t a n t to them, and issue s relat ed to as trono m y in the infor ma l learni n g enviro n me n t 
(such as television news) that they had noticed .  
Table 9.2 Summary of affective and conati ve learning by case study students 
Learning 
category 
Nonku
 luleko 
Botho Neo John Fatima Brenda Helen Totals 
Affective 9 5 9 10 8 4 7 52 
Conative 0 1 1 1 1 2 3 9 
Totals 9 6 10 11 9 6 10 61 
Many schola r s have sugges t e d that the affect i v e doma i n is of great im por ta n c e in learnin g 
(Alsop and Watts 2003), but relatively few st udi e s have exami n e d it in the infor ma l 
learning environment, and those that have ma inl y invol v e d gene r al muse u m visi t o r s and 
fa mily learning rather than school groups (D ierking 2005). Dierking suggests that (with 
respe ct to learn i n g ) the affec t i v e doma i n compr i s e s motiv a t i o n a l , emoti o n a l and perso n a l 
satisf a c t i o n , elemen t s which corres p o n d to so me of the affect i v e issues I examin e d . 
Table 9.3 and Table 9.4 show the activi t i e s and demons t r ations at the study sites 
which students enjoyed most. At HartRAO the three most popular activities were the water 
rocket s , the star turnta b l e and the whispe r dishes . It is impor t a nt to stress that these were 
activities, which probab l y accoun t s for their popula r i t y , and the findin g suppor t s the notion 
of stude n t s wanti n g to have fun durin g their visit . The findi n g s on cogni t i ve and affec t i v e 
learni n g sugges t that HartRA O has achie ved a successful balance between the 
entert a i n me n t and educat i o n aspec t s of their sc hool visits, where stude nts have had fun and 
learnt at the same time. At the plane t a r i u m the most enjoyable presentations were those 
involv i n g the stars. 
Table 9.3 Activities at HartRAO whic h resulted in affective learning 
Activity/presentation at HartRA O  Enjoy Germane Wonder Total 
R o c k e t s 15 4  19 
Star turn t a b l e 4 3 2 9 
Whisp e r dish 5 2 1 8 
Planet facts, Solar Sy ste m for a walk 3 3  6 
Slide show 4 2  6 
Radio Tele s c o p e 3 2  5 
Mars & Mars show 2 2  4 
264 
 
Activity/presentation at HartRA O  Enjoy Germane Wonder Total 
S i z e and scal e   4 4 
Star descr i p t i o n s 4   4 
Sun fact s , Sun dial 1 3  4 
Coke cans and scale s 2 1  3 
Satel l i t e lase r 1 1  2 
Lectu r e r s nice 1   1 
Telesc o p e s 1   1 
 
Table 9.4 Presentations at the planetariu m which resulted in affective learning 
Presentation at planetarium  Enjoy Germane  Total 
S t a r s in nigh t sky 3  3 
Stars spin n i ng 3 1 4 
Const e l l a t i o n pictu r e s & name s 2 1 3 
Dust cloud phot o s & forma t i o n of SS 2  2 
Mars phot o s 1  1 
?How much is out there ? 1  1 
More plane t s  1 1 
No studen t s expres s e d rema rk s classi f i e d as  ?wonder ? . The data in this table include s 
comme n t s from stude n t s at  St. Augustine School. 
 
Part of the impor t a n c e of the entert a i n i n g activ i t i e s is that they can be relat e d to situat i o n al 
inter e s t . Situa t i o n al intere s t is the abil i t y of th e conte x t (?sit u a t i o n ? ) to devel o p in peopl e an 
inter e s t in theme s being prese n t e d (Hidi & Harac h i e w i c z , 2000) . While situat i o n al inter e st 
can occur in any educa t i o n a l envir o nme n t , it ha s been studi e d mainl y in the classr o om and 
less in museu ms (Dier k i n g , 2005) . Limit e d resea r c h suggest s that student interes t in the 
topic of astron o my might be relatively high (Jarma n & McAl ee s e , 1996). While a scienc e 
centr e cannot predi c t what degre e of perso n a l inter e st each stude n t will enter with, it can 
aim to impar t situa t i o n a l inter e st to its visit o r s . If it is succe s s fu l in doing so, this may in 
turn raise stude n t motiv a t i o n for the topic , and lead to great e r achie v eme n t in scien c e , a 
desperat e need in developi n g c ountries such as South Africa.  It appears that hands-on 
acti vi t i e s are effec t i v e ?tri g g e r s? of situa t i o n a l interest for many students who visited 
HartR A O . Furth e r rese ar c h could aim to exami n e such trigg e r s in detai l , and determi n e 
what can be done to maintai n such  interes t to promote motiva t i o n .  
265 
 
The case study students showed few exampl es  of learnin g in th e conativ e domain. 
However , the conativ e learnin g shown by Both o and Helen, where they actively followed 
up issue s discu s s e d at the study site are furth e r areas which relat e to inter es t and 
motivati o n , and should be develope d by the science centres. Si milar l y , the conflict 
experien c e d by Brenda between the science pr esented at HartRAO and her own religious 
belief s has potenti a l for further action . The pl ane t a r i u m direc t o r once remar k e d that, durin g 
a presen t a t i o n , if studen t s ask her ?where is  God in all this?? she does not answer the 
questio n direct l y (C. Flanaga n pers. comm . ) . Inste a d , such a questi o n might be an 
oppor t u n i t y to spark some addit i o n a l think i n g on the part of the student , to encour a ge 
debat e after the show. In thes e ways the conative aspect of learnin g can be encoura g e d , as 
Braund and Reiss?s defini t i o n of learni n g (2004 ) inclu de s aspec t s of the conat i v e 
dime nsi o n , such as ?an increas e  in the capacity to reflect? a nd ?the desire to learn more? 
(p.5) . 
While previ o u s studi es exami n i n g th e relationship between education and 
enter t a i n me n t have taken diffe r e n t posit i o ns (as I elabor a t e d in secti on 2.5), the findi n g s 
from my study sugge s t that they are entir e l y  compat i b l e , confir mi n g the conclus i o n s of 
some researc h e r s (e.g. Brunell o , 1992; Falk et al ., 1998). 
9.4  Methodological Findings 
M y study shows a number of findings relate d to the method s which are import a n t to 
highlig h t as part of this chapter . 
9.4.1  Issues in Interviews 
9.4.1.1  Models 
I n Chapte r 5 I descri b e d how, when I ques ti o n e d studen t s about day and night in the 
interv i e w , I introd u c e d a model of the Sun (a  torch) and the Earth (a small globe) and 
asked them to explai n their initia l thinki n g about  why the Sun moves across the sky using 
the model. I found that nearly one third of the student s changed thei r (initia l l y rather 
confused) explanation of the cause of day and night as soon as they were refer r ed to the 
model . With the model as a ?prop? these stud e n t s were able to give a more scien t i fi c 
explana t i o n for the phenome n o n , which they were  unable to do without it. This is highly 
signi f i c a nt for other resea r c h st udies in the area of astronom y , as the subject involves an 
understa n d i n g of relative l y co mplex three-d i me n s i o n a l inte ractions between bodies. My 
findi n g s sugge s t that if quest i o n s invol v i n g a ppare n t or relativ e motion are asked of people 
266 
 
either by quest i o n na i r e or inter v i e w in th e absen c e of a suppo r t i n g mo del , the resul t i ng 
answe r s may relat e to their me mor y of a diag ram in a book or at a le ctur e . In the case of 
?day and night?, responde n t s of an appropri a t e age (probab l y 10 years and older) need to 
decide whethe r it is the Earth orbiti n g the Sun or the Earth spinning on its axis which 
causes day and night. Instead of engaging with the questi o n in a meanin g f u l way, 
respon d e n t s in my study began by answer i n g what  appear e d to be the first idea that came 
into their mind, which in 13 cases out of 20 was that the Earth re volves around the Sun 
(altho u g h their use of langua g e was someti me s unclear ? see section 9.4.1.2). Only when 
the model was presen t e d did they then sugges t  that day and night are due to the Earth? s 
spin or rotat i o n rathe r than its orbit . The f act that only 3 of the 16 peer-rev i e w e d research 
studie s of day and night used mode ls as part  of the data collection process suggests that 
their data may be biased towards their res pondents? ?first thought s? which may well be 
erroneous. Wagenschein?s notion of synthetic stupidity (Engestr?m 1991) which I 
intro d uc e d in Chapt e r 2 is releva n t here. In Wa gen s c h e i n? s premi s e the repre se n t a t i o n s of 
the Earth and Sun that students learn about in the class r o o m ar e comple t e l y unrela t e d to the 
Earth and Sun which they experie n c e in everyda y life. Engest r ? m argues his case for 
students ? represen t a t i o n of the Moon phases, but I consider it ap pl ic a b l e to the case of day 
and night too. In respo n s e to my quest i o n , instead of thinking through a response which 
makes sense in terms of a 24-hou r cycle of ni ght and day, students think back to an image 
of the Earth orbiting the Sun, and res pon d with ?empty senten c e s ? (Enges t r ? m 1991, p 
155) about the Earth ?revol ving around the Sun?.  
The clear implic a t i o n this has for studie s of astron o my learni n g is that resear c h e r s 
need to ensur e that they are colle ct i ng data that is salie nt for the parti c i p ant s rathe r than the 
first thoug h t s that enter their minds . This impli c a t i o n would mean that such studies need to 
involve not just student s res ponding verbally, in writing or  by drawing a diagram but by 
talkin g throug h their thinki n g with  the help of a model. Thi s is what Schoult z sugges t s in 
his arti cl e (Sch o u l t z et al . 2001) critical of the methods of  Vosniad o u and collabo r a t o r s , 
though he also critique s the nature of the interview itself. In her rebuttal, Vosniadou 
main t a i n s that ther e is a place for quest i o ni n g in the absen c e of an artefact such as a globe 
(Vosni a d o u et al . 2005), to investigate children?s thinki ng in the development of their own 
menta l model s . While this might be relev a n t fo r the inves t i ga t i o n of me nta l model s in the 
young childre n (ages 6 to 11) Vosniad o u and Schoult z were working with, by the time 
children reach age 12 it is hi ghly likely they have a more  sophisticated view of basic 
267 
 
astrono my concep t s (Roald and Mikalse n  2001, Trumpe r 2001a) . Using models is 
theref o r e likel y to be  the most appropr i a t e way of e ngender i n g response s to question s 
posed by researc h e r s , to avoid th e dangers of synthetic stupidity. 
9.4.1.2  Language 
I explained in Chapter 3 that my research di d not use theori e s around language to provide a 
concept u a l fra mewo r k for the study. However ,  during the process of data collection I 
found that in some cases student s ? inabili t y to commun i c a t e effect i v e l y in English meant 
that they were not able to e xpre s s thems e l v e s as fully as they might have done in their 
home langua g e . Where possibl e I gave them the chance to express themse l v e s in the 
langu a g e they were mo st comfo r t a b l e with (when I was accompa n i e d by an interpr e t e r ) , 
but none availed themsel v e s of this opportu n i ty. Indeed I found on o ccasion that students 
were unable to use common seTswana words fo r some of the astronom i c a l phenome n a we 
were discussing 17 , and were more comfor t a b l e using Englis h .  
There was one aspect of languag e , whic h was common to students across all 
langua g e s and all school s , and that was the use of the terms for orbit and spin. In Chapte r 5 
I discus s e d the diffic u l t y many studen t s had in expres s i n g themse l v e s particularly the use 
of ter ms such as rotate and revolve to descri b e the passag e of the Earth around the Sun and 
the turning of the Earth to cause day and night. In 1998, Parker and Heywood noted that 
?the recog n i t i o n of this probl e m is consp i c u o u s  by its abse nce in re sear c h litera t u r e ? (1998, 
p 516). I consid e r that this is still the case, as althoug h some instr u c t i o n a l mater i a l s 1 8  
recogn i s e the issue and attempt to assist stude n t s in their under s t a n d i n g only Camer o n 
(2003) and briefly Stahly et al. (1999) have referre d to it in a resear c h contex t . If Parker 
and Heywoo d found it to be a proble m among English trainee teachers one can i magine 
that the issue is likel y to be great e r for teen age students in South Afri c a , many of whom are 
learning in another language. The issue has si gnif i c a n t impli c a t i o ns for teache r s as well as 
the educators at the planeta r i u m and HartRAO , as  they need to use strat eg i e s to clari fy the 
terms for studen t s . My recomm e n d a t i o n would be that only the terms orbit  and spin should 
be used, so that the simila r - s o u n d i n g revolve and rotate are avoide d in the contex t of 
                                                 
 
17  I have a basic know ledg e of seTsw an a and chec k ed stud e n ts ? know ledg e of words such as ?moon? and 
?star ? if their home lang u ag e was seTs w an a . 
18  e.g. the Natio nal Cen ter for Math ema tics and Scie n c e at the Univ e r s ity of Wisco ns in -Mad is on and the 
Capital Region on Scien c e Educ a tio n Partn ers h ip See the Mode ling for Unde r s tan d in g in Scien c e Educ a tio n 
course at www.wcer .wisc.e du/N CI S LA /MUS E /in d ex .h tm and 
www. cr sep .o rg/Per p lex ing Pair s/An o th erPerplex in gPair . Ro tatio n and Rev o lu tio n12 110 2.pd f 
268 
 
teachi n g about astron om y . Parker and Heyw ood (1998) found however, that even when 
this strateg y is adopted the teacher s in  their study still conf used the two terms 
concep t u a l l y. Clearl y , more effect i ve techni qu e s would need to be implemen t e d , and I 
have referre d to two helpful program m e s in footnot e 18. 
The issue of confusion of terms however , also has signifi cant implications for 
researchers into people?s understanding of astr ono my concep t s . Resear c h e r s who choose to 
use only questio n n a i r e s or intervi e w s for determi n i n g peopl e ? s conce p t i o n s of the 
processes of rotation and revolution, without a model present , are likel y to gather 
unreliable data, and mu ch of the data already coll e ct e d and inter p re t e d over the past thre e 
decade s may be partly flawed . A critica l revi ew of the studies conduc t e d is therefo r e a 
strong recomm e n d a t i o n from this thesis. 
9.4.1.3  The interview as a situated event 
In Chapter 3 I explained how I planned and ex ecu t e d struct u r e d interv i e w s with studen t s 
during my data collec t i o n , and in  Chapt e r s 5 to 8 I descri b e d the finding s emanat i n g from 
the intervi e w s . Althou g h I consid e r I adopt e d the ri ght strategy for my data collec t i o n , my 
analys i s shows that the struct u r e d nature of the inter vi e w s was somew h a t const r ai n i n g, and 
in retros p e c t I consid er that  I could have been less struct u r e d and more probing in most 
cases. This would have made some of the da ta more fruitf u l for deeper analys i s , and 
probab l y enable d some discou r s e analys i s in ad dit i o n to the conte n t an alys i s I carrie d out.  
I also need to recogn i s e that my interv i e w s  were a situate d even t in the sense used 
by Schoul t z and collea g u e s (Schou l t z et al . 2001). The setting for intervie w was at the 
school , in an office or classr o o m, despit e the fa ct that I was examin i n g infor ma l or out-of -
 s c h o o l learnin g . I, as the intervi e w e r , was th e domi nant party who acted like a teacher, 
along with my intell e c t u a l and cultural capital. Further, I wa s askin g quest i o n s , some of 
which were possibly abstract and difficult, adding further to the imbalance in power 
relat i o n s in the inter v i e w situa t i o n. For these reasons, Schoultz and colleagu e s would 
argue, the interv i e w was not an  objective method of enquiring into the intervi e w e e ? s mind 
to probe their knowled g e of astrono m y concept s , but ?simp l y anoth e r so cial practice, and a 
highly problema t i c one as well? (2001, p 116) . However, when conducting the interviews, 
I consi d er I ameli o r a t e d some of the possi b l e conte x t u al l y alien aspects of the situa t i o n , 
and was closer to the type of intervie w conduc t e d by Schoult z and colleag u e s than the 
interv i e w s of Vosnia d o u that they critiq ue . I did not start by firing a barrage of questions 
on abstract astronomy at the students , but asked about the forthcomi n g (o r recent) visit, in 
269 
 
order to set the scene for the inter vi e w . As in the case of Schou l t z , the globe was on the 
table at the start of the inter v i e w , and I in tro d uc e d this mo del into the quest i o ni n g at an 
early stage, which I hoped provide d a further ?prop? for students to assist with their 
answer s . I also used the student s ? own product i o n s , their previou s l y - d r a w n Persona l 
Meani n g Map as an additi o na l suppo r t which provi d e d a focus for quest i o n i n g later in the 
intervi e w . I therefo r e acknow l e d g e the nature  of the intervi e w proces s , and althoug h I 
interp r e t the result s from a cognit i v e viewpo i nt , like Candel a (2001) I also accept the 
socioc ul t u ral cont e x t of the interact i o n whic h gave rise to them. Further, the use of 
intervie w s allowed students to ?hear? what  they thems e l v e s were sayin g , as in all 
inter v i e w s I repeat e d at least some of their words after they utter e d them, or repea t ed the 
idea they were talkin g about. This allowe d them to reflec t on what they had just said, and 
in severa l cases this caused studen t s to ch ange their explan a t i o n . Although it is equally 
possib l e for studen t s to read over what they have writt e n in a questi on n a i r e , there is no 
?othe r voic e? (the inte r vi e w e r ) to repea t thei r ideas , and facil i t a t e th e process. This would 
appear to be a clear advant a g e of intervie w s over question n a i res for encouragi ng refl ect i ve 
thinkin g while respond i ng to questions, again di scou r a g i n g Wagens c h e i n ? s empty 
sentences (Enges t r ? m, 1991) . 
My final comment on the intervi e w process is th at in retrospe c t I consider I should have 
interv i e w e d the educat ors at HartRA O and the pres e n t e r at the plane t a r i u m , in order to gain 
an unders t a n d i n g of their persp e c t i v e of what they were tr ying to get across during the 
visits by the school groups. Although my observa tions made at the centres were recorded, 
my inter pr e t a t i o n s of what the prese nt e r s were  doing may be quite different from their own 
intentions. A future study of this sort should follow met hods recommended by researchers 
such as Cox- Pet e r s e n et al. (2003), where not only students  and teachers are intervie w e d , 
but also the museu m staff . In this way a cleare r contex t u a l bac kground could be obtained 
for the study, which would provid e a rich er descript i o n of the overall findings . 
9.4.2  Personal Meaning Mapping as a technique for data collection 
As explained in Chapter 3, I chose to use P MM as a techn i q u e to compleme n t my other 
data collect i o n method of structu r e d intervi e w s . The structu r e d interv i e w s focuse d on the 
Big Ideas in astronomy , and could be regarded as  a for m of ?pre- and post-test? related to a 
tradi t i o na l expec t a t i o n of cogni t i v e learn i n g . In  contrast , the method of PMM creates less 
expec t a t i o n of what the learne r should know, in that there are no questions to respond to, 
270 
 
and is a less ?threatening? fo rm of assess me n t . Devel o p e d by John Falk in the late 1990s, 
PMM is analo g o u s to conce p t mappi n g , but is more suita b l e to the muse u m envir o n me n t , 
and require s no prepara t i o n on the part of th e partic i p a n t s (Adelma n and Falk 2000, Falk 
2003). From this point of view PM M is an ex treme l y releva n t method for use in a study 
based on a constr u c t i v i s t episte mol o g y , as it allows the partic ipant complete freedom in 
writi n g what they like on the map, and cons tructing their knowledge in their own way. 
Falk (2003) recomme n d s a partic ul a r method of analysi n g PMMs, which involve s looking 
acros s four dimens i o n s of learni n g : exte nt , breadt h , depth, and mas t ery . While Falk 
suggests that PMMs can be analysed both qua nti t a t i ve l y and qualit a t i v e l y , most of the 
studie s in which they have been used have been domi na t e d by quan ti t a t i v e tec hniques (e.g. 
Adelma n et al . , 2000; Falk & Storksd i e c k , 2005; Falk et al ., 1998). My study being 
qualit a t i v e in nature, implie d that I forego extensi v e quantit a t i v e analysi s , and make 
indivi d u a l learn e r s the units of analysi s . In this respect the personal meaning maps and 
accompa n y i n g interv i e ws were very helpful , as th ey provide d details of the sort of learning 
not captur e d in my struct u r e d  interv i e w s . In additi o n , alth ough not analysed for all the 
dimensio n s suggeste d by Falk, I was able to us e the PMMs to assist with some descriptive 
stati s t i c a l data, such as the numbe r of astrono mi c a l vocabul a r y words ( extent  in Falk?s 
termi n o l o g y ) used by each parti c i pa n t in th e study. I found no partic u l a r drawba c k s in 
using PMMs as one of my data collecti o n methods, but I have the followin g 
recomme n d a t i o n s for their use by future resear c h e r s :  
? W h e r e possib l e , spend adequa t e time in pre li mi n a r y analy si s of the PMM prior to the 
initi a l inter vi e w . Simil a rl y , spend adequ a t e time in anal y si s of the PMM befor e the 
second round of data collec t i o n , and prior to the second intervi e w . Unfortu n a t e l y , in the 
sequence of data collecti o n i nvol v e d in museu m visit s , this  is not always possibl e .  
? I n a pilot study, experi me n t with the two alternatives of handing the original PMM 
back to the parti ci p a n t s for addit i on / c o r r e c t i o n and aski ng them to complet e a new 
PMM. Falk strong l y recomme n d s the for mer for museum vi sito r s , for ethica l reason s 
due to their time constr a i n t s and the inconv e n i e n c e they are being put to. For school 
groups time is less of an issue and they are ?used to? formal and informa l testin g as part 
of their school work. A compari s o n betwee n these two altern a t i v e s  might therefor e be 
of value. 
? I t would be worth doing some addit i o na l ?t esti n g ? of the techni q u e as part of the 
piloting . For example, if a P MM is given to a group of pe ople, who then repeat the 
271 
 
proce d u re so me time later , with no inter v e nt i on between the two processes, is there any 
differe n c e between what people complet e on the PMM? To what extent does the very 
act of comp le t i n g a PMM result in possib l e changes in people?s thinking about the 
topic? 
? I t would also be worth resear c h i n g the techni q u e as a possible teachi n g tool in a simi la r 
way to how concept maps are used in the classro o m (McClur e et al ., 1999). 
Advant a g e s of Person a l Meanin g Mappin g are that the techni q u e doe s not have to be 
taught , that it is congru e n t w ith constr u c t i v i s t pedago g y , and th at its infor ma l natu r e is 
less intru si ve in the class r o o m. 
 
9.5  Misconceptions 
Chapter 2 showed that many studies conducte d on learning over the past three decades 
have confirme d that people hold numerou s  misconc e p t i o n s about various aspects of 
astron o my . Some of the studie s identi f i e d th e misconc e p t i o n s only, while others showed 
that despite some for m of interve n t i o n , many of the misconc e p t i o n s remain e d persis t e n t . 
My study did not set out to ident i f y and catal ogu e misconce p t i o n s , but it does show that 
most of the stude n t s  hold misco n c e p t i o ns simil a r to those identified by previous 
resear c h e r s . Howeve r , I do not wish to discus s my finding s in the light of researc h e r s such 
as Vosniad o u and Brewer (1994) and Roald and Mikalse n (2001) who investi g a t e d 
misco n c e p t i o n s as the princi p a l objec t of the resea r c h . Neithe r do I think it appro p ri at e to 
use researchers who looked at a ?before an d after instruction? scenar i o (Parke r & 
Heywoo d , 1998; Trundl e et al. , 2002) as a basis for discuss i o n . All these studie s , as well as 
Minda Borun? s study of grav i t y in the museu m (Boru n et al . , 1993) appear to be based on 
the fact that miscon c e p t i o n s are a faulty form of scientific knowledge which needs to be 
rectif i e d . Instea d , I take  Jeremy Roschell e ? s view that all prior knowledge (including the 
miscon c e p t i o n s ) for ms a basis for the constr u c t i o n of scienti f i c knowled g e , and that we 
need to find ways of incorpor a t i n g the misconce p t i o n s into the scienc e rathe r than trying to 
eradic a t e the ?fau lty? notions (Roschel l e , 1995; Smith, diSessa & Roschel l e , 1993). This 
view is consis t e nt with my concep t u a l fram ewo r k of human constru c t i v i s m, where I have 
attempt e d to demonst r a t e how student s build thei r knowled g e , even if so me of the resulta n t 
cons t r u ct i o n is not scie nt i fi c al l y corre c t . I th ere f o r e prese n t two exam ples of how students 
built on their prior knowl e d g e to const r u ct new conce p t i on s of astro n o mi c a l pheno me n a . 
272 
 
Both examp l e s are repre s e n t a t i v e of the sort of misconc e p t i o n s student s held, or in the case 
of Neo, acquire d . 
In the first examp l e I show how Tlotl o had mi scon c e p t i o n s about gravit y prior to 
her visit, but what she experi e nc e d during the visit change d them to a more scient i f i c a l l y 
accept a b l e notion of gravit y . Before her visit, Tlotlo knew that gravit y is a force which 
?puts things down?, but she be lieved there is no gravity on  the Moon or on Jupiter. Her 
evidenc e for there being no gravity on the M oon was the fact that she saw televisi o n 
images of Mark Shuttl e w o r t h : ?he wasn?t like on land , he was floatin g around? 
(tsw02p r e i n t 154). Althoug h Shuttle w o r t h went  to the Intern a t i o n a l Space Statio n many 
studen t s seemed to believ e he went to the Mo on, and used his floating as evidence that 
there is no gravi t y there . Tlotl o did however think there might be gravity on the Sun, using 
her own person a l theor y that ?becaus e you get heated and then you just, it grabs you, the 
heat? (tsw02 p r e i n t 170). After her visit, Tlotlo  had very differe n t id eas about gravity . She 
still believed that things might float on the Moon, but having seen pictur e s of Neil 
Ar mstro n g on the surface , she now conside r e d th at the gravit y there is very low. Like 
many stude n t s , and as descri b e d in Sectio n 4.2.6, Tlotlo thought peopl e needed to wear 
boots on the Moon to keep them on the surfac e . She also now though t that the gravit y on 
Jupiter is high, and gave a short descrip t i o n of  how the Sun and Jupiter, with their strong 
gravity, fought for asteroid s , one of which cras hed into Jupiter. Her interpretation of an 
explanation at HartRAO in c onnection with asteroids and (probab l y ) Co me t Hale-B o p p 
crashing into Jupiter is likely to have been the source of this idea, althou g h it is also 
possibl e she played with the ?Cos mic Pinbal l ? exhibi t in which one makes a comet crash 
into Jupite r (see Sectio n 4.2.4) . In her post-v i s i t PMM she stated that ?the sun and Jupite r 
are likely to have the same amount of grav ity ? (tsw02p o s 9), but dur ing the intervie w she 
elaborated on a new understanding of gravity ?Well I think gravity is caused by the ... like 
maybe if somet h i n g is big then there ? s more  gravit y ? (tsw02 p o s i n t 1 8 9 ) and decide d that 
the Sun, as it?s bigge r would have more gravi t y than Jupiter . Clearl y , Tlotlo had a very 
differe n t concep t i o n of grav ity after the visit, and he r understanding had changed 
substa n t i a l l y toward s a scient i f i c under s t a n d i n g . It is impor t a n t  to note that Tlotlo built on 
her relati ve l y na?ve prior knowl e d g e about gravi t y to do this, and also that she still retai n e d 
some miscon c e p t i o ns . She thought that boots he lp to hold you to the Moon? s surfa c e in an 
enviro n me n t of low gravit y , and she did not realise that everythi n g has gravity, however 
small . These misco n c e p t i o n s howev e r are rela ti v e l y ?mino r ? compa r e d with the ones she 
273 
 
had before the visit, and it is likely that fu rthe r exposu r e to for mal or infor mal teaching 
about gravity would assist he r underst a n d i n g still furthe r . 
In the second examp l e I show how Neo acquir e d a miscon c e p t i on during her visit. 
In Chapter 7 I describ e d how Neo added to he r knowled g e of the Sun and differe n t i a t e d it 
so that the end result was a more complex understanding of th e Sun, its scale with respect 
to the Earth, and the notion of sunspot s . Duri ng this process of add ition and differen t i a t i o n , 
Neo changed her ideas in the followi n g ways. Pr ior to the visit she kne w a mixture of facts 
about the Sun: that it is a star, but bigger , th at it is bigger than the Moon and furthe r away 
and that it can be ?used? for growin g plants . She stress e d the heat of th e Sun (instea d of its 
light) , both for the growin g of plants and the reason why stars are not visibl e in the day. 
She gave errone o u s estima t e s of room tempe r a t ur e , boili n g water and the Sun?s 
temper a t u r e , showin g little unders t a n d i n g of th e concept of tempera t u r e. After the visit she 
showe d simi l a r knowl e d g e of the Sun and tempe r a t u r e as she had in the pre-vi s i t interv i e w , 
though she now inflate d her estima t e of th e Sun?s surfa c e to 5 billio n (degr e e s ) . In 
additio n , she now referre d to somethi n g she was shown at HartRAO:  a sunspot. Although 
she had some idea of the size of the sunspot she had seen ? she referr e d to the fact that 100 
Earth s can fit into it ? her understanding of what a sunspot  consists of was some what 
flawed . She seemed to think that the spot is at the centre of the Sun, and that it is ?a bit 
cold?. Howeve r , her limite d concep t i o n is really to be expect e d . The sunspo t was 
demons t r a t e d by the HartRA O educat o r using the Sun teles c ope proje ct i ng the image on to 
card, and Neo would have used any prior knowle d g e she had about the Sun, togeth e r with 
what the educator was explai ning, to build her own concep tion of the sunspot. It so 
happened (on the day she visited HartRAO) that this sunspot wa s lying in the middl e of the 
Sun?s disc so it is natura l that Neo would thi nk it is the Sun?s middle. She would also have 
heard the educa t o r sayi ng that the spot is a coole r part of the Sun (see Chapt e r 4), and Neo 
interpreted this as being ?a b it cold?. The other fa ct that Neo reme mber ed about the Sun in 
her post-vis i t intervie w was that the Sun is goi ng to expand, contract to become a dwarf, 
and ?ther e will be no sun anymo r e ? .  
What Neo has construc t e d is a greater, but partially flawed, knowledge of the 
concept Sun. Althoug h she could be said to ha ve acqui r e d a misco n c e p t i o n durin g th e visit, 
her knowle dge has clearl y incre a s e d, and it is quite possi b l e that she will reme mb e r the 
idea of the sunspo t for the futu re. Neo therefo r e , is an exam ple of a student who learnt 
from her visit, even though her pre- and pos t-vis i t mean ?score? for Big Ideas hardly 
274 
 
changed . The implic a t i o n of Neo?s experi e n c e is  that the visit was able to build on her 
prior learni n g . Like 12 other studen t s in  the study, she alrea dy had above-average 
knowle d g e of Big Ideas, and was able to inco rporate further knowledge into her existing 
fra mewo r k . I do not conside r it of concern that  she left HartRAO with a misrepr e s en t a t i o n 
of sunspots as the ?cold mi ddle of the Sun?. The fact that sh e has now seen and can discuss 
a sunspo t sugge s t s that she has the basis for furthe r learn i n g about them. In Roschell e ? s 
terms we can ?expec t learn i n g to occur throu g h  gradua l refine me n t and restru c t u r i n g of 
small comp on e n t [ s ] ? (1995, p 43), and it is for this reason that follow- u p by the teacher 
back at the school is so im porta n t (Griffi n , 2004), althoug h it wa s limite d or non-ex i s t e n t in 
my study (Chapte r 3).  
The HartRA O educa t o r provi d e d a clear  demons t r a t i o n and explana t i o n of 
sunspot s . So me student s (such as Richard and Julius ) lear n e d a bout sunspots without 
acquiring a misconception while most others (s uch as Douglas and Judy) never mentioned 
sunsp o t s in their post- v i s i t PMM or interview. I therefore s uggest that the learnin g about 
sunsp o t s varie d acros s the studen t s who visite d HartRAO, and the extent of learning or 
acqui si t i o n of a misco n c e p t i o n was littl e di ffe r e n t to other lear n i ng envi r o n me n t s . 
However, there was one incident that t ook place at HartRAO which appeared to promote 
the acquis i t i o n of miscon c e p t i o n s by studen t s . My evide n ce for it is limit e d , but I draw 
attent i o n to it here to demons t r a t e how an attempt to relat e stude n t s ? under s t a n d i n g to 
somet h i n g they are famil i a r with may cause  confus i o n for some student s . At HartRAO 
durin g the water bottl e rocke t activ i t y the educ ato r sugges t e d that the student s experi me n t 
with differen t proporti o n s of ai r and water in their bottles to see how high they can make 
the rocket go. This is a good activi t y to get st uden t s to make informa l hypoth e s e s and test 
them out. Howeve r , I observ e d that during th e discu s si o n at the end of the activi t y the 
educator describe d the water pu t inside the rocket s as ?fue l?, and although she explained 
why a particular proportion of wa ter to air inside the bottl e resu l t e d in great e r pres s ur e and 
highe r altit ud e reach e d , stude n t s were left with  the impre s s i o n that a simil a r proce ss takes 
place in real rocket s (e.g. notebo o k 1 page 49 7/6/03 ) . Neo explai n e d after her visit that 
?I?ve learnt that the rocket or let me say the space shuttl e needs, it doesn? t need more 
maybe like air or gas or more air or more gas in it,  it just needs a little bit of gas so that the 
gas and the air can combi n e toge t h er so that it can lift off.  So if it?s too much gas or if 
there?s no gas it can?t go up? (swo42po s i n t 157). Her explana t i o n suggest s that Neo 
actuall y thinks that differe n t propor t i o n s of fu el or gas in a space shutt l e are neede d in the 
275 
 
same way that water and air are combi n e d in  the water bottle . Anothe r studen t (from St 
August i n e s School , not used in my main study)  wrote in his post-v i s i t PMM ?In order for 
spacec r a f t to fly it needs approx i ma t e l y a quarte r of fuel and the air should take the space?. 
Althou g h these are only two exampl e s , they de mons t r a t e the danger of an analog y which is 
taken literal l y by student s , re sulting in them acquiring a misconception about rockets and 
their fuel requir e me n t s . 
What does this tell us then about misco n c e p t i o n s in the museu m envir o n me n t ? It 
suggests that with a variety of  activities around a th eme, conceptual change can occur as a 
resul t of a visit to a scien c e centr e . It al so accept s that mi scon c e p t i o ns can be acquir e d 
during a visit to a centre, but  finds that, as Roschell e ( 1995) has suggested and Anderson 
(1999) later demons t r a t e d , such misconc e p t i o n s are a steppin g stone towards more 
compr e h e n s i v e and (idea l l y ) sc ientif i c knowledg e . As Falk a nd Dierking (2000) lamented, 
there is a dearth of research study in this area, and my findings  help to add to the growing 
body of knowle d g e of how miscon c e p t i o n s are worked with in an informa l settin g . 
9.6  Science Literacy 
The extent to which students improved their science literacy was vari able. In section 2.6 I 
identi f i e d Shen?s classi f i c a t i o n of scienc e literacy (Shen, 1975) as being the most 
approp r i a t e for museum learn i n g . Evide n c e from my study suggests that students did 
?know more scienc e ? after thei r visit, and could be said to have improved their basic 
knowled g e of astrono m y . While Re nnie?s statement that deep science learning probably 
does not occur durin g a scienc e centr e visit may be true (Renni e , 2001), my study sugges t s 
that some cogni t i ve under s t a n di n g can occur for the majori t y of studen t s . Some stude n t s 
(notab l y Nkulue k o and Fatima ) also increa s e d th eir cultur a l scienc e litera c y , whereb y they 
showed a greater appreciation of the achieveme n t s of scienc e . Both the planet a r i um and 
HartRAO aligned their activit i e s and present a t i o n s to the conte n t of the schoo l curri c u l u m 
rather than to notio n s of scien c e liter a c y . Measur i n g the achiev e me n t or otherw i s e of 
science literacy goals may therefore be  more appropriate for visits by groups other than 
school students . 
9.7  Role of Vocabulary  
S t u d e n t s ? use of astronom y - r e l a t e d vocabula r y wa s discusse d in section 7.4. The numb er of 
words studen t s used in their PMMs varied conside r a b l y (mean 20.3, standard deviation 
276 
 
7.5), and all student s except Ntobek o increa s e d their vocabu l a r y in their post-v i s i t PMM. 
There appear s to be no relati o n s h i p betwee n  vocabulary and knowledge of Big Ideas, 
except in the case of catego r y D studen t s , who a ll used a larger number of words tha n their 
peers. On this basis I suggest that coun tin g words in PMMs is not a reliabl e way of 
assessin g students ? concept knowledg e or understanding, as propounded by Falk (2003). 
9.8  Critical Reflection of the Research Process 
C a r r y i n g out resea r c h in museu m envir o n me n t s is notori o u s l y diffic u l t , as any attempt to 
acquire empirica l data tends to inter fe r e with the lear n i n g exper i e nc e of the visit or (Alle n , 
2002; Falk & Dierkin g , 2000). My strategy was to acquire pre- and post-visit data using 
personal me aning maps and intervie w s to de termin e whether students had construc t e d 
knowl e d g e as a resul t of the visit . Simil a r me tho d s have been used succe s s f u l l y in many 
studies of museum learnin g (e.g. Adelma n et al ., 2000; Cox-Petersen et al ., 2003) but it is 
unclea r to what extent the proces s of data co llection has altered visitors? perceptions of the 
experien c e . While I did not encounte r any diffi c u l t i e s in gathe ri n g my data, I need to 
accept that my collecti on of PMM data followed  by one-to - one intervi e w s with a select i on 
of studen t s was likely to have highli g h t e d the im port a n c e of the visit. Six of the 34 studen t s 
I interviewed (e.g. Banyan a in section 5.7.1) showed that my  visit had caused them to do 
extra prepar a t i o n for the visit, which they might not have done if I had not intervie w e d 
them. Altho u g h these numbe r s are relat i v e l y low (18% of student s ) my finding s may 
possibly overesti ma t e the overall learning th at took place in the study. Although my study 
did not examine teache r s and their work with the classes my role could be regarded as that 
of a teache r , and my wor k with the stude n t s as  preparat i o n for and follow-u p after the visit 
which was not done by the stude nt s ? accompa n y i n g teach e r s .  
One stran d of curre nt infor ma l learn i n g resea r c h stres se s the impor t a n c e of 
deter mi n i n g the long- t e r m effec t of museu m  visits (e.g. Falk & Dierki n g , 1997; Medved & 
Oatley , 2000), and my study might have benef ited from follow-up data collection some 
months later. Unfort u n a t e l y the time fra me of the study preclud e d this possibi l i t y , but 
gettin g studen t s to write an e ssay abou t thei r visi t seve r al m onth s after the event might be 
an approp r i a t e way of findin g out what they  reme mbe r . This could be followe d by an 
interv i e w to discus s what studen t s had writte n , though analys i s of the data collec t e d would 
need to consider the further impact of th e data collecti o n methods  on the ?research as 
inter v e n t i o n? compa r e d with  visits not researched. 
277 
 
My study did not involv e a compar i s o n betw een the learnin g taking place at the 
planeta r i u m and HartRAO , but a brief commen t on the two sites is pertin e n t here. The 
coll ec t i ve lear n i n g desc ribed in Chapter 5 show that stude nt s were able to lear n about Big 
Ideas at both sites. Two of the portrait student s who showed an incr eas e in their Big Ideas 
knowled g e (Fatima and Helen) visited the planet a r i u m, again sugges t i n g that visits to both 
study sites enabled learning. The one area of difference was that the planetarium did not 
provid e activi t i e s for studen t s to engage in, ex cep t that they were encou r a g e d to use star 
chart s (hand e d out to them) and to obser v e  the sky on subsequ e n t nights . Given the 
import a n c e of affect i v e learn i n g (such as wonder and enjoyme n t ) I have identif i e d in this 
study, the planet a r i u m might consi d e r provi d i n g additi o n a l hands- o n ac tivities for students 
as part of their visit exper i e nc e .  
9.9  Conclusions and recommendations 
A study of this kind has never been conduct e d be fore in South Africa. There has been very 
little research into inform al learn i n g , and none combi n i n g it with astro n o my . The 
infere n c e s drawn are likely to be restri c t e d to simil a r cont ext s of school visi t s invol v i n g 
12- to 15-ye a r - ol d s , scien c e centre s thems e l v e s with simil a r cont e x t s to the planeta r i u m 
and HartRAO , in that didacti c methods are used as part of visitors ? experien c e .  
This thesi s set out to answe r the follow i n g resear ch quest i o n s: 
1.  To what extent do students learn in the pr oce s s of a visit to a plane t a r i u m or the 
visit or s ? centr e of an astro n o mi c a l obser v a t or y?  
2.  H o w is the content of astronom y communi c a t e d to student s ?  
3.  W h a t are stude n t s ? individ u a l experiences of the visit? 
4.  H o w do students construc t knowledg e during and after the visit?  
5.  H o w do student s ? interes t s and prior knowle dge affect the learning experience of a 
school visit?  
While my case study canno t be used to genera lise to other science centres either in 
Southe r n Africa or overse a s , I sugges t that th e point s I make can be regar d e d as ?fuzz y 
general i s a t i o n s ? (Bassey , 1999). These are stateme n t s with some uncert a i n t y built - i n , 
which acknow l e d g e s the comple x i t y of the natu re of human inter a c t i o n s , and can lead to 
both limite d recomme n d a t i o n s and s ugge s t i o n s for furth e r resea r c h . 
Answering research questions 2 and 3, I ha ve shown (in Chapter 4) the experience 
of a studen t visiti n g a scienc e centre , that it is a unique enco unter for each individual, and 
that some of the perce p t i o n s of schoo l visit s  found in the science educat i o n litera t u r e are 
278 
 
confir me d , while othe r s are chall e nge d . For ex amp l e , stude n t s regar d  the visit as a fun 
activi t y which will also invol v e learn i n g , confi r mi n g re sea r c h by Falk et al . (1998). 
Howev e r , notio n s of full class participation and teach er invol v e me n t in sc hool visit s appea r 
to be even less preval e n t than they ar e in develope d countri e s (e.g. Griffi n , 2004; 
Storksd i e c k , 2004). Chapter 5 answers researc h question 1: I showed that student s do learn 
appreciably during school visits , and I described the main areas  of learni n g in  terms of Big 
Ideas in astron o my . Addres s i n g resear c h questi o n s 4 and 5, and using a human 
cons t r u ct i vi st frame w o r k for lear ning, I showed in Chapters 6 to 8 that each stude n t ? s 
learni n g expe r i e nc e is uniqu e , and that it is diffi c u l t to predi c t how lear ni n g will occur for 
any one individ u a l . I was also able to show th at incre me n t a l addit i o n of facts was by far the 
commo n e s t form of cogni t i ve lear n i n g expe ri e n c e d by the student s , and that more 
substan t i a l knowle d g e restru c t u r i n g also occu rre d , though less often. I also demons t r a t e d 
that student s with a greater prior knowledge of astronomy we re able to show a greate r 
degree of knowledg e restruct u r i n g . I furthe r described the importance of affective and 
conati ve aspect s of learni n g , and that they need to be taken into accoun t when exami ni n g 
how learn i ng occur s at a scien c e centr e . The effect of student persona l interes t on their 
ability to learn at a scien c e centre was less clear, and I s uggest that future research 
examin e s how situat i o n a l intere s t can be promot e d , to improv e motiva t i o n . 
The import a n c e of my study has been in a number of different areas, focusing on 
the studen t s invol v e d in school vi sits, and how they learn. First,  I have shown that learning 
occurr e d at the study sites, and that for some st udent s the sorts of things learnt were in line 
with ?acad e mi c ? learn i ng assoc i a t e d with the school curri c ul u m (such as gravit y and stars) . 
Other stude n t s learn t littl e academi c factua l knowle d g e but still reme mb e r e d numero u s 
facts from the visit which may for m the basi s for future learnin g if properl y followe d up 
after the visit. Learning t ook place at the centres despit e the limited involvem e n t of 
teache r s ? prepar i n g studen t s fo r or follow i n g up on the visit.  
Secondl y , I have shown that all stud ent s both learnt during the visit and enjoyed 
thems e l v e s , which provi d e s furth e r evide n c e against  the notion that planet a r i a and hands-
 o n scienc e centre s are locati o n s for mere entert a i n me n t .  
Thirdly, I have shown that prior knowled g e is of key importa n c e in influen c i n g the 
extent to which a student was able to restru cture the knowledge they learnt. Students with 
extensi v e prior knowle d g e were able to show  greater knowle d g e cons truction than those 
with limited prior knowled g e . This finding adds furthe r weigh t to the concept of pre-visit 
279 
 
prepar a t i o n : if all studen t s had been adequa t e l y prepar e d for their visit they would have 
entered the study site with greater prior know ledge which in turn would have enabled a 
greater degree of knowled g e restruc t u r i n g . 
Fourt h l y , while I have demon s t r a t e d that  for some stude n t s , misco n c e p t i o n s were 
altere d by the visit, for others the visit had little or no effect. In addition, some students 
acquired mi sconce p t i o n s during the visit. Ho weve r , if misconc e p t i o n s are viewed as 
stepping stones towards scientif i c knowledg e , th en their altera t i o n or even acquisi t i o n can 
assist studen t learni n g . 
Fifth l y , I have made so me impor t a n t addi ti o n s to methods used in studies of 
learning about astronomy . The importan c e of us ing models cannot be undere s t i ma t e d in 
enablin g student s to discuss abstrac t concept s  with meaning . Similar l y , researc h e r s need to 
pay careful attentio n to the us e of language when discussing concepts such as orbit and 
spin.  
Sixthl y , I have demons t r a t e d the value of Persona l Meaning Mapping in researc h of 
this kind. PMMs have a distinct adva ntage over questio n na i r e s and structu r e d intervi e w s in 
enabl i n g stude n t s to ident i fy what they regard as impor t a n t in relat i o n to the topic , rather 
than the ideas of the resear c h e r . 
Finall y , I have confir me d that a human c onstru c t i v i s t mo del of learnin g is an 
approp r i a t e fra mew o r k with which to view l earni n g at a scienc e centr e . The model enabl e s 
a resea r c h er to ident i fy the princ i pal ways in which stude n t s lear n , which can inform the 
develo p me n t of presen t a t i o n s an d activit i e s at science centre s. In additio n , I have extende d 
Anders o n ? s cognit i v e model of HC into the affect i v e and conati v e domain s , highli g h t i n g 
the import a n c e of these aspect s of learni n g . 
 
The followin g recomme n d a t i o n s with respect to school visits emanat e from my study: 
? A n astron o m y - f o c u s e d scienc e centre should focus on Big Ideas (simi l a r to the ones I 
have suggest e d ) to ensure that key concep t s in astro n o my are cover e d . The numbe r and 
extent of the Big Ideas would depend on th e natur e of the scie nce centre and its 
audie n c e . 
? C u r r e n t l y , the scienc e curric ul u m for grades  7 and 8 does not highlight the importance 
of all the Big Ideas I have identified fo r basic astrono my learnin g . During the next 
280 
 
revision of the curriculum, I recommend that gravit y , size and scale, stars and the Sun, 
and the Solar System be given greate r prom in e n c e as key concep t s in basic astron o my . 
? T h e scienc e centr e shoul d aim to design a vari ety of differen t acti vities or exhibits 
aroun d a limit e d numb e r of Big Idea theme s , so that students are exposed to related 
activiti e s which help to build conc eptual understanding around a Big Idea. 
? S c i e n c e centre s should note th at all students who visit are capable of learning, and plan 
differen t levels of activity which may be appreciated by students of differing ability. 
Present a t i o n s should consci o u s l y aim to refer to students ? prior knowledg e , to 
encour a g e emerge n t learni n g and knowledge restructuring. 
? S c i e n c e centr e s shoul d provi d e writt e n guide l i n e s for teacher s in advance of their visit 
(e.g. Braund 2004 and Appendix K). 
? C e n t r e s shoul d provi d e visit i n g stude n t s a nd their teachers with follow-up activities 
relat e d to cogni t i ve , affe c t i v e or conat i v e lear n i ng . The impor t a n ce of such activ i t i e s is 
to ensur e that stude n t s are remi n d e d of th eir visit over a perio d of time, so that it 
become s a longer - t e r m learn i n g exper i e n c e . 
? G i v e n than all students have  idiosyncratic and individua l ways of learning, centres 
should aim to strike a balance between di dacti c ?teachin g ? a nd allowing a greater 
degree of contro l over stude n t s ? own moveme n t s . At both the planet a r i u m and 
HartRAO the focus is on instruct i o n , but to allow students to pursue  their own interests 
is also impo r t a nt . 
9.10  Future Research 
M y thesi s has focus e d princ i pa l l y on lear nin g by individ u a l stude nts visiting science 
centres, and for ms part of the ?Persona l C ontex t ? (Secti o n 2.8.1) in  Falk and Dierking?s 
Contextual Model (Falk & Dierking, 2000). This  model has only been partly invest i g a t e d 
in empir i c al studi e s , and there is consi d er a bl e furth er scope for resea r c h into the 
Sociocu l t u r a l and Physica l C onte xt s , parti c ul ar l y in devel o p i n g count r i e s . All three 
context s interac t togethe r , and with the notion of ti me, and studies which examine 
mediu m- and long- t e r m infor ma l learn i n g are likely to be import a n t , if method o l o g i c a l 
issues can be addressed. Rennie and colleag u e s (Rennie  et al., 2003) provided a useful 
agenda for furthe r resear c h based on the NARST Ad Hoc Committee? s polic y state me n t on 
Informa l Sci e nc e Educat i o n . The issues includ e  precu r s o r s to learni n g , the physic a l setti n g , 
socioc u l t u r a l factor s , longitu d i n a l researc h , the process of learning and methods used for 
281 
 
capturing data. The affective domain is include d in the list, and I would also expand the 
proce s s of learn i n g to embra c e the conat i ve , wher e the visit to the centr e is a precur s o r for 
action afterw a r d s . 
While the above issues are relevant worldw ide, what is partic ularly important in 
developing countries such as South Africa is rigorous resear ch in the field. The number of 
scienc e centre s is increa s i n g annual l y , but mo s t of the challen g e s faced by the science 
cent re commu n i t y relat e to visit or nu mbe r s and practi t i o n e r issues . Resear c h is needed into 
how centre s impact on learni n g at school leve l, and how teachers can be encourag e d to 
become full partic i p a n t s in th e visit exper i e n c e . Resea r c h al so needs to be conducted into 
how previo u s l y disad v a n t a g e d co mmu n i t i e s can be encou r a g e d to  visit such centres , so that 
the sort of family learni n g found to be so enriching by researchers elsewhere (e.g. Dierking 
& Falk, 1994) can be promo t e d . Final l y , infor ma l  learni n g needs to be inves t i ga t e d at sites 
where the majorit y of the populat i o n reside , su ch as the rural areas and townshi p s , or 
where they visit, such as shoppi n g malls . In these ways the field could be expand e d to 
addres s issue s of import a n c e to under-developed ?South?, and not just the developed 
?Nort h ? . 
9.11  Endpiece 
I end with three quotations on learning by the students. Wh en asked about the purpose of 
the visit, one of the student s  in the pilot study suggest e d  
I?m not sure becau s e we haven ? t actua l l y l earnt any thin g yet.  I still have to see 
abou t that .  
In his intervie w he demonstr a t e d learning about Jupiter and its Moons, the Moon phases 
and the concep t of ?lig ht minutes? , yet he didn?t feel he had learnt anything. Maybe he 
needed more ?overt ? learni n g for it to count for him.  
In contra s t , Antoni a said  that the purpose was  
To just learn mor e than we knew alr ead y ? because we learnt the stuff we 
knew alread y . 
She appea r s to reali s e that th e learni n g is more about deep ening and extending her existing 
knowle d g e . For her, emerge n t lear nin g appears to be importa n t . 
The last word goes to Julius who tried to expla i n , at some lengt h , the impor t a n c e of 
learning and fun: 
282 
 
Julius : Ja, I thoug h t about going back there , but becau s e I thoug h t that if 
there was mo re ti me we would have learn t even more than what 
we?v e lear n t and I think it didn? t only bene f i t me, but I think 
many peop l e who went they we re happ y to go there and they 
learnt more because ther e? s so me people who they don? t, they 
aren? t really intere s t e d , but because they had fun they learn t at the 
same ti me becaus e of the fun.  Now they learnt becaus e there? s 
fun so what I?ve learnt is that most people who learn they learn 
throu g h fun so whatev e r they do should be fun so that they can 
learn. 
Julius? comme n t sums up what learni n g at scienc e centre s should be during school visits, 
incorporating cognitive learning and affective fun. He not only  learnt and had fun, but he 
also learnt that that?s wh at learnin g should be about. 
283 
 
References 
A d e l ma n , L. M., Falk, J., & James, S. (2000) . Impac t of Nation a l Aquar i um in Baltim o r e 
on Visito rs ' Conser v a t i on Attitu de s , Behavi o r and Knowle d g e . Curator , 43(1), 33-
 61. 
Adey, P., & Shayer, M. (1994). Really Raising Standards . London: Routledg e . 
Adler, J., & Lerman , S. (2003) . Getting the Description Right and Making It Count: 
Ethical Practice in Mathematics Education Research . In Bishop, A., Keitel, C., 
Kilpatri c k , J. & Leung, F. (Eds.), Sec ond Intern a t i o n a l Handbo o k of Mathema t i c s 
Educati o n (pp. 441-470 ) . Dordrec h t : Kluwer Educati o n a l Publish e r s . 
Agan, L. (2004). Stellar Ideas:  Explor ing Students ' Understa n d i n g of Stars. The 
Astronomy Education Review , 3(1), 77-97. 
Aikenh e a d , G. (1996) . Scienc e e ducatio n : border crossing into the subculture of science. 
Studies in Science Education , 27(1-52). 
Aikenhe a d , G. (2000). Renegotiating the culture of school science . In Millar, R., Leach, J. 
& Osborn e , J. (Eds.) , Improv i n g Scienc e E ducati o n : the contrib u t i o n of researc h . 
Buckingh a m : Open Universi t y Press. 
Aikenh e a d , G., & Jegede , O. (1999). Cross- C u l t ur a l Scienc e Educat i o n: A Cogniti v e 
Explan a t i o n of a Cultura l Phenome n o n . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 
36(3), 269-287. 
Albanese, A., Danhoni Neves, M. C., & Vicentin i, M. (1997). Models in Science and in 
Educati o n: A Critica l Review of Researc h on Studen t s ' Ideas about the Earth and its 
Place in the Unive r s e . Science and Education , 6, 573-590. 
Allen, R. (Ed.). (2000). New Penguin English Dictionary. London: Penguin. 
Allen, S. (2002). Looking for Learning in Visitor Talk: A Methodological Exploration . In 
Leinhard t , G., Crowley, K. & Knutson, K. (Eds.), Learning Conversa t i o n s in 
Museums (pp. 259-303 ) . Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum. 
Alsop, S. (2000). Meeting the needs of lifel ong learne r s : recogn i z i n g a conativ e dimensi o n 
in physic s educat i o n . Physics Education , 35(3), 202-208. 
Alsop, S., & Watts, M. (1997). Sources from a Somerse t Villag e : A Model for Informa l 
Learni n g about Radiati o n and Radioac t i v i t y . Science Education , 81(6), 633-650. 
Alsop, S., & Watts, M. (2003). Sc ience education and affect. International Journal of 
Science Education , 25(9), 1043-1047. 
Ameri c a n Assoc i a t i o n for the A dvance me n t of Science . (1993). Benchmarks for science 
literacy . New York: Oxford University Press. 
Ameri c a n Assoc i a t i o n for the A dvance me n t of Science . (2001). Atlas of Science Literacy . 
Washin g t o n DC: Americ a n Associ a t i o n for the Advanc e me n t of Scienc e / Natio n a l 
Scienc e Teache r s Assoca t i o n . 
Americ a n Associ a t i o n for the Advanc e me n t of Scien ce. (2005). 2061 Connections 
Nove mbe r / D e c e mb e r 2005. Retri e ve d 23 March 2006, from 
http://www.project2061.org/publicat ions/2061Connections/2005/2005-11a.htm  
Anderson, D. (1999). The Development of Sc ien c e Conce p t s Emerg e n t from Scien c e 
Museum and Post-Vi s i t Activi t y Expe ri e n c e s : Studen t s ' Constr u c t i o n of 
Knowledge. Unpublished PhD, Quee nsland University of Technology. 
Anders o n , D., & Lucas, K. (1997). The Effecti v e n e s s of Orientin g Students to the Physical 
Featu r e s of a Scienc e Muse um Prior to Visitat i o n . Research in Science Education , 
27(4), 485-495. 
Anderson , D., Lucas, K., & Ginns, I. (2003). Th eore t i c a l Perspe c t i v e s on Learnin g in an 
Infor ma l Setti n g . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 40(2), 177-199. 
284 
 
Anderso n , D., Lucas, K., Ginns, I., & Dierkin g , L. D. (2000). Develop me n t of Knowled g e 
about Electric i t y and Magnetis m during a Vi sit to a Scien c e Museu m and Relat e d 
Post - v i si t Acti vi t i e s . Science Education , 84(5), 658-679. 
Ansbach e r , T. (1998a) . John Dewey's Experie n ce and Education: Lessons for Museums. 
Curator , 41(1), 36-49. 
Ansbac h e r , T. (1998b) . Review of Lear nin g in the Museum, by Geoge Hein. Curator , 
41(4), 285-291. 
Ansbach e r , T. (2002a) . Misunde rstandings of Meaning Making. E xhibitionist , 21(1), 1-4. 
Ansbache r , T. (2002b). What Ar e We Learnin g ? Outcome s of the Museum Experie n c e . 
Informal Learning Review , 53(March-April), not known. 
Atwood , R., & Atwood , V. (1996) . Prese r vi c e Eleme n t a r y Teach e r s ' Conce p t i o n s of the 
Causes of the Seasons . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 33(5), 553-563. 
Ausubel , D. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view . New Yor k: Holt, Rinehart 
and Winston. 
Ausubel, D., Novak, J., & Hanesian, H. (1978). Educational psychology: A cognitive view  
(2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. 
Bailey, J., & Slater, T. (2003). A Review  of Astrono my Educati o n Researc h . Astronomy 
Education Review , 2(2). 
Bale, J. (2006). It inspired generations of childre n , but now the Planeta r i u m is focusin g on 
lesser stars. Retrie v e d July 12, 2006, from 
http:// w w w . t i me s o n l i n e . c o . u k / a r t i c l e / 0 , 2 - 2 0 1 7 2 1 0 , 0 0 . h t ml  
Bar, V., Sneider , C., & Martimb e a u , N. (1997). Is There Gravit y in Space? Science and 
Children ( Ap r i l ) , 38-43. 
Bar, V., Zinn, B., Goldmuntz, R., & Sneider, C. (1994). Childre n 's Concept s about Weight 
and Free Fall. Science Education , 78(2), 149-169. 
Barba, R., & Rubba, P. (1992). A Comparison of Preserv i c e and In-Serv i c e Earth and 
Space Scienc e Teach e r s ' Gener a l Menta l  Abilities, Content Knowledge, and 
Proble m- S o l v i n g Skills . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 29(10), 1021-
 1035. 
Barnet t , M., & Morran, J. (2002). Addres s i n g childr e n 's altern a t i v e framew o r k s of the 
Moon's phases and eclipse s . International Journal of Science Education , 24(8) , 
859-879 . 
Bassey, M. (1999). Case Study Research in Educational Settings . Buckin g h a m, UK: Open 
Univer s i t y Press. 
Baxter , J. H. (1989) . Childr e n 's unders t a n di n g of fa mili a r astro n o mi c a l event s . 
International Journal of Science Education , 11(Special Issue), 502-513. 
Baxter, J. H. (1991). A constructivist appro ach to astronomy in the National Curricul u m . 
Physics Education , 26, 38-45. 
Beetlestone, J. G., Johnson, C. H., Quin, M ., & White, H. (1998). The Science Center 
Moveme n t :  context s , practi c e , next challen g e s . Public Understanding of Science , 
7, 5-26. 
Berg, T., & Brewer, W. (1991). Teache r Awa renes s of Student A ltern a t e Concep t i o n s 
about Rotational Motion and Gravity. Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 
28(1), 3-18. 
Berk, L. (2003). Child Development . Boston: Allyn and Bacon. 
Bliss, J. (1995). Piaget and afte r: the case of learni n g scienc e . Studies in Science 
Education , 25, 139-172. 
Block, E., & Rollnick, M. (2003). Wo rds used are underst a n d a b l e ; the way the informa t i o n 
is phrased is impossi b l e to underst a n d . In  Putsoa, B., Dla mini, B. & Kelly, V. 
285 
 
(Eds.), Proceedi n g s of the 11th A nnual SAARM ST E confere n c e (pp. 277-285 ) : 
Universi t y of Swazilan d , Mbabane. 
Boaler, J. (1998). A Tale of Two Schools: Alternati ve Teaching Approaches and Situated 
Learning . In Watson , A. (Ed.), Situat e d C ogni t i on in Mathem a t i c s (pp. 83-92 ) . 
Oxford: Oxford Universi t y Departme n t of Educatio n a l Studies. 
Boisvert, D. L., & Slez, B. J.  (1994). The Relationship betwee n Visitor Charact e r i s t i c s and 
Learni n g- A s s o c i a t e d Behavi o r s in a Scienc e Museu m Disco v e r y Space . Science 
Education , 78(2), 137-149. 
Boone, W., & Rogan, J. (2005). Rigour in qua ntit a t i v e analys i s : Th e promise of Rasch 
analysis techniqu e s . African Journal of Research in Mathematics, Science and 
Technology Education , 9(1), 25-38. 
Borun, M., Massey, C., & Lutter, T. (1993). Na ive Knowledge and the Design of Science 
Museu m Exhib i t s . Curator , 36(3), 201-219. 
Braund , M. (2004). Learning science at museums and hands-on centres . In Braund, M. & 
Reiss, M. (Eds.) , Learni n g Scienc e Outsid e the Classr o o m (pp. 113-12 8 ) . London : 
Routle d g e F a l me r . 
Braund , M., & Reiss, M. (2004) . The nature of learning scie nce outside the classroom. In 
Braund, M. & Reiss, M. (Eds.), Learni n g Scienc e Outsid e the Classr o o m (pp. 1-
 12). London : Routle d g e F a l me r . 
Brodie, K. (2005). Textures of talking and th inki n g in second a r y mathema t i c s classr o o ms . 
Unpublished PhD, Stanford University. 
Brown, J. S., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989) . Situated Cognitio n and the Culture of 
Learni n g . Educational Researcher , 18(1), 32-42. 
Brunell o , C. (1992). Enterta i n me n t and Educat i o n : Are they compat i b l e ? The Planetarian , 
21(1), 10-14. 
Calvin , W. (1996) . How Brains Think . London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson. 
Ca me ron , A. (2003). An Investi g a t i o n into Collate r a l Learnin g and Cultura l Border 
Cross i n g in Basic Astro n o my among s t Foundat i o n Student s in a South African 
University. Unpublished Master of Scien ce, University of the Witwa t e r s r a n d , 
Johannes b u r g . 
Ca me ron , A. K., Rollnic k , M., & Doidge, M. (2005). The Influence of Prior Knowledge, 
Cultur e and Religi o n on First Year and Foundati o n Students ' Understa n d i n g of 
Basic Astrono my , 13th Annual Souther n African Associa t i o n for Researc h in 
Mathema t i c s , Scienc e and Technol o g y E ducat i o n Confer e n c e . Windho e k , Na mi bi a . 
Candela, A. (2001). Earthly Talk. Human Development , 44, 119-125. 
Case, J., & Gunston e , R. (2002). Metaco g n i t i v e Devel o p me n t as a Shift in Approa c h to 
Learni n g : an in -depth study. Studies in Higher Education , 27(4), 459-470. 
Cha mpa g n e , D. W. (1975) . The Ontari o Scienc e Centre in Toront o : Some impres s i o n s and 
sime questi o n s . Educational Technology , 15(8), 36-39. 
Chaplin, S. (1997). The developing role of interacti ve science centres within science 
education. In Thomps o n , D. (Ed.), Scien c e E ducat i o n in the 21st Centur y (pp. 59-
 76). Aldersho t , UK: Arena. 
Chapl i n , S., & Graham- S mi t h , F. (1992) . Jodrell Bank Visitor Centre . In Durant, J. (Ed.), 
Museums and the public unders t a n d i n g of  science (pp. 108-109 ) . London: Science 
Museum. 
Chinn, C., & Brewer, W. (1993). The role of anomalo u s data in knowled g e acquisi t i o n : A 
theor et i ca l frame w o r k and impli c a t i o n s for scienc e inst r u ct i on . Review of 
Educational Research , 63, 1-49. 
Clerk, J. (2006) . Weight l e s s Astron a u t s . Paper presen t e d at the South Africa n Associ a t i o n 
of Science and Technology E ducato r s Confere n c e , Durban. 
286 
 
Cobern, W. W. (1991). Worldv i e w Theory a nd Science Educat i o n Resear c h : Fundame n t a l 
Epist e mol o g i c a l Struct u re as a Critica l Factor in Scien c e Lear n i n g and Attit u de 
Devel o p me n t . 
Cobern , W. W. (1996). Worldv i e w Theory and Concept u a l Change in Science Educat i o n . 
Science Education , 80(5), 579-610. 
Cohen, L., & Manion, L. (1994). Research methods in Education . Becke n h a m: Croom 
Helm. 
Comi n s , N. (2001) . Heavenly Errors: Misconceptions About the Real Nature of the 
Universe . New York: Columbi a Univer s i t y Press. 
Coombs , P. A., & Ahme d, M. (1974) . Attacking Rural Poverty: How Non-formal 
Education Can help . Baltimor e : John Hopki ns University Press. 
Cortazzi , M. (1993). Narrative Analysis . London : Falmer Press. 
Cox-Pet e r s e n , A. M., Marsh, D. D., Kisiel, J ., & Melber, L. M. (2003). Investigation of 
Guided School Tours, Student Learnin g , a nd Science Reform Recomme n d a t i o n s at 
a Museum of Natura l Histo r y . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 40(2), 
200-218 . 
Crane, V. (1994). An Introduction to Informal Science Learning and Research . In Crane, 
V., Chen, M., Bitgood , S., Thompso n , D., Ni chols o n , H., Weiss, F., and Campbel l , 
P. (Ed.), Informa l Scienc e Learni n g : Wh at the Researc h Says About Televis i o n , 
Science Museums, and Communit y - B a s e d Pr ojects . Ephrata, PA: Science Press. 
Crawley, F. E., & Koballa, T. R. (1994). A ttitude Research in Science Educatio n : 
Contempora r y Models and Methods. Science Education , 78(1), 35-55. 
Csiksz e n t mi h a l y i , M., & Herman s o n , K. (1995) . Intrinsic Motivation in Museums: Why 
Does One Want to Learn?  In Falk, J. H. & Dierkin g , L. D. (Eds.), Public 
Instit u t i o n s for Person a l Learni n g :  Esta blishing a Research Agenda (pp. 34-37, 59-
 61 ) . Washi n g t o n , D.C.: Americ a n Assoc i a t i o n of Museums . 
Davidowitz, B., & Rollnick, M. (2005). Develo pment and applicat i o n of a rubric for 
analysi s of novice student s ? laboratory flow diagrams. International Journal of 
Science Education , 27(1), 43-59. 
Depart me n t of Arts Cultur e Scienc e and Technolo g y . (1996). White paper on science and 
technol o g y . Prepar i n g for the 21st century . Pr etori a : Governme n t of South Africa. 
Depart me n t of Educat i o n . (2000) . Draft Interv e n t i o n Strate g y for Scienc e , Mathema t i c s 
and Technol o g y Educati o n (SMTE). Pretor i a : Govern me n t of South Africa . 
Department of Education. (2002). Revised Nation al Curric u l u m Stateme nt for Grades R - 9 
(Schoo l s ) Natura l Scien ces (No. 23406). Pretoria. 
Departme n t of Science and Technol og y . (2006) . The Budget Vote Speech by the Minist e r 
of Science & Technol o g y , Mr Mosibud i Mangena . Retriev e d 25 June, 2006, from 
http:// w w w . d s t . g o v . z a / m e d i a / s p e e c h e s . p h p ? i d = 1 90& p r i nt = 1  
Dewey, J. (1938). E xperience and Education . New York: Kappa Delta Pi. 
Diakidoy , I.-A. N., & Kendeou, P. (2001). Facili ta t i n g conceptu a l change in astronom y : a 
compar i s o n of the effect i v e n e s s of two instruc t i o na l approa c h e s . Learning and 
Instruction , 11, 1-20. 
Dierking , L. D. (2005). Museums, Affect, and Cognition: The View for another Window . In 
Alsop, S. (Ed.), Beyond Cartesi a n Dualism. Toronto : Springe r . 
Dierking, L. D., Cohen Jones, M., Wadma n, M., Falk, J. F., Storksd i e c k , M., & 
Ellenbo g e n , K. (2002). Broade n i n g our Noti ons of the Impact of Free-Ch o i c e 
Learnin g Experie n c e s . Informal Learning Review , 53(Jul y / A u g u s t ) . 
Dierkin g , L. D., & Falk, J. F. (1994). Fa mily Behavi o r and Learni n g in Informa l Scienc e 
Settings:  A Review of the Research. Science Education , 78(1), 57-72. 
287 
 
Dierking, L. D., & Martin, L. ( 1997). Guest Editoria l : Introduc t i o n . Science Education , 
81(6), 629-631. 
Dodick , J., & Orion, N. (2003) . Cognit i ve f actors affecti n g student underst a n d i n g of 
geological time. Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 40(4), 415-442. 
Dollard, J. (1935). Criteria for the life history . New Haven: Yale Universi t y Press. 
Dove, J. (2002). Does the man in the moon ev er sleep? An analysis of student answers 
about simpl e astro n o mi c a l event s : a case study. International Journal of Science 
Education , 24(8), 823-834. 
Driver, R., Asoko, H., Leach, J., Mortime r , E., & Scott, P. (1994). C onstr u c t i n g scient i f i c 
knowle d g e in the classro o m. Educational Researcher , 23, 5-12. 
Duit, R., & Treagus t , D. (1998). Learning in science - from behaviourism towards social 
constructivism and beyond . In Fraser, B. J., and Tobin, K.G (Ed.), Internat i o n a l 
Handbo o k of Science Educat i o n (pp. 3- 25). Dordre c h t : Kluwer Academi c 
Publis h e r s . 
Dunlop, J. (2000). How Children Observe the Universe . Publications of the Astronomical 
Society of  Australia , 17, 194-206. 
Durant, J. (1993). What is scientific literacy?  In Durant, J. & Gre gory , J. (Eds.), Science 
and Culture in Europe (pp. 129-138). London: Science Museum. 
Dza ma, E., & Osborne , J. (1999). Poor perfor ma n c e among Africa n stud e n t s : an alter n a t i v e 
explana t i o n of the Afri c a n World View thesi s . Journal of Research in Science 
Teaching , 36(3), 387-403. 
Engestr ? m, Y. (1991). Non Scoale sed v itae discimu s :  Toward overcomi n g the 
encapsul a t i o n of school. Learning and Instruction , 1, 243-259. 
Engest r ? m, Y. (1993) . Developmental studies of work as a testbench of activity theory:  
The case of primary care medical practice. In Chaiklin, S. & Lave, J. (Eds.), 
Understanding practice:  Perspectives  on activity and context (pp. 65-103): 
Ca mbrid g e Univer s i t y Press. 
Erickson , G. (2000). Research programmes and the stude nt science learning literature . In 
Millar , R., Leach, J. & Osborn e , J. (Eds.) , Improv i n g Scienc e Educat i o n : the 
contribution of research (pp. 271-292). Buckin g h a m: Open Univer s i t y Press. 
Explora t o r i u m . (2006). fact sheet 2005-06. Retrieved August 4, 2006, from 
http://www.exploratorium. e d u / a b o u t / f a c t _ s h e e t . h t ml  
Eylon, B.-S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Learni n g an d Instruc t i o n : an Examina t i o n of Four 
Research Perspect i v e s in Science Educati o n . Review of Educational Research , 
58(3), 251-301. 
Falc?o, D., Colinva u x , C., Krapas, S., Querioz , G., Alves, F., Cazelli, S., et al. (2004). A 
model-based approach to science exhibi ti o n evaluati o n : a case study in a Brazilia n 
astro n o my museu m. International Journal of Science Education , 26(8), 951-978. 
Falk, J., & Storks d i e c k , M. (2005) . Using the Context u a l Model of Learnin g to Underst a n d 
Visito r Learni n g from a Sc ience Center Exhibition. Science Education , 89(5), 744-
 778. 
Falk, J. H. (2001). Free-Choice Science Learning: Framing the Discussion . In Falk, J. H. 
(Ed.), Free-Cho i c e Science Educatio n (pp. 3-20). New York: Teachers College 
Press . 
Falk, J. H. (2003). Personal Meaning Mapping . In Caban, G., Scott, C., Falk, J. H. & 
Dierki n g , L. D. (Eds.) , Museums and Crea tivi t y : A study into the role of museums 
in design educati o n . Sydney: Powerho u s e Publish i n g . 
Falk, J. H., & Dierkin g , L. D. (1992). The Museum Experience . Washington DC: 
Whalesback Books. 
288 
 
Falk, J. H., & Dierking , L. D. (1997). School  Field Trips: Assess i n g their Long-T e r m 
Impac t . Curator , 40(3), 211-21 8 . 
Falk, J. H., & Dierkin g , L. D. (2000). Learning from museums: Visitor experiences and 
their making of meaning . Walnut Creek, CA: Altami ra Press. 
Falk, J. H., & Dierking , L. D. (2001). L earni n g from Scienc e Ce nter s : A Broade r 
Perspe c t i v e . ASTC Dimensions , Jan/Feb, 4-7. 
Falk, J. H., Moussouri, T., & Coulson, D. (1998). The Effect of Visitor s ' Agendas on 
Museum Le arni n g . Curator , 41(2), 107-120. 
Fisher, M. S. (1997). The Effect of Humor on Learn i n g in a Plane t a r i u m. Science 
Education , 81(6), 703-713. 
Flavell, J. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving . In Resnick , L. (Ed.), The 
Nature of Intell i g e n ce . New York: Lawren c e Erlbau m. 
Fleer, M. (1997). A Cross-cult u r a l Study of Rural Austral i a n Aborigi n a l Childre n 's 
Understa n d i n g of Night and Day. Research in Science Education , 27(1), 101-116. 
Gardne r , H. (1993). Multiple Intelligences: The theory in practice . New York: Basic 
Books. 
Gaylard , M. (undate d ) . Hist ory of the Science Awarene s s Program me at HartRAO : 
Harteb e e s t h o e k Radio Astrono my Observ a t o r y . 
Gilbert , J. K. (2001). Towards a Unified Model of Education and Entertainment in Science 
and Technology Centres . In Stocklmayer, S. M., Gore , M. M. & Bryant, C. (Eds.), 
Science Communication in Theory and Pr actice (pp. 123-142) . Dordrech t : Kluwer. 
Goldbe r g , S. (1995) . Big Scienc e : Atomic Bomb Resear c h and The Beginn i n g s of High 
Energy Physic s . Retrie v e d 24 March, 2006, from 
http:// d e p t s . w a s h i n g t o n . e d u / h s s e xec/newsletter /1998/goldberg.html  
Gorard, S. (2001). Quantitative methods in educational  research: the role of numbers 
made easy . London: Continu u m. 
Gorard, S., & Taylor, C. (2004). Combining Methods in Educ ational and Social Research . 
Buckingh a m : Open Universi t y Press. 
Gregor y , J., & Miller, S. (1998). Science in Public: Communication, Culture and 
Credibility . Ca mbrid g e , Mass.: Perseus Publish i n g . 
Griffin, J. (2004). Research on Students a nd Museums : Looking More Closely at the 
Students in School Groups. Science Education , 88(Sup p l e me n t 1), S59-S70 . 
Griffin, J., & Symi ngton , D. (1997). Moving fr om Task-Oriented to Learning-Oriented 
Strategi e s on School Ex cursio n s to Museums. Science Education , 81(6), 763-779. 
Gunston e , R. F., & White, R. T.  (1981). Unders t a n d i n g of Gravity . Science Education , 
65(3), 291-299. 
Hargre a v e s , A. (1996) . The emotio n a l practi c e of teachi n g . Teaching and Teacher 
Education , 14, 835-854. 
Hawkey , R. (2001). Science beyond school:  Repres entation or re-presentation?  In 
Loveless , A. & Ellis, V. (Eds.), ICT,  Pedagogy and the Curricul u m (pp. 104-127) . 
London and New York: Routle d g e / Fa l me r . 
Hein, G. E. (1998). Learning in the Museum . London: Routledg e . 
Henning , E. (2004). Finding your way in qualitative research . Pretoria: Van Schaik. 
Hewson, P., & Hewson, M. (1992). The status of students' conceptions . In Druit, R., 
Goldberg, F. & Niedderer, H. (Eds.), Re sear c h in physics learnin g : Theoret i c a l 
issues and empiri c a l studie s (pp. 176-18 7 ) . Kiel, Germa n y : Insti t ut e for Scien c e 
Educat i o n . 
Hidi, S., & Harachi e w i c z , J. (2000). Motiva t i o n the Academi c a l l y Unmoti v a t e d : A Critica l 
Issue for the 21st Century . Review of Educational Research , 70(2), 151-179. 
289 
 
Hilgard , E. (1980). The trilogy of mind: Cogniti on , affect i o n , and conation . Journal of the 
History of the Behavioral Sciences , 16, 107-117. 
Hlatshwa y o , S., & Stanton, M. (2005). Alte rnative conceptions of high school science 
studen t s on project i l e motion . In Kasanda , C., Muhammed , L., Akpo, S. & Ngololo, 
E. (Eds.), Proceed i n g s of the13th Annual SAARMST E Confere n c e . Windhoe k , 
Nami bi a : Unesco Office . 
Hofstein, A., & Rosenfeld, S. (1996). Bridgi n g the Gap Between Formal and Informa l 
Scienc e Learni n g . Studies in Science Education , 28, 87-112. 
Hollida y , A. (2002). Doing and Writing Qualitative Research . London: Sage. 
Hufnag e l , B. (2002) . Develo p me n t of the Astron o my Diagno s t i c Test. Astronomy 
Education Review , 1(1), 47-51. 
Huitt, W. (1999) . Conati o n as an import a nt factor of mind. Re trieve d 27 April, 2006, from 
http://c h i r o n . v a l d o s t a . e d u / w h u i t t / c o l / r e g s y s / c o n a t i o n . h t ml  
Jarman , R., & McAlees e , L. (1996). Physic s for the star-ga z e r : pupils ' attitu d e s to 
astrono my in the Norther n Irelan d Scienc e Curric u l u m. Physics Education , 223-
 226. 
Jarvis, T., & Pell, A. (2002). Effect of the Challen g e r Experie n c e on Element a r y Childre n 's 
Attit u de s to Scien c e . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 39(10), 979-1000 . 
Jarvis, T., & Pell, A. (2005). Factors influen c i n g eleme n t a r y schoo l child r e n 's attit u des to 
scienc e before during and follow i n g a visit to the UK Nation a l Space Centre . 
Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 42(1), 53-83. 
Jegede, O. J. (1995). Collater a l Learnin g and the Eco-Cul t u r a l Paradig m in Science and 
Mathe ma t i cs Educa t i o n in Afric a . Studies in Science Education , 25, 97-137. 
Jegede, O. J., & Okebukola , P. A. (1991). Th e relation s h i p between African traditio n a l 
cosmo l o g y and stude n t s ' acqui si t i o n of a scienc e proces s skill. International 
Journal of Science Education , 13(1), 37-47. 
Johannes b u r g Planetar i u m . (1999). History of the Johanne s b u r g Planeta r i u m. Retriev e d 30 
August , 2005, from http:// w w w . w i t s . a c . z a / p l a n e t a r i u m/ h i s t o r y . h t m  
Kagan, D. (1990). Ways of Evaluat i o n Teach e r Cogni t i o n : Infer e n c e s Conce r n i n g the 
Goldi l o c k s Princ i pl e . Review of Educational Research , 60(3), 419-469. 
Kikas, E. (1998a) . The Impact of Teachin g on Students ' Defi niti o n s and Explanat i o ns of 
Astron o mi c a l Phenome na . Learning and Instruction , 8(5), 439-454. 
Kikas, E. (1998b). Pupils' explanat i o n s of seasona l changes : age differe n c e s and the 
influe n c e of teachi n g . British Journal of Educational Psychology , 68, 505-516. 
King, N. (n.d.) . Templa t e Analys i s: Qualit y Checks . Retrie ve d 20 July, 2006, from 
http:// w w w . h u d . a c . u k / h h s / r e s e a r c h / t e mp l a t e _ a n a l y s i s / t e c h n i q u e / q u a l i t y r e f l e x i v i t y . h
 t m  
Klein, C. A. (1982). Children 's C oncep t s of the Earth and the Sun. Science Education , 
65(1), 95-107. 
Koballa , T. R. (n.d.). The Motivat i o n a l Power of Science Discrepant Events. Retrieved 20 
July, 2006, from 
http://w w w . a r c h e s . u g a . e d u / ~ b c r a mo n d / h o me / D i s c r e p a n t E v e n t s . h t m  
Koosimi l e , A. T. (2004). Out-of - s c h o o l experi e n c e s in science classe s :  problems , issues 
and challenges in Botswana. International Journal on Science Education , 26(4) , 
483-496 . 
Kubota, C., & Olstad, R. (1991). Effects of Novelty- r e d u c i n g preparat i o n on Explorat o r y 
Behav i o r and Cognit i v e Learn i n g in a Scien c e Museu m Setti n g . Journal of 
Research in Science Teaching , 28(3), 225-234. 
290 
 
Larkin, S. (2000) . How Can We Discer n Me tacog n i t i o n in Year One Childr e n from 
Intera c t i o n s betwee n Studen t s and Teach er, Paper presented at ESRC Teac hing and 
Learnin g Researc h Program m e Confere n c e . Leicest e r , UK. 
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitima te peripheral participation. 
Ca mbri d g e : Ca mbri d g e Univer s i t y Press. 
Lederma n , N., Abd-El - K h a l i c k , F., Bell, R., & Schwartz, R. (2002). Views of Nature of 
Science Questi o n n a i r e . Journal Research on Science Teaching , 39, 497-521. 
Leinha r d t , G., & Gregg, M. (2002). Burning Buses, Burning Crosses: Student Teachers 
See Civil Rights . In Leinhardt, G., Crowley, K.  & Knutson, K. (Eds.), Learning 
Conversations in Museums (pp. 139-166). Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum. 
Lelliot t , A. D., Rollni c k, M., & Pendle b u r y , S. (2006) . Gravit y in the Solar System ? the 
role of informa l learnin g , 14th Annual S outher n African Associa t i o n for Researc h 
in Mathema t i c s , Scienc e and Techno l o g y Educatio n Conferen c e (pp. 78-88). 
Pretoria , South Africa. 
Lemmer, M., Lemmer, T. M., & Smit, J. J. A.  (2003). South Africa n st udent s ' views of the 
unive r s e . International Journal of Science Education , 25(5), 562-582. 
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry . Beverly Hills: Sage. 
Loughran , J., Mulhall, P., & Berry, A. ( 2004). In Search of Pedagog i c a l Conten t 
Knowledge in Science: Developing Wa ys of Art i cul a t i n g and Docume n t i n g 
Profess i o n a l Practic e . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 41(4), 370-391. 
Lucas, A. M. (1983). Scient i f i c Literacy and Informal Learning. Studies in Science 
Education , 10, 1-36. 
Lucas, A. M. (1991). 'Info-t a i n me n t ' and informa l sources for learnin g science . 
International Journal of Science Education , 13(5), 495-504. 
Lucas, K. B. (1999). Wh en Mr Jones  took Grade 5 to the Science c e nt r e . Retrie ve d 
2003/0 4 / 0 2 , 2003 
Lucas, K. B. (2000). One Teacher 's Agenda for a Class Visit to an Interac t i v e Science 
Cente r . Science Education , 84, 524-544. 
Luke, J. J. (1998). Art Around the Corner: An Assess me n t of the Long-t e r m Impac t of an 
Art Museum Progr a m on Stude nts? Interpretations of Ar t. Unpubli s h e d Master of 
Museum Studie s Thesis , Univer s i t y of Toronto , Toront o . 
Luke, J. J., Ca mp, B. D., Dierki ng , L. D., & Pearce, U. J. (2001). The First Free-Choice 
Science Learning Conference: From Issues to Future Directions.  In Falk, J. H. 
(Ed.), Free-Choice Science Education. New York: Teacher s College Press. 
Maher, F. A., & Tetreault , M. K. (1994). The Feminist Classroom . New York: Basic 
Books. 
Malcol m, C., & Alant, B. (2004). Findin g Di rectio n When the Ground Is Moving: Science 
Educat i o n Resear c h in South Africa . Studies in Science Education , 40, 49-104. 
Mali, G. B., & Howe, A. (1979). Develop m e n t of Earth and Gravit y Concep t s among 
Nepali Childr e n . Science Education , 63(5), 685-691. 
Martin, L. (2001). Free-Choice Science Learning: Futu re Directions for Researchers . In 
Falk, J. (Ed.), Free-C h o i c e Scienc e Educat i o n . New York: Teacher s College Press. 
Martin, L. (2004). An Emergin g Researc h Fr ame wo r k for Studyin g Informa l Learni n g and 
Schools . Science Education , 88(Suppl e me n t 1), S71-S82. 
Matthe w s , M. (2000). Time for Science Education : Kluwer Academi c / Plenum Publis h e r s . 
McClafferty, T. P. (1995). Did You Hear Grandda d ? Childre n 's and adults' use and 
underst a n d i n g of a sound exhibit at  interactive science centres. Journal of 
Education in Museums , 16, 12-16. 
291 
 
McClure, J. R., Sonak, B., & Suen, H. K. (1999) . Concep t Map Assess me nt of Classr o o m 
Learn i n g: Relia bi l i t y , Validi t y , and Logisti c a l Practi c a l i t y . Journal of Research in 
Science Teaching , 36(4), 475-492. 
McManu s , P. M. (1992) . Topics in museums and scienc e educat i o n . Studies in Science 
Education , 20, 157-182. 
Medved , M., & Oatley , K. (2000) . Memori e s and scient i f i c lit era c y : reme mb e r i n g exhibi t s 
from a scienc e centr e . International Journal of Science Education , 22(10), 1117-
 1132. 
Miles, M. B., & Huberma n , A. M. (1994). Qualitative Data Analysis  (2nd ed.). Thousand 
Oaks, Ca: Sage. 
Mintzes , J. J., & Wanders e e , J. H. (1998). Reform and Innovation in Science Teaching: a 
Human Constructivist View . In Mintzes, J. J., Wa nderse e , J. H. & Novak, J. D. 
(Eds.) , Teachi n g Scienc e for Unders t a n d i n g . A Human Constr u c t i v i s t View. San 
Diego: Acade mi c Press . 
Mintzes, J. J., Wanderse e , J. H., & Novak, J. (1997). Meaningful Learning in Science: The 
Human Constructivist Perspective . In Phye, G. D. (Ed.), Handbook of Academic 
Learning (pp. 405-447). San Die go, CA: Academi c Press. 
Mische l , W. (1996) . From good intentions to willpower . In Gollwit z e r , P. & Bargh, J. 
(Eds.), The Psychol o g y of Action (pp. 197-218). New York: Guildford Press. 
Mishler, E. G. (1990). Valida tio n in Inquir y - G u i d e d Resear c h : The Role of Exe mpl a r s in 
Narrat i v e Studie s . Harvard Educational Review , 60(4), 415-442. 
Mohapatra, J. K. (1991). The interaction of cu ltural rituals and the concepts of science in 
student learnin g : a case  study on solar eclipse . International Journal of Science 
Education , 13(4), 431-437. 
Moolla. (2003). An interven t i o n with student teac h e r s : pupil misco n c e p t i o n s in mecha n i c s . 
Unpublished PhD, University of the Witwate r s r a n d , Johann e s b u r g . 
Morine-D e r s h i me r , G. (1993). Tracing Con cep t u a l Change in Preser v i c e Teache r s . 
Teaching and Teacher Education , 9(9), 15-26. 
Mosoloane, R., & Stanton, M. (2005). Learners ? concept i o n s about astrono mi c a l concep t s 
related to the Sun and the Earth. Paper pr esent e d at the Proceed i n g s of the13th 
Annual SAARMSTE Conference, Windhoek, Na mi bia. 
Novak, J. (1985). Metalearning and Metaknowledge strate gies to help students learn how 
to learn . In West, L., and A. Pines (Ed.), Cognitive structure and conceptual 
change . Orland o , FL: Academi c Press. 
Novak, J. (1988). Learning Scien ce and the Science of Learning . Studies in Science 
Education , 15, 77-101. 
Novak, J., & Gowin, D. (1984). Learning How to Learn . New York: Cambridge University 
Press. 
Nussba u m, J. (1979). Childr e n s ' Concep t i o n s of  the Earth as a Cos mic Body: A Cross Age 
Study. Science Education , 63(1), 83-93. 
Nussbau m, J., & Novak, J. (1976). An Assessme nt of Childre n 's Concept s of the Earth 
Utili z i n g Struc t u r e d Inter v i e w s . Science Education , 60(4), 535-550. 
Ogunnyi, M. B. (1996, 1998). Introduc t i o n . Pape r presen t e d at the Promot i n g Publi c 
Underst a n d i n g of Science and Technol ogy in Southern Africa, Cape Town. 
Ojala, J. (1992). The third planet. International Journal of Science Education , 14(2), 191-
 200. 
Opie, C. (Ed.). (2004) . Doing Educati o n a l Rese ar c h : A Guide for First Time Researc h e r s . 
London: Sage Publicat i o n s . 
292 
 
Orion, N. (1993) . A Model for the Develo p me n t and Imple me n t a t i o n of Field Trips as an 
Integr a l Part of the Scienc e Curric u l um . School Science and Mathematics , 93(6), 
325-331 . 
Osborne , J. (1996). Beyond Constru c t i v i s m. Science Education , 80(1), 53-82. 
Osborn e , J., Simon, S., & Collins , S. (2003). At titud e s towards science : a review of the 
liter at u r e and its impli c at i o n s . International Journal of Science Education , 25(9), 
1049-10 7 9 . 
Palmer , D. (2001) . Studen t s ' altern a t i v e c onceptions and scientifically acceptable 
conceptions about gravity. International Journal of Science Education , 23(7), 691-
 706. 
Paris, S. G. (1999). From the Guest Editor . Journal of Museum Education , 23(1), 2. 
Parker, J., & Heywood, D. (1998). The earth and beyond : develo p i n g primar y teache r s ' 
unders t a n d i n g of basic astron o mi c a l events . International Jour nal of Science 
Education , 20(5), 503-520. 
Parker, S. K. (1995). Lessons from  Museum and Leisur e Resear c h . Planetarian , 24(2), 9-
 11. 
Parkyn, M. (1993). Scient i f i c Imagin g . Museums Journal , October 1993, 29-34. 
Pasachoff, J. M. (2001). Wh at should Students Learn? The Physics Teacher , 
39(Sep t e mb e r ) , 381-38 2 . 
Pasach o f f , J. M. (2002) . Pasach o f f 's Points . The Physics Teacher , 40(April), 196-198. 
Pearsa l l , N., R., Skipper , J. E. L., & Mintzes ,  J. J. (1997). Knowledg e Restruct u r i n g in the 
Life Science s : A Longitud i n a l Study of Conceptu a l Change in Biology. Science 
Education , 81, 193-215. 
Piaget, J. (1929). The Child's Conception of the World . London: Routledg e & Kegan Paul. 
Piaget, J. (1930). The Child's Conception of Physical Causality . London: Routledge & 
Kegan Paul. 
Polkinghorne, D. (1995). Narrative configuration in qualitative analysis. In Amos Hat ch, 
J., and Wisniew s k i , R. (Ed.), Life Histor y and Narrati v e . London: Falmer Press. 
Poole, M. (1995). Beliefs and Values in Science Education . Buckin g h a m: Open Univer s i t y 
Press . 
Potgie t e r , M., Engelbr e c h t , J. C., & Hardi ng, A. F. (2006). Procedural and conceptual 
knowl e d g e in underg r a d u a t e mathe ma t i c s and chemis t r y , 14th Annual South e r n 
African Associat i o n for Research in Mathema t i c s , Scienc e and Technol o g y 
Educat i o n Confer e n c e . Pretor i a , South Africa. 
Pushkin, B. D. (1996). A comment on the need to use scient i f i c termin o l o g y approp r i a t e l y 
in concep t i o n studie s . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 33(2), 223-224. 
Rahm, J. (2004). Multip l e Modes of Meaning - M a k i n g in a Science Center . Science 
Education , 88(2), 223-247. 
Ra me y- G a s s e r t , L., Walber g , H. J. I., & Walb erg , H. J. (1994). Reexami n i n g Connect i o n s : 
Museums as Science Learni n g Eviron me n t s . Science Education , 78(4), 345-363. 
Rennie, L. J. (2001). Communicating Science through Inte ractive Science Centres: A 
Research Perspective . In Stocklma y e r , S. M., Gore , M.M., and Bryant, C. (Ed.), 
Science Communication in Theory and Pr actice (pp. 107-121) . Dordrech t : Kluwer. 
Rennie, L. J., Feher, E., Dierking, L. D., & Falk, J. H. (2003). Toward an Agenda for 
Advanci n g Resear c h on Science Le arning in Out-of-S c h o o l Settings . Journal of 
Research in Science Teaching , 40(2), 112-120. 
Rennie, L. J., & McClaffe r t y , T. P. (1 996) . Scienc e Centre s and Scienc e Learni n g . Studies 
in Science Education , 27, 53-98. 
Riorda n , R. (1991) . Planet a r i u m Educa tio n : A Review of the Literatu r e . Planetarian , 
20(3), 18-26. 
293 
 
Roald, I., & Mikals e n , O. (2000) . Wh at are the Earth and heavenly bodi es like? A study of 
objectua l concepti o n s among Norweg i a n deaf and hearing pupils. International 
Journal of Science Education , 22(4), 337-355. 
Roald, I., & Mikalse n , O. (2001). Config u r a t i o n and dynami c s of the Earth-S u n - M o o n 
syste m: an inves t i ga t i o ns into conc eptions of deaf and hearing pupils. International 
Journal of Science Education , 23(4), 423-440. 
Roberts, L. C. (1997). From Knowledge to Narrative: Educators and the Changing 
Museum . Washi n g t o n: Smith s o n i a n Insti t ut i o n Press . 
Rollnick , M. (2000). Current Issues and Pe rspectives on Second Language learning of 
Scien c e . Studies in Science Education , 35, 93-122. 
Rosche l l e , J. (1995). Learning in Interactive Envir onments: Prior Knowledge and New 
Experience . In Falk, J. H. & Dierki n g, L. D. (Eds. ) , Publi c Insti t ut i o n s for Perso n a l 
Learni n g :  Establi s h i n g a Researc h Agend a . Was hi n g t o n D.C.: Ameri c a n 
Assoc i a t i o n of Museums . 
Roth, W.-M. (2005) . Talking Science: Language and Lear ning in Science Classrooms . 
Lanha m: Rowma n & Littl e fi el d . 
Ruiz-Pri mo , M., & Shavelson , R. (1996). Problem s and issues in the use of concep t maps 
in scienc e assess me n t . Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 33(6), 569-600. 
Rusk, J. (2003). Do Science  Demonst r a t i o n s in the Planeta r i u m Enhanc e Learni n g . 
Planetarian , March 2003, 5-8. 
Ryan, G., & Weisner, T. (1997). Analyzin g words in brief descriptions: Fathers and 
mothers describe their children . Retrieve d 16 April, 2005, from 
www.ana l y t i c t e c h . c o m/ b o r g a t t i / r y a n t e x t . h t m  
Sadler , P. M. (1998) . Psycho me t r i c Models of Student Concepti o n s in Science:  
Reconc i l i n g Quali t at i v e Studi e s and Dist r a ct o r- D r i v e n Assessme n t Instr ume n t s . 
Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 35(3), 265-29 6 . 
Sadler, P. M. (2001). Choosin g Between Helios e i s mol o g y and Phases of the Moon. The 
Physics Teacher , 39(Dec e mb e r ) , 554-55 5 . 
Sadler, P. M. (2002). Further Discussi o n . The Physics Teacher , 40(Apri l ) , 198. 
Sander s , M. (1998) . No simple panace a s : A re spons e to Fish. In Ogunnyi , M. B. (Ed.), 
Promot i n g Public Unders t a n d i n g of Scien ce in Souther n Africa (pp. 85-91). Cape 
Town: SSME, Univers i t y of Western Cape. 
Schauble, L., Leinhardt, G., & Martin, L. (1997). A Framework of Organizing a 
Cumul a t i v e Resea r c h Agend a in Infor ma l Learn i n g Conte x t s . Journal of Museum 
Education , 22(2 and 3), 3-7. 
Schoultz, J., S?lj?, R., & Wyndha mn, J. (2001). Heavenly Talk: Discourse, Artifacts, and 
Childre n 's Underst a n d i n g of  Ele mentary Astronomy. Human Development , 44, 
103-118 . 
Schur, Y. (1999) . Constr u c t i v i s m and Mediat e d  Learning Experience  as a Basis for a 
Process of Concept u a l Change in Student s ' Concepts of Earth. Unpublis h e d Doctor 
of Philosophy, University of th e Witwat e r s r a n d , Johann e s b u r g . 
Setati, M. (2003). Speakin g math ema t i c a l l y : langua g e ( s ) , disc ou r s e and cultur a l models in 
a multi c u l t ur a l meth e ma t i c s class r o om in South Africa, Procee d i n g s of the 11th 
Annual SAARMSTE conference (pp. 605- 615): University of Swaziland, 
Mbaba n e . 
Setati, M., Adler, J., Reed, Y., & Bapoo, A. (2002). Code-switching and other language 
practices in mathematics, science and English language classrooms in South 
Africa . In Adler, J. & Reed, Y. (Eds.), Chall e n g e s of Teach e r Devel o p me n t : an 
investig a t i o n of take-up in Sout h Africa. Pretoria: Van Schaik. 
294 
 
Sha mos, M. (1995). The Myth of Scientific Literacy . New Brunswi c k , NJ: Rutgers 
Univer s i t y Press. 
Sharma , M. D., Millar , R. M., Smith, A., & Sefton, I. M. (2004). Studen t s ' Unders t a n d i n g 
of Gravity in an Orbiting Space-Ship. Research in Science Education , 34(3), 267-
 289. 
Sharp, J. G. (1996) . Childr e n 's astron o mi c a l be liefs : a prelimin a r y study of Year 6 children 
in south-w e s t Englan d . International Journal of Science Education , 18(6), 685-712. 
Shen, B. (1975). Science Literacy and the P ublic Understanding of Science . In Day, S. 
(Ed.), Commun i c a t i o n of Scient i f i c Informa t i o n . Basel: Kager. 
Shortl a n d , M. (1987) . No busin e s s like show busine s s . Nature , 328, 213-214. 
Shulma n , L. S. (1986) . Those who unders tand: Knowledge growth in teaching. 
Educational Researcher , 15(2), 4-14. 
Silver ma n , L. H. (1995) . Visito r Meani ng-Making in Museums for a Ne w Age. Curator , 
38(3), 161-170. 
Sj?berg, S. (2000). The Disencha n t me n t w ith Science . Retriev e d 16 June, 2006, from 
http:// f o l k . u i o . n o / s v e i n s j / T h e % 2 0 d i s e n c h a n t me n t % 2 0 w i t h % 2 0 s c i e n c e E x p . h t m  
Slisko, J., & Dykstra, D. (1997). The Role of  Scienti f i c Termin o l o g y in Researc h and 
Teac h i n g : Is Somet h i n g Impo r t a n t Miss i n g? Journal of Research in Science 
Teaching , 34(6), 655-660. 
Smith, J., diSessa , A., & Roschel l e , J. (1993). Misconceptions Reconceived: A 
Constructivist Analysis of  Knowled g e in Transit i o n . The Journal of the Learning 
Sciences , 3(2), 115-163. 
Squar e Kilome t r e Array . (nd). South Afric a ' s Compe t i t i ve Advan t a g e . Retri e v e d 25 June, 
2006, from http:// w w w . s k a . a c . z a / s a c a . h t ml  
Stahly, L., Krockov e r , G., & Shepard s o n , D. (1 999) . Third Grade Studen t s ' ideas About the 
Lunar Phases. Journal of Research in Science Teaching , 36(2), 159-177. 
Stake, R. E. (1995). The Art of Case Study Research : SAGE Publications. 
Statistics South Africa. (2001). Census 2001: Key Results . Retriev e d July 12, 2006, from 
http://www.statssa.gov.za/census01/htm l/Ke y % 2 0 r e s u l t s _ f i l e s / K e y % 2 0 r e s u l t s . p d f  
Storks d i e c k , M. (2004). Testi ng a Model for Underst a n d i n g Fi eld Trips in Enviro n me nt a l 
Educati o n . Unpubli s h e d Doctor of Philo sophy thesis, Univer s i t a t Lunebur g . 
Strike , K., & Posner , G. (1985) . A conceptual change view  of learning and understanding . 
In West, L., and A. Pines (Ed.), Cognit i ve struct ure and concept u a l change. 
Orland o , FL: Academi c Press. 
Strike , K., & Posner , G. (1992) . A revisionist theory of conceptual change . In Duschl, R. & 
Ha milt o n (Eds.) , Philos o p h y of scienc e , c ognitive science, and educati o n a l theory 
and practive (pp. 147-176). Al bany , NY: State Univer s i t y  of New York Press. 
Summer s , M., & Mant, J. (1995) . A survey  of British primary school teacher s ' 
unders t a n di n g of the Earth's place in the univer s e . Educational Research , 37(1), 3-
 19. 
Taylor, N., & Vinjevold, P. (Eds.). (1999). Ge tting learni n g right: repor t of the Presid e n t 's 
Educa t i o n Initi a t i ve Resea r c h Proj e c t . Johann e s b u r g : Joint Educat i o n 
Trust. D e p a r t me n t of Educat i o n . 
Tennys o n , R. D., & Rasch, M. (1988) . Linkin g cognit i ve learni n g theory to instruc t i o n a l 
prescriptions. Instructional Science , 17(4), 369-385. 
Tesch, R. (1990). Qualitative Research: Analys is Types & Software Tools . Basingst o k e , 
UK: Fal mer Press. 
Tobin, K. (2000). Interpretive Research in Science Education . In Kelly, A., and Lesh, R. 
(Ed.), Handbook of Research Design in Mathematics and Science Education. 
Mahwah, New Jerset: Lawrence Erlbaum. 
295 
 
Tobin, K., Tippins , & Gallard . (1994) . Research on Instructional Strategies for Teaching 
Science . In Gabel, D. L. (Ed.), Handbook of  Res earch on Science Teaching and 
Learni n g . New York: Macmil l a n . 
Treagu s t , D., & Smith, C. L. (1989) . Second a r y  Students' Understanding of Gravity and 
the Motion of Planet s . School Science and Mathematics , 89(5), 380-391. 
Trowbrid g e , J. E., & Wa nderse e , J. H. (1998). Theory-Drive Graphic Organizers . In 
Mintzes , J. J., Wanders e e , J. H. & Novak, J. D. (Eds.), Teachi n g Science for 
Understa n d i n g . A Huma n Construc t i v i s t Vi ew (pp. 109-131). San Diego: Academic 
Press . 
Trumpe r , R. (2001a) . A cross-a g e study of j unior high school student s ' concept i o n s of 
basic astro n o my conce p t s . International Journal of Science Education , 23(11), 
1111-11 2 3 . 
Trumper, R. (2001b). A Cross-age study of Se nior High School Student s ' Concept i o n s of 
Basic Astron o my Conce p t s . Research in Science & Technological Education , 
19(1), 97-109. 
Trundle , K., Atwood, R., & Christop h e r , J. (2002). Preserv i c e Element a r y Teacher s ' 
Concept i o n s of Moon Phases be fore and after Instruction. Journal of Research in 
Science Teaching , 39(7), 633-658. 
Tyson, M., Venvil l e , G., Harris o n , J., & Treagu st , D. (1997) . A multid i m e n s i o n a l 
fra mew o r k for interp r e t i n g concep t u a l  change events in the classr o o m. Science 
Education , 81, 387-404. 
Univers i t y of Hudders f i e l d . (2006). Templa t e Analys i s . Retrie v e d 15 July, 2006, from 
http:// w w w . h u d . a c . u k / h h s / r e s e a r c h / t e mp l a t e _ a n a l y s i s / t e c h n i q u e / q u a l i t y r e f l e x i v i t y . h
 t m  
Urke, T. (1993). Researc h on the Effects of  Teachi n g Astron o my with a Planet a r i u m. 
Planetarian , 22(4), 19-20. 
Uzzel, D. (1993) . Contrasting psychological persp ectives on exhibition evaluation . In 
Bicknel l , S. & Farmelo , G. (Eds.), Museum  visito r studie s in the 90s (pp. 125-129 ) . 
London: Science Museum. 
van der Riet, M., Dison, A., & Quinn, L. ( 1998). Concep t u a l develo p me n t throug h proces s -
 w r i t i n g in a Psycho l o g y II course . South African Journal of Higher Education , 
12(1), 221-226. 
Vosniad o u , S. (1991). Designi n g curricu l a for con cep t u a l restru c t u r i n g : Lesson s from the 
study of knowledg e acquisit i o n in astronomy . Journal of Curriculum Studies , 
23(3), 219-237. 
Vosnia d o u , S., & Brewer , W. F. (1992) . Ment al Models of the Earth: A Study of 
Concept u a l Change in Childho o d . Cognitive Psychology , 24, 535-585. 
Vosnia d o u , S., & Brewer , W. F. (1994) . Ment al Models of the Day/Night Cycle. Cognitive 
Science , 18, 123-183. 
Vosniad o u , S., Skopeli t i , I., & Ikospenk a t i , K. (2 005) . Recons i d e r i n g the ro le of artifact s in 
reason i n g : Childr e n ? s unders ta n d i n g of the globe as  a model of the earth . Learning 
and Instruction , 15, 333-351. 
Wall, C. (1973). A Review of Resear ch Related to Astronomy Education. School Science 
and Mathematics , 73(8), 653-669. 
Watts, D. M. (1982). Gravity -- don't take it for grant e d ! Physics Education , 17(3), 116-
 121. 
Weinbu r g h , M. (1995). Gender Differ e n c e s in St udent Attitu d e s toward s Scienc e . A Meta-
 An a l y s i s of the Literat u r e from 1970 to 1991. Journal of Research in Science 
Teaching , 32, 387-398. 
296 
 
Wellin g t o n , J. (1990) . Formal and informa l learni n g in scienc e :  the role  of the interac t i v e 
scienc e centre s . Physics Education , 25, 247-252. 
White, R. (1997). Trends in Re searc h in Science Educati o n . Research in Science 
Education , 27(2), 215-221. 
Wolcot t , H. (1988). Adequate School and Inadequate Educa tion: The Life History of a 
Sneaky Kid . In Jaeger, R. (Ed.), Compleme ntary Methods for Research in 
Educat i o n . Washi n g t o n DC: Americ a n Educational Research Association. 
Yin, R. K. (1994). Designing Single- and Multiple-Case Studies . In Bennett, N., Glatter, 
R., and Levacic (Ed.), Improvin g Edu cati o n a l Manageme n t (pp. 135-155 ) . London: 
Open Universi t y and Paul Chapma n Publishi n g . 
Zeilik , M. (1994) . Astronomy: The Evolving Universe . New York: John Wiley. 
Zeller, N. (1995a). Narrative Rationality in  Educational Research . In McEwan, H. & 
Egan, K. (Eds.), Narrati v e in Teachin g , Learnin g, and Researc h . New York: 
Teach e r s Colle g e Press . 
Zeller, N. (1995b). Narrative strategies for case reports . In Amos Hatch, J. & Wisnie w s k i , 
R. (Eds.), Life History and Na rrat i v e . London : Falmer Press. 
 
 
Appendices 
298 
Appendix A 
O r i g i n a l questi o n n a i r e admi ni stered as a pilot study 
 
Survey 
We would like to find out your ideas about things in space. This is not a test, and 
won?t be used at school in any way. 
1.  Wha t is the sola r syst e m? 
_________________________________________________________________ 
_________________________________________________________________ 
2.  T h e sun is a star. 
a.  W h a t is it made of? 
___________________________________________________________ 
b.  W h a t makes the sun shine ? 
___________________________________________________________ 
___________________________________________________________ 
c.  W h y does the sun look differen t to the stars you see at night? 
___________________________________________________________ 
___________________________________________________________ 
3.  N a me the four inner planets of the solar system 
_________________________________________________________________ 
4.  W h y don?t the planet s orbiti n g (going around) the sun fly off into space? 
_________________________________________________________________ 
_________________________________________________________________ 
5.  W h a t makes the moon shine at night (and someti me s durin g the day)? 
_________________________________________________________________ 
6.  D r a w a pictur e of the Earth and the Sun to show why day-ti me and night- t i me 
happen . Includ e these labels : Ea rth, Sun, day-ti me , night- t i me . 
 
 
 
 
 
 
Thank You 
Appendices 
299 
Appendix B  
C o n d e n s e d versio n s of Pre Visit Inte rvie w Schedule s (white space remo ved) 
HartRAO Pre-Visit Student Interview (PVSI) Code:  
Introduction 
1 . Introduc i n g myself 
I am Tony Lelliott, a student in the Schoo l of Education, University of the 
Witwatersrand, doing research at the Jo hannesburg Planetarium and the Hartebeeshoek 
Radio Astronomy Observatory in Gauteng, South Africa. 
2. Purpos e, Context, and Intended Use of the Interview 
The purpose of this interview is to find out about your proposed visit to the 
Johannesburg Planetarium  or the Hartebeeshoek Radio Astronomy Observatory . I intend to 
use the information you provide to get an overall picture of what happens during your visit and 
what you experience.  
3 . Assurin g Confide n t i a l i ty 
I assure you of complete confidentiality of any information you share with me, and the 
use of pseudonyms instead of actual names in the transcript and the report. No one except 
my supervisor and myself will have access to the videos. 
4. Permission to Tape  
I would like to videotape the conversation for recollection of our discussion. Will you 
give me the permission to videotape this conversation? [if the answer is yes, I set the tape on; 
if the answer is no, I take notes during the interview]. 
5. Any Ques tions  
Before we start, first, do you have any questi ons about the purpose of the interview, 
confidentiality, tape recording, or any other thing you would like to ask me?  
 
Section A. Demographic information. 
1 . Fictitious name: ________ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ 
2. Age: ________ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ 
3. Sex (Male/Female): _____________ 
4. Your parent/guardians names _______ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ 
5. Place of Origin (Farm/Township /Town/City/Other): ______________________ 
6. Name of School you are at _________ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ 
7. Grade _______ _ __ _ _ __ _ 
8. Your home phone no. ________ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ 
9. What language(s) do you mainly speak at home? ________ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ 
Appendices 
300 
Section B. The Proposed Visit. (HartRAO) 
1. Where are you going on the forthcoming visit?  
2. What subject and topic area is the visit related to? 
3. What do you think is the purpose of the visit? 
4. What preparation have you been doing for the visit? Probe for organisational 
preparation; tasks/worksheets, learning prep aration in class on topic; preparation for 
the nature of the venue; etc. 
5. Are you looking forward to the visit? Why/not? 
Section C. Knowledge (relevant for HartRAO trip) 
1 . How is the sun similar or differ ent to stars that we see at night? 
2. What is the temperature today (approx.)?  What temperature does water boil at? How 
hot do you think the sun is? What else c an you tell me about the sun? What can we 
use the sun for? 
3. Why does the sun move across the sky every day? Use model if you like. 
4. The sun and the moon look the same size in our sky. Are they? Why do you say that? 
[Which is really bigger?] Why do they look the same size? 
5. Stars at night look like pinpri cks of light. Why? What are they? 
6. What does the word ?satellite? mean to you? Probe: if DSTV, where is signal from? 
What is the shape of the dish? Why is it th is shape? Is the word satellite used in any 
other way/meaning? [What about satellite dish] 
7. What does the moon look like if you see it every night over the course of a month? 
Why does the moon look different on different nights throughout the month? 
8. What is gravity? What does it do? W hat would gravity be like on the moon? On 
Jupiter? Why the difference? So what is the pull of gravity related to? Does the 
earth?s? gravity have any effect on the moon? What? What about the moon?s grav. 
effect on the earth? 
9. Have you heard any planet or space-related things on TV, in magazines or in the 
news at all recently? 
Section D. Attitude to Science/Astronomy 
1. Do you like school? Probe why/not. 
2. What are your 2 favourite  subjects at school? Why? 
3. What are your 2 least  favourite  subjects at school? Why? 
4. What do you do in your spare time? Wh at TV programmes do you like? Do you read 
much? Tell me any books you have read recently. 
5. So far at school, have you ever covered th ings like the sun, t he moon, planets, space 
stars etc? In which subject did you cover them? 
6. Do you find anything to do with space, planets and stars interesting? If so, what? 
why? If not, why not? 
7. If you  were a teacher teaching about space, planets and stars, what would you  do 
with your students (in your own grade)? 
8. Have scientists found life anywhere ot her than earth? Do you think there is life 
anywhere else in the universe? Do you believe in aliens? Probe why. Have aliens 
ever visited earth?  
9. Do you believe in the horoscope in magazines etc? [astrology]. Probe. 
Appendices 
301 
1 0 . Are you religious? What is god?s relationship to the universe? 
Planetarium Pre-Visit Student Interview (PVSI) 
Sections A and B are same as the HartRAO PVSI 
Section C. Knowledge (relevant for planetarium trip) 
1. Do you know the names of any stars? Can you see any planets in the night sky? 
What do they look like? 
2. What is the solar system? What [?things?]  does it consist of? What shape is it? How do 
you know? 
3. [Tell me anything you know about the s un] ? if not covered in PMM interview. 
4. Why does the sun move across the sky every day? 
5. The sun and the moon look the same size in our sky. Are they? Why do you say that? 
[Which is really bigger?] Why do they look the same size? 
6. Stars at night look like pinpricks of ligh t. Why? What are they? [why are they there?] 
7. In the fastest space ship we could build, how long would it take to reach the closest 
star outside our solar system? Guess? 
8. Why does the moon look different on di fferent nights (and days) throughout the 
month? 
9.  have you heard any planet or space-relate d things in the news at all recently? 
Section D. Attitude to Science/Astronomy 
1. Do you like school? Probe why/not. 
2. What are your 2 favourite  subjects at school? Why? 
3. What are your 2 least  favourite  subjects at school? Why? 
4. What do you do in your spare time? Wh at TV programmes do you like? Do you read 
much? Tell me any books you have read recently. 
5. So far in your schooling, have you ever  covered things like the sun, the moon, 
planets, space stars etc? In wh ich subject did you cover them? 
6. Do you find anything to do with space, planets and stars interesting? If so, what? 
why? If not, why not? 
7. If you  were a teacher teaching about space, planets and stars, what would you  do 
with your students (in your own grade)? 
8. Do you believe in aliens? Probe why. Have aliens ever visited earth? Have scientists 
found life anywhere other than earth? 
9. Do you believe in the horoscope in magazines etc? [astrology]. Probe. 
10. Are you religious? What is god?s relationship to the universe? 
 
Appendices 
302 
Appendix C  
C o n d e n s e d versi o n s of Post Visit Inte rvi e w Schedu l e s (white space remove d ) 
HartRAO Post-Visit Student Inte rview Questions (PoVSI) Code:  
Introduction 
1 . Introduc i n g myself (remind e r ) 
A s you know, I am Tony Lelli ot t , a Doctor of Philoso p h y studen t in the School of 
Educati o n , Univer s i t y of the Witwate r s r a n d , doing research at the Johannes b u r g 
Planet a r i u m and the Hartebe e s h o e k Radi o Astron o my Observ a t o r y in Gauten g , 
South Africa. 
2. Purpos e, Context, and Intended Use of the Interview 
Thank you for allowing me to observe your class trip to the Johannesburg 
Planetarium (pltm) or the Hartebeeshoek Radi o Astronomy Observatory (hartrao). The 
purpose of this interview is to find out how your visit to the pltm or hartrao went. I intend to 
use the information you provide to get an overall picture of how the visit takes place, and what 
the students learn. I will be aski ng some similar questions to t he ones I asked before the trip, 
but also some new questions. 
3 . Assurin g Confide n t i a l i ty 
I assure you of complete confidentiality of any information you share with me, and the 
use of pseudonyms instead of actual names in the transcript and the report. No one except 
my supervisor and myself will have access to the videos. 
4. Permission to Tape  
I would like to videotape the conversation for recollection of our discussion. Will you 
give me the permission to tape this conversation? [i f the answer is yes, I set the tape on; if the 
answer is no, I take notes during the interview]. 
5. Any Ques tions  
Before we start, first, do you have any questi ons about the purpose of the interview, 
confidentiality, tape recording, or any other thing you would like to ask me?  
Fictitious name of student: _______ _ ___ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ 
Grade(s)/Class(es) taken on trip: _______ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ __ _ _ 
PoVSI HartRAO 
Section B. The Recent Visit. [Answers will be noted by the interviewer on a separate sheet] 
6. Where did you go on your [recent] science [or HSS] visit? When did you go? 
7. What learning area and/or topic was the visit related to? 
8. What do you think was the purpose of the visit? 
9. Did you do any work on topic of the visit? Probe for what was done; 
tasks/worksheets, any classwork or home work on topic; essay written; etc. 
 
Appendices 
303 
Section C. Follow-up of questionnaire 
1 0 . How is the sun similar or differ ent to stars that we see at night? 
11. What is the temperature today (approx.)?  What temperature does water boil at? How 
hot do you think the sun is? What else c an you tell me about the sun? What can we 
use the sun for? 
12. Why does the sun move across the sky every day? Use model if you like. 
13. The sun and the moon look the same size in our sky. Are they? Why do you say that? 
[Which is really bigger?] Why do they look the same size? 
14. Stars at night look like pinpric ks of light. Why? What are they?  
15. What does the word ?satellite? mean to you? Probe: if DSTV, where is signal from? 
What is the shape of the dish? Why is it th is shape? Is the word satellite used in any 
other way/meaning? [What about satellite dish] 
16. What does the moon look like if you see it every day over the course of a week? Why 
does the moon look different on different nights throughout the month? 
17. What is gravity? What does it do? What  would gravity be like on the moon? {Probe 
further if they think there is no gravity on moon] On Jupiter? Why the difference? So 
what is the pull of gravity related to? Does  the earth?s? gravity have any effect on the 
moon? What? What about the moon?s grav. effect on the earth? 
18. Have scientists found life anywhere ot her than earth? Do you think there is life 
anywhere else in the universe? Do you believe in aliens? Probe why. Have aliens 
ever visited earth? 
19. Since the visit, have you heard any planet or space-related things on TV, in 
magazines or in the news? 
Section D. Attitude to Science/Astronomy 
11. Now you?ve been to the HartRAO, have your ideas about space, planets and stars 
changed at all? What? 
12. What things did you most enjoy about the visit? Can be anything. 
13. What did you enjoy least? Is ther e anything you disliked about the visit? 
14. Have you told anyone about the vi sit? Who? What did you tell them? 
15. If your school arranges a visit to Hart ebeeshoek Radio Astronomy Observatory, 
would want to you go (again)? 
16. The Hartebeeshoek Radio Astronomy Obse rvatory run public visits. Would you want 
to visit again together with your family? 
17. What sort of job do you wa nt to do when you leave school? 
18. Other than today and when you told xx [# D4 above], have you thought about the trip 
since? Tell me what. 
Planetarium Post-Visit Student Interview (PoVSI) 
Sections A and B are same as the HartRAO PoVSI 
 
Section C. Follow-up of questionnaire 
1. Do you know the names of any stars? Can you see any planets in the night sky? 
What do they look like? 
2. What is the solar system? What [?things?]  does it consist of? What shape is it? How do 
you know? 
Appendices 
304 
3. [Tell me anything you know about the s un] ? if not covered in PMM interview. 
4. Why does the sun move across the sky every day? Use model if you like. 
5. The sun and the moon look the same size in our sky. Are they? Why do you say that? 
[Which is really bigger?] Why do they look the same size? 
6. Stars at night look like pinpric ks of light. Why? What are they?  
7. In the fastest space ship we could build, how long would it take to reach the closest 
star outside our solar system? Guess? 
8. Why does the moon look different on di fferent nights (and days) throughout the 
month? 
9. Have you heard any planet or space-re lated things in the news at all recently? 
Section D. Attitude to Science/Astronomy 
1. Now you?ve been to the planetarium, have your ideas about space, planets and stars 
changed at all? What? 
2. What things did you most enjoy about the visit? Can be anything. 
3. What did you dislike about the visit? 
4. Have you told anyone about the vi sit? Who? What did you tell them? 
5. If your school arranges a visit to Planetarium/Hartebeeshoek Radio Astronomy 
Observatory, would want to you go (again)? 
6. The Planetarium/Hartebeeshoek Radio As tronomy Observatory run public visits. 
Would you want to visit again together with your family? 
7. What sort of job do you wa nt to do when you leave school? 
8. Other than today, have you thought about the trip since? Tell me what. 
 
Appendices 
305 
Appendix D  Ethics sheets and forms 
Information Sheet 
Research Study on Learning in Astronomy 
My name is Tony Lelliott. I?m a member of staff in  the School of Education at the University of 
the Witwatersrand and also a Doctor of Philo sophy student at the same institution. 
I am carryi n g out a study of infor ma l le arning at the Johannes b u r g Planeta r i u m and 
the Visitor?s Centre of the Hartebees h o e k Radio Astron o my Observ a t o r y , mainly 
looking at how school student s and teacher s learn about astron o my and scienc e as a 
result of a school visit to the site. My  research will bene fit not only the two 
instit u t i o n s where it is taking place, but also the South Af ric a n educa t i o na l syste m in 
impro v i n g the learn i n g and teachi n g of scien c e . 
I selected your school as representative of many of the schools in South Africa. I would like to 
interview you as a [student]/[teac her] who is going to visit one of the above places, as well as 
observe the class when you actually visit the planetarium or observatory. In addition, I?d like to 
check what you might learn by using a questionn aire that asks about particular astronomical 
phenomena. After your return to school, I?d like to interview you again about your experiences 
of the visit, and possibly follow this up with a final questionnaire some weeks later. I have 
selected you because your school class is intending to conduct a school visit to 
Johannesburg Planetarium or Hartebeeshoek Radio Astronomy Observatory in the near 
future, and because your school is representati ve of the bulk of schools in South Africa. 
Each part of the research will take about 40 minutes to complete , that is the interview before 
the visit; the questionnaire during the visit, the post-visit interview and the post-visit 
questionnaire. In addition, I?d like to conduct a follow up interview with you several weeks or 
months after the visit. 
If you agree to take part in my study, I?d like to make it clear that your participation is entirely 
voluntary, no harm will come to you, and all in formation will be treated with confidentiality and 
anonymity. If you do choose to participate, you may decline to answer any questions, and you 
may withdraw from the study at any time. I hope to publish the results of my study in 
academic journals. In order to protect conf identiality, all names I use will be fictitious. 
I will provide you with a summary of my research results on completion if you would like me 
to. 
Thank You. 
Appendices 
306 
Informed Consent Form 
R e s e a r c h Project: Learning about Astronomy 
 
I, ________ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ c o n s e n t to particip a t e in this study conducte d 
by Mr. A. Lelliot t for his researc h on lear ning at an observat o r y and a planetari u m. 
I realise that no harm will come to me, and that the study is being conduct e d for 
educat i o n a l purpo s e s .  
I partici p a t e volunt a r i l y and underst a n d that I ma y withdra w from the study at any 
time . 
I further consent to being video and/or  audio recorded as part of the study. 
I also underst a n d I have the right to revi ew the question n a i r e s I comple t e and the 
transcr i p t s made of our convers a t i o n s before these are used for anal ysis if I so choose. 
I can delete or amend any materi a l or retrac t  or revise any of my remarks . Everyt h i n g 
I say will be kept confi de n t i al by the inter v i e w e r . I will only be ident i fi e d by a 
pseudo n y m in the transcr i p t . In additi o n , any pe rsons I refer to in the intervi e w will be 
kept confide n t i a l . 
 
Verbat i m quote s from me may be us ed in the researc h report, but th ey will be reported 
so that my identi t y is anonymo u s . Any specif i c individ u a l s or course s I refer to will be 
given pseud o n y ms . I unders t a n d that the resu lt s of the study may be publ is h e d , but my 
identi t y will be anonymo u s . 
Na me ____________________________________________________ 
Signature _________________________________________________ 
Date ________ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ 
 
Appendices 
307 
Informed Consent Form  ? Parent/Guardian 
R e s e a r c h Project: Learning about Astronomy 
 
I, _______________________________, parent/guardian of my ward 
______ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ consen t to her/hi m partic i p a t i n g in the study 
conduct e d by Mr. A. Lelliot t for his res earc h on learnin g at an observa t o r y and a 
planet a r i u m. 
I realise that no harm will come to my war d, and that the study is being condu c t e d for 
educa t i o n a l purpo s e s .  
I allow my ward to part i c i p a t e volunt a ri l y  and underst a n d that s/he may withdra w 
from the study at any time. I further c onsent to my ward being video and audio 
recorded as part of the study. 
 
Everyt h i n g my ward says will be kept conf i d e n t i al by the intervi e w e r . My ward will 
only be identif i e d by a pseudon y m in the tran script. In addition, any persons my ward 
refer s to in the inter v i e w will be kept confi d e n t i al . 
 
Verbat i m quotes from my ward may be used in the resear c h repor t , but they will be 
reporte d so that her/his identi t y is anonym o u s . Any specifi c indivi duals or courses my 
ward refers to will be given pseudo n yms . I underst a n d that the result s of the study 
may be publ is h e d , but my ward?s ident i t y will be anonymo u s . 
Na me ____________________________________________________ 
Signature _________________________________________________ 
Date ________ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ _ 
 
 
Appendices 
308 
Appendix E  Screenshots of ATLAS.ti Hermeneutic Unit 
 
 
 
Appendices 
309 
Appendix F 
Student Gravity 
Star 
concept 
Sun 
concept SS Size/Scale 
Sun 
movement 
Moon 
phases 
Parabolic/Satellite 
Dish 
Mean 
score 
  pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
ave 
pre 
ave 
post 
 Difference 
in mean 
score 
scf06 2 n/a 2 2 1 3 2 2 2 3 1 1 2.5 2 n/a n/a 1.8 2.2 0.4 
scf08 n/a n/a 2 2 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 2 n/a n/a 2.7 2.7 0.0 
scf10 n/a n/a 2 2 3 3 2 2 2 2.5 2 3 2.5 2.5 n/a n/a 2.3 2.5 0.3 
Helen 2 3 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 3 1 3 3 3 n/a n/a 2.1 2.6 0.5 
scf12 n/a n/a 2 2 2 3 2 2 2 3 2 3 2 2 n/a n/a 2.1 2.5 0.4 
Fatima n/a n/a 2 2 2 3 2 2 1 2 1 2 1 1 n/a n/a 1.5 2.0 0.5 
scf17 n/a n/a 2 2 2 2 1 2 2 3 1 1 1 1 n/a n/a 1.5 1.8 0.3 
scf19 n/a n/a 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 2 2 2 2 n/a n/a 2.7 2.7 0.0 
swo05 2 2 1 2 2 3 n/a n/a 2 3 3 3 3 1 1.5 2 2.1 2.3 0.2 
swo06 1 2 1 1 1 2 n/a n/a 2 2 2 2 1 1 2 1 1.4 1.6 0.1 
swo07 1 2.5 1 1 2 2 n/a n/a 2 2 3 3 3 2.5 1.5 2 1.9 2.1 0.2 
swo14 1 2 1 2 2 2 n/a n/a 2 2 1 1 1 1 1 1.5 1.3 1.6 0.4 
swo20 2 3 2 2 1 2 n/a n/a 2 3 3 3 2 2.5 2 2 2.0 2.5 0.5 
swo22 1 2 1 1 1 1 n/a n/a 1 2 2 3 1 n/a 2 2 1.3 1.8 0.5 
Nonkul 0 1 1 1 1 1 n/a n/a 1 1 1 1 1 n/a 2 2 1.0 1.2 0.2 
swo29 1 2 2 2 2 2 n/a n/a 2 3 3 2 1 n/a 2.5 3 2.1 2.3 0.3 
swo36 1 2 2 2 2 3 n/a n/a 2 2 3 2 2.5 n/a 2 2.5 2.0 2.3 0.3 
Neo 1 1 2 2 2 3 n/a n/a 3 3 3 3 2.5 n/a 1.5 2 2.1 2.3 0.2 
swo53 1 1.5 1 2 1 2 n/a n/a 2 2 3 1 2 n/a 1 1.5 1.5 1.7 0.2 
swo59 2 2.5 1 2 2 2 n/a n/a 1 2 3 3 1 n/a 1 3 1.7 2.4 0.8 
swo69 2 3 1 1 3 3 n/a n/a 2 2 3 3 1 n/a 2 2.5 2.2 2.4 0.3 
Brenda 1 2 1 2 2 3 n/a n/a 2 3 3 3 1 n/a 2.5 3 1.9 2.7 0.8 
tsw02 1 3 1 1 2 2 n/a n/a 2 2 1 3 1 1 1.5 2 1.4 2.0 0.6 
Botho 1 2 2 3 2 2 n/a n/a 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 1 1.6 1.9 0.3 
tsw08 2 2 1 1 2 2 n/a n/a 2 2 1 1 1 1 1.5 2 1.5 1.6 0.1 
Appendices 
310 
Student Gravity 
Star 
concept 
Sun 
concept SS Size/Scale 
Sun 
movement 
Moon 
phases 
Parabolic/Satellite 
Dish 
Mean 
score 
  pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post pre post 
ave 
pre 
ave 
post 
 Difference 
in mean 
score 
tsw15 1 2 1 1 1 2 n/a n/a 2 3 3 3 2 2.5 2 3 1.7 2.4 0.6 
vho02 2 2.5 2 2 3 3 n/a n/a 3 3 3 3 3 n/a 2 2.5 2.5 2.7 0.2 
vho06 1 1.5 1 2 2 3 n/a n/a 2 3 2 2 2 n/a 2 3 1.7 2.4 0.8 
vho09 1 1 2 2 1 1 n/a n/a 1 1 1 1 1 n/a 1 1 1.2 1.2 0.0 
vho10 2 2 2 2 2 3 n/a n/a 2 2. 5 3 3 1 n/a 3 3 2.3 2.6 0.3 
vho11 1 1 1 1 3 3 n/a n/a 2 2 2 2 1 1 1 2 1.6 1.7 0.1 
vho12 3 3 2 2 3 3 n/a n/a 3 3 3 2 1 n/a 1.5 2.5 2.6 2.6 0.0 
vho13 2 2 2 2 2 2 n/a n/a 2 2 2 3 1 n/a 2 2 2.0 2.2 0.2 
John 3 3 3 3 3 3 n/a n/a 3 3 2 3 3 n/a 3 3 2.8 3.0 0.2 
Ave 1.5 2.1 1.6 1.8 2.0 2.4 2.1 2.3 2.0 2.4 2.2 2.3 1. 7 1.7 1.7 2.2 1.8 2.1 0.3 
 
Appendices 
311 
Appendix G  
O r i g i n a l catego r i s a t i o n scheme 
 Post- v i si t lear n i n g 
Pre-vi s i t catego r y 1 2 3 
Low swo07 a 
swo26 aN k u l u l e k o *  
vho09 a  
vho11 a  
tsw04 a  
tsw08 a  
scf17 ? Kefi lwe a 
swo06 ? Zanele a  
swo14 can?t code  
swo22 a  
swo59 a  
swo70 a  
tsw02 a  
swo53 a  
vho06 a  
scf15 ? Fatima a* 
Mediu m swo42 ? Neo a*  
vho13 a  
These 2 very simil a r : 
neither learnt much, 
despite good pre-
 kno w l e d g e . 
scf06 a 
swo29 a  
swo36 a 
tsw15  
vho10  
scf11 ? Helen a* 
scf12 
swo05  
swo20  
High scf08 
scf10 
scf19 
swo69  
vho02 
vho12  
vho16 - John a* 
  
?
 a- means that the PDs have been recoded in December 2005- early 2006. 
* means that a portrai t has been ?comple t e d ? (early 2006)  incl HC & ILF discussion. 
The 5 most  impro v e d accor d i n g to the Revise d Class i f i c a t i o n are: 
scf15 (Fatma), swo05, swo59, tsw15 and vho06. 
 
Appendices 
312 
Appendix H  
 Scatterg r a m of student s ? pre- and post-vi s i t mean scores recalcu l a t e d using the Rasch 
techni q u e . 
-2. 5
 -1. 5
 -0. 5
 0. 5
 1. 5
 2. 5
 -2. 5 -1. 5 -0. 5 0. 5 1. 5 2. 5
 Rasch Mean score pre-visit
 Ras
 ch Me
 an
  s
 cor
 e 
po
 st
 -vis
 it
  
Appendices 
313 
Appendix I  
A s t r o n o my - r e l a t e d Vocabu l a r y used by student s 
 
Student 
code 
S t u d e n t 
name 
Nu mber 
of 
words 
used in 
pre-vi s i t 
PMMs 
Additi o n a l 
numbe r of 
words 
used in 
post- v i s i t 
PMMs 
scf06 Sibongile 29 6 
scf08 Susan 24 11 
scf10 Lara 14 15 
scf11 Helen 16 9 
scf12 Sarah 19 2 
scf15 Fatima 26 7 
scf17 Kitso 13 3 
scf19 Antonia 28 8 
swo05 Sipho 12 7 
swo06 Zanele 17 1 
swo07 Bhekiwe 22 1 
swo14 Mpho 13 10 
swo20 Banyana 20 10 
swo22 Ntobeko 21 0 
swo26 Nkululeko 18 7 
swo29 Douglas 8 6 
swo36 Judy 23 3 
swo42 Neo 7 11 
swo53 Batsile 10 5 
swo59 Thapiso 13 2 
swo69 Phillip 23 4 
swo70 Brenda 19 9 
tsw02 Tlotlo 17 11 
tsw04 Botho 11 9 
tsw08 Fane 17 11 
tsw15 Nnaniki 26 12 
vho02 Paul 30 4 
vho06 Vicky 27 2 
vho09 Theresa 22 8 
vho10 Lynn 23 8 
vho11 Cathy 19 1 
vho12 Richard 37 10 
vho13 Ross 31 4 
vho16 John 36 14 
Mean  20.3235 3 6.79411 8 
SD  7.53062 7 4.04354 6 
 
 
Appendices 
314 
Appendix J:  Personal Meaning Maps  
N o n k u l u l e k o ? s pre-vi s i t Person a l Meanin g Map 
 
Appendices 
315 
Nonkululeko?s post-visit Personal Meaning Map 
 
Appendices 
316 
Botho? s pre-vi s i t Pe rsona l Meaning Map 
 
Appendices 
317 
Botho? s post-v i s i t Person a l Meanin g Map 
 
Appendices 
318 
Neo?s pre- and post-vis it Personal Meaning Map 
Appendices 
319 
John?s post-v i s i t Pers onal Meaning Map 
 
Appendices 
320 
Fatima ? s pre- and post-v i s i t Person a l Meanin g Map 
 
Appendices 
321 
Brenda?s pre- and post-vi s i t Persona l Meaning Map 
 
Appendices 
322 
Helen?s pre- and post-vis it Persona l Meaning Map 
 
Appendices 
323 
Appendix K 
Getting the most from your visit to a museum or hands-on centre 
 
Before you go: 
? T h i n k about the purpose of your visi t and its position in your scheme of work. Is the visit to give 
general experie n c e and stimula t i o n as an introdu c t i o n to the topic? Is it to  support specific learning 
of certa i n conc e p t s? Is it to consoli d a t e teachi n g that  has already taken place? 
? V i s i t the mu seum, or if you can't , visit its webs ite or talk with someone who has been before. 
? P l a n for what scienti f i c concep t s and skills s hould be met before the visit and what should be 
followed - u p back at school. 
? D e c i d e what part of the exhibit i o n or what exhib i t s will form the focus for learni n g and/ o r wheth er 
your pupils need to follow a set route or sequence. 
? F i n d out what facili t i e s and serv ices the museum offers, e.g. whet h e r the museu m has 'expl a i n e r s ' 
to assist pupils. a classroom where work can be followed-up or works ho p activi t i e s led by the 
museum 's educat i o n servic e . Deci de how you will use these servic e s . 
? F i n d out what additio n a l adult suppo rt is availab l e and can be provi de d for the visit, e.g. parents , 
studen t teache r s , etc. 
? D e c i d e how adults might be informe d and support e d so that they can offer help to pupils at the 
museum, e.g. devise prompt sheets or use ones provid e d by the museum. 
 
A t the museum: 
? P r o v i d e some time and space for pupils to orient a t e themse l v e s and 'play'. This allows the class to 
have some free explor a t i o n time and to di ssi p a t e some of their initi al 'energ y '. 
? Tell the class what you exp ect them to do. It is usually better fo r pupils to work in pairs or small 
groups so that social intera c t ion at exhibits can occur. 
? Y o u may wa nt to offer some limited guidan c e or prompt s , e.g. by way of a 'trail card'. Record 
experien c e s , e.g. by taking digital photographs or making a video. 
 
F ollowing your visit: 
? A s k pupils to tell you what they remembe r e d mo st from their visit. What were they impres s e d by? 
What new things did they learn? 
? Allow pupils to develop their learning by broadcas ting their experiences to others. You could ask 
them to do this by prepari n g and sharing poste rs and displays or by giving a presenta t i o n . 
? U s e activi t i es and pract i ca l tasks that enhan c e and devel o p the learni n g exper i e nc e s at the 
museum. Avoid trying to replic a t e what they did at the museum. 
? R e f e r back to experie n c e s at the mus eum, not only in  the topic but in future lesson s as well. This 
helps pupil s to value the experi e n c e and to consoli d a t e learnin g by integra t i n g gains from the 
infor ma l situ a t i o n in the muse u m with the more forma l lear ni n g in school . 
 
 
From: Braun d, M. (2004 ) . Lear n in g scien c e at muse u ms and hands -o n centr e s . In Braun d, M. & Reiss , M. (Eds .) , Learn ing 
Scien ce Outside the Classroom (pp . 113- 128 ). Londo n : Rou tled g eFalmer .